...

BIOPOLITICS AND SUBJECTIVITY: THE CASE OF AUTISM

by user

on
Category: Documents
492

views

Report

Comments

Transcript

BIOPOLITICS AND SUBJECTIVITY: THE CASE OF AUTISM
BIOPOLITICS AND SUBJECTIVITY: THE CASE OF AUTISM SPECTRUM
CONDITIONS IN ITALY
By
M. ARIEL CASCIO
Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of
Philosophy
Department of Anthropology
CASE WESTERN RESERVE UNIVERSITY
May, 2015
2
CASE WESTERN RESERVE UNIVERSITY
SCHOOL OF GRADUATE STUDIES
We hereby approve the thesis/dissertation of
M. Ariel Cascio
candidate for the degree of Ph.D.*.
Committee Chair
Atwood D. Gaines
Committee Member
Eileen Anderson-Fye
Committee Member
Lee Hoffer
Committee Member
Anastasia Dimitropoulos
Date of Defense
March 6, 2015
* We also certify that written approval has been obtained for any proprietary material
contained therein.
3
Table of Contents
List of Tables ...................................................................................................................... 7
List of Figures ..................................................................................................................... 8
Preface................................................................................................................................. 9
Acknowledgments............................................................................................................. 11
List of Acronyms .............................................................................................................. 13
Abstract ............................................................................................................................. 11
Introduction ....................................................................................................................... 16
The Autism Concept...................................................................................................... 16
Experiences along the Autism Spectrum ...................................................................... 16
The Research Questions ................................................................................................ 22
Dissertation Overview by Chapter ................................................................................ 25
Chapter 1: Literature Review ........................................................................................ 29
Chapter Overview ....................................................................................................... 29
Section 1: The Anthropology of Biomedicine ........................................................... 32
Section 2: The Anthropology of Autism .................................................................... 41
Critical Autism Studies ............................................................................................. 42
Cross-Cultural Studies of Autism ............................................................................ 49
Section 3: New Biopolitical Theory............................................................................ 52
Overview of New Biopolitical Theory ...................................................................... 54
Molecularization ....................................................................................................... 58
Somatic Expertise ..................................................................................................... 60
Illness Identities ........................................................................................................ 64
Collective Identities and Biosocialities .................................................................... 65
Cross-Cultural Studies of New Biopolitical Theory ................................................ 66
Section 4: The Anthropology of Youth...................................................................... 69
Rites of Passage ........................................................................................................ 72
Identity....................................................................................................................... 73
Peer Relationships .................................................................................................... 75
Sexuality .................................................................................................................... 79
Transition to Adulthood ........................................................................................... 81
Conclusion .................................................................................................................... 83
Chapter 2: Background .................................................................................................. 85
Chapter Overview ....................................................................................................... 85
4
Section 1: History of Autism ...................................................................................... 85
Section 2: History of Italian Autism Services ........................................................... 90
Section 3: Management of Italian Autism Services................................................ 100
Chapter 3: Research Design ......................................................................................... 116
Section 1: Conceptual Framework .......................................................................... 116
Section 2: Methods .................................................................................................... 119
Participant-observation .......................................................................................... 119
Staff and Other Professionals ................................................................................ 120
Parents/Guardians .................................................................................................. 121
Youth with Autism ................................................................................................... 125
Data Analysis .......................................................................................................... 127
Dissemination of Results ........................................................................................ 130
Limitations .............................................................................................................. 130
Section 3: Description of Fieldsites .......................................................................... 132
Day Center 1 ........................................................................................................... 133
Day Center 2 ........................................................................................................... 136
Day Center 3 ........................................................................................................... 139
Residential Center 1................................................................................................ 142
Residential Center 2................................................................................................ 145
Social/Recreational Group 1 .................................................................................. 147
Social/Recreational Group 2 .................................................................................. 152
Results Overview ....................................................................................................... 156
Chapter 4: Professional Conceptualization of Autism............................................... 157
Chapter Overview ..................................................................................................... 157
Section 1: What is Autism? ....................................................................................... 158
Autism, Asperger’s Syndrome, and Intellectual Disability ................................... 166
What Causes Autism ............................................................................................... 172
Section 2: How Should Autism be Managed? ......................................................... 174
Psicoanalitica .......................................................................................................... 176
Psicologico .............................................................................................................. 177
Ufficiale ................................................................................................................... 178
Psicoeducativo......................................................................................................... 183
Cognitivo-Comportamentale .................................................................................. 184
Relazionale .............................................................................................................. 187
5
Ecologico ................................................................................................................. 188
Psicomotricità ......................................................................................................... 189
Auto-Mutuo-Aiuto .................................................................................................. 190
Specific Approaches................................................................................................ 191
Life Cycle ................................................................................................................ 209
Conclusion .................................................................................................................. 215
Chapter 5: Parents’ Perspectives ................................................................................ 218
Chapter Overview ..................................................................................................... 218
Section 1: What is Autism? ...................................................................................... 219
Discussing Autism .................................................................................................. 227
Contesting the Diagnosis ........................................................................................ 231
Autism, Asperger’s Syndrome, and Intellectual Disability ................................... 236
What Causes Autism ............................................................................................... 237
Section 2: How should autism be managed? ........................................................... 239
Therapies and Interventions................................................................................... 240
School ...................................................................................................................... 254
Services .................................................................................................................... 260
Conclusion................................................................................................................... 283
Chapter 6: Youth with Autism .................................................................................... 285
Chapter Overview ..................................................................................................... 285
Section 1: What Is Autism? ...................................................................................... 286
Sex, Gender, and Sexuality .................................................................................... 306
Section 2: Interfacing with Services ........................................................................ 309
Being Occupied ....................................................................................................... 309
Dopo di Noi ............................................................................................................. 310
Satisfaction with Services ....................................................................................... 311
Youth Relationships ................................................................................................ 311
Conclusion .................................................................................................................. 317
Chapter 7: New Biopolitical Theory ........................................................................... 319
Chapter Overview ..................................................................................................... 319
Section 1: Defining New Biopolitical Theory .......................................................... 320
Molecularization ..................................................................................................... 323
Somatic Expertise ................................................................................................... 330
Individual Identities ................................................................................................ 343
6
Collective Identities and Socialities........................................................................ 351
Section 2: Complicating New Biopolitical Theory ................................................. 360
Frequency of Themes ............................................................................................. 360
Overlap of Themes .................................................................................................. 365
Section 3: Whose New Biopolitical Theory? ........................................................... 368
Conclusion .................................................................................................................. 375
Conclusion and Implications........................................................................................... 378
Research Questions and Answers ............................................................................... 378
Contributions to the Literature .................................................................................... 384
Future Research ........................................................................................................... 386
Broader Impacts .......................................................................................................... 387
Appendix 1 – Interview Guide Topics ............................................................................ 389
Staff and Professionals ................................................................................................ 389
Parents ......................................................................................................................... 391
Youth with Autism – Individual Interview ................................................................. 395
Youth with Autism – Group Interview ....................................................................... 398
Appendix 2 – Codebooks ................................................................................................ 400
Appendix 3 – Italian Block Quotes ................................................................................. 407
Chapter 4 ..................................................................................................................... 407
Chapter 5 ..................................................................................................................... 416
Chapter 6 ..................................................................................................................... 425
Chapter 7 ..................................................................................................................... 430
References ....................................................................................................................... 438
7
List of Tables
Table 1: Demographic Profile of Interview Participants (Professionals) ...................... 121
Table 2: Demographic Profile of Interview Participants (Parents)................................ 124
Table 3: Demographic Profile of Interview Participants (People with Autism) ............. 126
Table 4: Time Spent at Each of 7 Sites ............................................................................ 133
Table 5: Commonly Referenced Characteristics of Autism (Professionals) ................... 162
Table 6: Possible Causes of Autism (Professionals) ....................................................... 173
Table 7: List of Philosophies and Approaches ................................................................ 174
Table 8: Specific Approaches References ....................................................................... 192
Table 9: Parent-Reported Diagnoses ............................................................................. 222
Table 10: Commonly Referenced Characteristics of Autism (Parents) .......................... 225
Table 11: Possible Causes of Autism (Parents).............................................................. 239
Table 12: Approaches ..................................................................................................... 240
Table 13: How Parents Heard About Services ............................................................... 261
Table 14: Parent Goals for Participation....................................................................... 268
Table 15: Commonly Referenced Characteristics of Autism (Youth) ............................. 300
Table 16: Emergent Codebook ....................................................................................... 400
Table 17: a priori Codebook ........................................................................................... 403
8
List of Figures
Figure 1: Domains of Child Neuropsychiatry .................................................................. 95
Figure 2: The Italian Educational System ........................................................................ 95
Figure 3: The Institutional Path of Someone with a Disability Certificate in Italy ....... 103
Figure 4: New Biopolitical Theory Themes and Subthemes ........................................... 322
Figure 5: New Biopolitical Theory Themes .................................................................... 361
Figure 6: Molecularization Subthemes ........................................................................... 361
Figure 7: Somatic Expertise Subthemes ......................................................................... 362
Figure 8: Individual Identities Subthemes ...................................................................... 362
Figure 9: Collective Identities and Socialities Subthemes ............................................. 363
9
Preface
This dissertation reports on data collected in Italy, primarily in Italian. I would
therefore like to take this opportunity to share some notes on the transcription,
translation, and writing conventions herein.
I have put Italian words in Italics even when they were English loan words such
as Milano è business. Translations are in parentheses (Milan is business) or, in block
quotes, under the original.
I use double quotations “Milan is business” for direct quotes from interviews or
from fieldnotes in which I know I copied down the verbatim statement. I use single
quotes, 'Milan is business' to denote speech, but speech that I recorded non-verbatim (in
fieldnotes or in interviews without audio)
Brackets indicate words that were added for clarification when they were not
spoken, “Milan is [for] business.” Brackets around ellipses indicate missing words,
“Milan is […] business.” Three dots indicates a missing words within a sentence. Four
dots indicates a sentence or more is missing. Ellipses without brackets indicate pauses
made by the speaker. To maintain confidentiality, I also use brackets to indicate that I
have replaced a proper noun (name of a person, place, or organization) with a common
noun or phrase, most often “[figlio]” ([my child]) in place of the child’s name.
I use parentheses to indicate phrases that were wholly inaudible, such as “Milano
è (inaudible)” (Milan is (inaudible)) or when I have doubts about the accuracy of my
transcription, such as “Milano è (business)” (Milan is (business)).
I am using the term each site used such as ospiti, atleto, ragazzo in thick
description. When I need to refer to people with autism as a whole I generally follow
10
person-first language in this dissertation as suggested by the American Psychiatric
Association. When translating, I use terms such as “people with autism” rather than
translating substantive adjectives such as autistici literally (i.e. “the autistics”) as such a
translation indexes a dehumanizing stance that the Italian phrase does not.
The quotations herein were checked by native Italian speakers Alice Larontonda
and/or Cecilia Maffei, but changes were approved or rejected by me. Most of the
interviews I trascribed myself, so the corrections were on the order of correcting articles,
pronouns, or prepositions. Substantive changes related to vocabulary choice or turn of
phrase were not made, meaning that some of the quotes do not sound “proper” but they
are “correct.” I also did not change indicative verbs to subjunctive even when the
subjunctive was warranted. Finally, I took out a substantial amount of intercalari –
interjections such as diciamo, no, insomma, ecco – which Italian speakers unanimously
told me disturbed the flow of reading, and whose meaning equates to English
interjections such as “like, you know, um.” The purpose of all modifications and
corrections were to increase readability and avoid creating unimportant distraction to the
reader through the use of colloquial speech.
All mistakes in transcription or translation remain my own.
11
Acknowledgments
Few works are created through the lone efforts of a single researcher, and this
dissertation is no exception. Ethnographic research in particular becomes especially
arduous without financial support, and I would therefore like to begin by thanking my
funders. A pilot study in 2011 was funding by the Eva L. Pancoast Memorial Fellowship
and the Phi Beta Kappa Graduate Research Grant, both from Case Western Reserve
University (CWRU). Data collection over academic year 2012-2013 was funded by the
U.S.-Italy Binational Fulbright Commission through the Fulbright IIE Program. Data
analysis was funded by a Dissertation Research Assistance Grant under the supervision of
Eileen Anderson-Fye and the Baker Nord Center for the Humanities Graduate Research
Grant, both at CWRU. The writing of this dissertation was supported by the Arts &
Sciences Dissertation Fellowship at CWRU.
There are several individuals and organizations who made this work possible. I
thank Roberto Malighetti and the Università degli Studi di Milano-Bicocca, without
whose affiliation for the Fullbright grant this project would not have been possible. I
especially thank everyone at Cascina Rossago; Cooperativa Aurora 2000 and the Spazio
Autismo; Cooperativa I Percorsi; Cooperativa Spazio Aperto Servizi; Fondazione Istitute
Sacra Famiglia ONLUS; Gruppo Asperger ONLUS; and Progetto Filippide for the help,
hospitality, and learning opportunities they provided me.
It is equally vital that I thank a number of people who have provided valuable
support on several specific tasks. I thank my chair Atwood Gaines and committee
members Eileen Anderson-Fye, Lee Hoffer, and Anastasia Dimitropolous for their time,
support, and feedback. In addition to my committee and the faculty at CWRU, Tamara
12
Daley helped me with the research design by talking to me about her own work on autism
in India. I also need to thank several individuals for their help in preparing my Italian
language documents in the first place, including Denise Caterinacci, Ann Cheney, Mirna
Cola, Veronica Pozzi, and the CWRU IRB staff. I owe my gratitude to several people
during the data analysis and write-up phase particularly. Alice Larontonda I cannot thank
enough for her transcription assistance, her valuable insight, and her continuous patience
with me. Alice also checked many of the translations that appear in this manuscript, as
did Cecilia Maffei. Abigail Ordillas assisted with the construction of Table 1 as well as in
searching for new sources for the literature review. Kristi Ninneman and Chantal Van
Esch provided detailed advice for the statistical analysis in Chapter 7.
The intellectual merit I hope the reader finds in this dissertation was strengthened
through conversations with a number of other scholars. I especially thank Catherine
Osborn, Kristi Ninneman, Elizabeth Fein, Clarice Rios, Barbara Costa, Enrico Valtellina,
Mirna Cola, and again Alice Larontonda. Enrico Valtellina provided feedback on Chapter
2. Kristi Ninneman provided detailed feedback on an early draft of Chapter 4. Sarah
Koopman-Gonzalez and participants in the CWRU Arts & Sciences Dissertation
Fellowship program provided detailed feedback on an early draft of Chapter 5. My
writing partners in the Women in Doctoral Studies Writing Guild helped keep me on
task, especially Chantal Van Esch.
On a personal note, I need to thank Jenny Hancher most especially, for talking me
through crises both existential and practical on both sides of the Atlantic. Finally, I of
course thank my family and friends who have provided unfailing love and support during
this journey.
13
List of Acronyms
ABA
APA
ADOS
ADI-R
AQ
CAA
CARS
CBT
CF
CHAT
DAN!
DGS
DIR
DPS
DSM-IVDSM-5
TR
ICD-10
PECS
PEP
TTAP
TEACCH
WAIS
WHO
Applied Behavior Analysis
American Psychiatric Association
Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule
Autism Diagnostic Interview – Revised
Autism Quotient
Comunicazione Aumentativa e Alternativa (Augmentative and
Childhood
Rating Scale
AlternativeAutism
Communication)
Cognitive Behavioral Therapy
Comunicazione Facilitata (Facilitated Communication)
Checklist for Autism in Toddlers
Defeat Autism Now!
Disturbo Generalizzato dello Sviluppo (Generalized Developmental
Developmental,
Individual-differences, Relationship-based Model
Disorder)
Disturbo Pervasivo dello Sviluppo (Pervasive Developmental Disorder)
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition,
Diagnostic
and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition
Text Revision
International Classification of Diseases, 10th Edition
Picture Exchange Communication System
Psychoeducational Profile
TEACCH Transition Assessment Profile
Treatment and Education of Autistic and Related Communication
Wechsler
Adult
Intelligence Scale
Handicapped
Children
World Health Organization
14
Biopolitics and Subjectivity: The Case of Autism Spectrum Conditions in Italy
Abstract
By
M. ARIEL CASCIO
The prevalence and scholarly interest in autism has increased dramatically in
recent decades. This study contributes to the social study of autism by investigating
autism in Italy. It asks: (1) How is autism conceptualized in Italy? (2) How do people
labelled as autistici and their families define, manage, and experience that diagnosis? (3)
How do the discourses expressed by professionals, people with autism, and their parents
resonate, or not, with new biopolitical theories such as biosociality and biological
citizenship? To answer these questions, data were collected using a cross-sectional
ethnographic design including participant-observation at seven autism-specific services
as well as interviews with staff and other autism professionals, parents of people who
attended, and the attendees themselves when possible. The results of this study
demonstrate that the local particularity of Italian health and human services impacts the
way autism is conceptualized in Italy and the ways that people with autism and their
families define, manage, and experience this diagnosis.
This study addresses several important bodies of anthropological literature,
including the anthropology of biomedicine; the anthropology of autism; “new biopolitical
theory;” and the anthropology of youth. Its central descriptive findings provide an
experience-near exploration of autism-specific services for youth. By exploring the
15
transition to adulthood for youth with developmental disabilities, it challenges
anthropological definitions of adolescence proposes a definition of adulthood as an
amalgamation of materials and roles to which all individuals have a right based on the
standard of their peers. Its central theoretical finding challenges and expands “new
biopolitical theory.” It refines and operationalizes this theory and explores the limits of its
applicability in Italy.
This study also has several implications for readers with a professional or
personal interest in autism through its rich description of the experiences of professionals,
parents, and people with autism. It may foster greater understanding and collaboration
between these groups by presenting their perspectives side by side. Furthermore, it
provides a rich account of the experiences of people with autism and their families,
making their perspectives accessible to a broader audience of scholars, professionals,
advocates, families, and people with autism themselves.
16
Introduction
The Autism Concept
This dissertation explores the concept of “autism,” called “autismo” in Italy. I
describe autism in this way, as a “concept,” because the term has been and continues to
be used to mean many different things. Just as Kleinman and Good (1985a:2) once wrote
that depression is “considered mood, symptom, and illness,” Silverman (2012:2) writes,
“autism refers to a symptom, a disorder, and a syndrome.” Autism may refer to a
diagnostic entity, variously conceived of as a disease to be cured, a disability to be
managed, or a neurological condition to be embraced as a part of human diversity (Cascio
2012; Eyal et al. 2014). It may refer to a symptom, as in Eugen Bleuler’s (1950) original
use of the term in descriptions of schizophrenia to refer to the characteristic of selfabsorption (Hacking 2010). It may refer, as it did to some of my participants, to a
personality trait or characteristic just like any other. It may refer to an integral part of an
autistic person’s experience – a way of being and not, in the words of Jim Sinclair, “an
appendage” (1993). In Anglophone discourse, autism is both a medical or psychiatric
diagnosis (and which of the two, if either, is often a point of contention) and also a locus
of increasing popular culture fascination. For these reasons and more, I introduce the idea
of “the autism concept” much as Margaret Lock (2013) discusses “the Alzheimer
phenomenon,” to encompass the range of ways that autism might be conceptualized and
to avoid wedding myself to a particular understanding of autism as I entered into my field
research and as I enter now into this dissertation which discusses its findings.
Experiences along the Autism Spectrum
One of the most definitive features of autism as it is often understood in the 21st
17
century – both in the United States and in Italy; both as a disorder and as a neurological
diversity – is its variation along a “spectrum.” Most discussions of autism include
reference to an “autism spectrum” and one way to refer to a person with autism is as a
person “on the spectrum.”1 In psychiatric terms, autism is not a categorical diagnosis
(someone either has it or does not) but a dimensional diagnosis (someone can have it to a
greater or lesser extent). Though not without problematizing the distinction, many
scholars and clinicians use the terms “high functioning” and “low functioning” autism to
describe the severity (or strength) of autism symptoms (or characteristics) (e.g., Fenton
and Krahn 2007). I prefer to use the terms “autism with lower levels of support needs”
and “autism with higher levels of support needs” to recognize the social basis of
disability.2 Regardless of terminology, the notion of an autism spectrum recognizes that
there is no singular experience of autism.
In the spirit of this observation as well as the spirit of ethnography, I open this
dissertation with a description of not one, but two amalgamated descriptions of a
participant with autism: one with higher levels of support needs and then one with lower
levels of support needs. In both of these descriptions, I describe an 18 year old man, as
most people with autism diagnoses are boys and men. The majority of people with autism
that I met and spent time with in Italy had very high levels of support needs, as
recognized by the Local Health Boards. They attended Day and/or Residential Centers
Philosopher Ian Hacking (2010) proposes the term “autism manifold” to recognize non-binary
complexity.
2
I learned of this sort of terminology through Gruppo Asperger (2013), whose website uses the phrase
“autismo con bisogno di supporto non intensivo” (autism with need of non-intensive support) following the
announcement of the DSM-5. For English language use of the term “support needs” see for example the
American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Disability’s Supports Intensity Scale, an
instrument which measures support needs (Thompson 2004)
1
18
with an attendee to staff ratio of three or fewer attendees to one staff member. However,
the majority of people with autism quoted in this dissertation had lower levels of support
needs, and attended Social/Recreational Groups in addition to schools or internships with
much higher attendee to staff ratios.
Take for example a participant with higher levels of support needs. He may speak
very little if at all, but having spent so much time with him, it would be unfair to say he
does not communicate. He is particularly interested in automobiles, spending his leisure
time paging through magazines and pointing out his favorites to me. Sometimes, with
lightning quickness, he darts out a hand and grabs a different magazine or a comic book.
He moves through the pages quickly, but laughs at the ‘right’ places, the pages where the
biggest jokes of the comic appear. When leisure time is over, he may return to the stack
of magazines several times, not to page through them but to adjust the stack, seeming to
seek some sort of order that isn’t apparent to me or to the staff. When satisfied or at least
convinced to leave it, he completes whatever work he has to do next without seeming to
look at it, tilting his head to the side. He hurries through it, and runs to the next place he
has to be. If he has to stay in one place very long, he flaps his hands or rocks back and
forth. He often makes a gesture the staff tells me is his way of asking “what’s going on?”
A staff member explains to him what they are waiting on and what they will do next. He
seems satisfied for about 30 seconds, then starts repeating the gesture. If someone has
taken the seat he wants, he might start rearranging the furniture to achieve a new optimal
seat. He is semi-autonomous in activities of daily living, setting the table, eating,
toileting, and changing clothes with the help of step by step visual agendas. He always
seems to want to be with people – especially the staff – and to know what they are doing,
19
constantly turning around in his chair to see who is there and often using the “what’s
going on?” gesture to ask for more information. There are clearly those other attendees
with whom he gets on, and others whose presence bothers him – and same with staff.
Even when bothered he seems cheerful, but staff and his parents tell me that he has gone
through periods of severe depression as well as severe aggression, which are only now
managed through a combination of properly dosed pharmaceuticals and the structure of
his daily life. Even though we don’t talk, I appreciate that I know his unique interests and
try to share them by talking to him about cars I’ve seen. I work to learn his idiosyncratic
sign language and offer appropriate verbal responses.
Take on the other hand the example of an individual with lower levels of support
needs. Conversation is somewhat easier in that we both speak Italian and relatively
fluidly. He is particularly interested in the fantasy genre, and rushes to tell me about it
every time we see each other. He is working on a fan fiction that he would like to turn
into a novel, and often pulls me, staff, or other attendees over to read his latest
installment. It is hard to tear him away from the group at the end of the day, because he
wants to share his project repeatedly with me – though he may get protective of it and
start to hide if it someone from the group he does not like tries to look. He may get very
frustrated and notably down if no one seems interested, snapping at others or
withdrawing completely to a private corner. However, he has several peers that share his
interests, and when they are there they seem almost inseparable, engaged in a constant
dialogue about fantasy novels, films, and games. They repeat favored quotes back and
fourth at each other more than daily. They construct elaborate plans for projects on which
they can collaborate. Sometimes I notice his language seems stilted, pausing in atypical
20
places, but other times his prosody is no different than anyone else’s. If these peers are
not there, he may wander far from the group, maintaining neither proximity nor eye
contact with anyone else. He is completely autonomous in activities of daily living, but
notes that he doesn’t dress like his peers. He may wear a costume piece drawn from his
favorite fantasy series for weeks at a time before he is bored with it. Despite his current
favorite, however, he always remembers which books I’ve read and films I’ve seen, since
he asks me about all of them, and he always shares the latest news on my favorites with
me. He also shows me around the places we visit, introduces me to people, helps me with
my Italian, and explains to me how things work – from the rules of the game to the rules
of Social/Recreational Group. However, I also notice little mismatches in our
communication, such as what seems to be an inordinant amount of concern and panic
when I bump my head. I learn to articulate my feelings much more explicitly, explaining
that I am not hurt by the bump on my head, that I am not mad that he spilt water on me,
that I am uncomfortable with a comment he made and why, that I am pleased that we are
playing this particular game, and so on. Despite these few difficulties, we generally
communicate quite freely.
These two brief descriptions present two very different young men. Indeed, each
of the young men and women I met and spent time with in Italy were different from the
others. Others had high levels of support needs, but were verbal, unlike the man
described in this example. Some people spoke very fluidly on their topic of interest, but
hardly participated at all if the conversation topic changed. Some asked specific
questions, continuing their pattern of questioning the same way in numerous
conversations regardless of the answer. Some had specific questions related to their
21
special interests that they wanted to ask everyone they met. Some almost never initiatied
contact or conversation unless specifically prompted to do so (and sometimes not even
then). Some spoke mostly in stilted single words, but could offer fluid greetings and other
formulaic speech. These differences may be reduced in text below, in which I aggregate
responses and do not assign pseudonyms, in order to better maintain anonymity. This
reduction is further complicated by the fact that interview participants had lower levels of
support needs than many of the youth who are discussed by their parents, and as such my
presentation of the perspectives of people with autism and quotations from thir interviews
favor individuals with lower levels of support needs participating in Social/Recreational
Groups (as in the second example above). However, the majority of people with autism I
met and spent time with had lower levels of support needs, and so much of this
dissertation is dedicated to their experiences through presentation of participantobservation data and interviews with significant others in their lifes – staff and parents.
For this study, it was important to me to spend time with people with autism
themselves, to attempt to understand their daily lives through both participantobservation and, when possible, semi-structured interviews that asked them to reflect on
their lives and on autism. It was also important to me to conduct this participantobservation not only with people with lower levels of support needs, who might more
easily participant in these interviews, but also with people with higher levels of support
needs. This attention to daily experiences across the spectrum is relatively unique in the
literature, which often focuses on interviews with parents of children with autism with
higher levels of support needs, or research including only people with autism with lower
levels of support needs (there are, however, notably exceptions in the ethnographic
22
literature, such as Hendrik’s 2013 book Autistic Company). The dissertation that follows
therefore stands out for its attention also to participants with higher levels of support
needs.
The Research Questions
The central purpose of the research I describe in this dissertation is to understand
how autism is defined, managed, and experienced in the Italian context. Because my
study seeks to understand how autism is defined, I did not start with a pre-established
definition. However, several authoritative documents provide a starting point for
understanding autism, both for myself and for my research participants. “Autism
spectrum disorders” are classified as developmental disabilities in the DSM-IV-TR (APA
2000), and described by the National Institute of Mental Health as “developmental brain
disorders” (National Institute of Mental Health [NIMH] 2007). Per the Diagnostic and
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5) (American Psychiatric
Association [APA] 2013:50–59), “autism spectrum disorder” is a neurodevelopmental
disorder which occurs along a spectrum of severity for the two domains of social
communication and restricted, repetitive behaviors. However, this fifth edition of the
DSM was released in May 2013, during my data collection period of 2012-2013.
Therefore, it is more useful to consider the fourth edition, text revision (DSM-IV-TR)
(APA 2000) and International Classification of Diseases, Tenth Edition (ICD-10) (World
Health Organization 2008). Although the DSM is extremely influential in Italian
professional circles and parents’ associations (Sistema Nazionale per le Linee GuidaIstituto Superiore di Sanità 2011; Autismo Italia n.d.) the ICD is the official diagnostic
manual for the local health units (Crialesi et al. 2007:280). The ICD-10 code F84
23
includes six classifications: Autistic Disorder, Rett’s Syndrome, Other Childhood
Disintegrative Disorder, Asperger’s Syndrome, Other Pervasive Developmental
Disorders, and Pervasive Developmental Disorder Unspecified (World Health
Organization 2008). In my work, I use the term “autism” to refer broadly to all pervasive
developmental disorders.
The cause of autism has been and still is much researched and discussed, but no
consensus exists (National Institute of Mental Health n.d.; CDC 2014; Sistema Nazionale
per le Linee Guida-Istituto Superiore di Sanità 2011). Relatedly, the way autism should
be treated and managed is much contested. In the 70 years of its history, autism has also
been approached as a psychological disorder, an “emotional disturbance” often attributed
to family life (Bettelheim 1972; Henry 1971), a perspective that can still be encountered
in the 21st century (Feinstein 2010; Grinker 2007; McDonnell 1997). Following the
history of U.S. psychiatry detailed in section on, this approach lost its dominance in the
U.S. to a model of autism as somewhat in between mental retardation and mental illness
treated through perceptual-motor therapies often designed for the former and behavioral
therapies for the latter (Eyal et al. 2010; Schreibman 2005; see also Silverman 2012;
Thompson 2007). The definition and management of autism remains contested in the 21st
century. In the United States there are professional, advocacy, and self-advocacy circles
supporting variously behavioral therapies, dietary and “biomedical” interventions, and
neurodiversity models (Cascio 2012; Orsini 2009; Silverman 2012). In many parts of the
world there is an active contest between different approaches, perhaps most prominently
psychoanalysis (Block and Cavalcante 2012; Chamak et al. 2010; Rios and Costa
Andrade in press).
24
The prevalence and scholarly interest in autism has increased dramatically in
recent decades. My study contributes to the emerging social study of autism by
investigating autism in a northern region of Italy. It asks: (1) How is autism
conceptualized in Italy? (2) How do people labelled as autistici and their families define,
manage, and experience that diagnosis? (3) How do the discourses expressed by
professionals, people with autism, and their parents resonate, or not, with new biopolitical
theories such as biosociality and biological citizenship? To answer these questions, data
were collected using a cross-sectional ethnographic design including participantobservation at 7 autism-specific services as well as interviews with staff and other autism
professionals, parents of people who attended, and the attendees themselves when
possible.
Anthropologists have been interested in medical systems since the origins of the
field. While early anthropologists focused on the “ethnomedicines” and
“ethnopsychiatries” of non-professionalized and, generally, non-Western medical
systems (Rubel and Hass 1996; Hughes 1996), in the 20th century, scholars recognized
and argued that all medicine is ethnomedicine and all psychiatry is ethnopsychiatry
(Gaines 1992a). In other words, all medical systems are social systems which are locally
particular and historically contextual. At the same time, meaning-centered approaches in
medical anthropology focused on the lived experiences of illness, disability, chronicity,
and suffering more broadly. Scholars recognized that no matter how clear the biological
basis of an illness state (e.g., Down syndrome with its cause of a singular genetic
mutation), illness is defined, managed, and experienced in the social world (Rapp 2000a).
Autism does not have such a clear biological basis, and remains what Silverman (2012:6)
25
calls “a contested category” without a single clear etiology, definition, or treatment plan.
Such contested categories have long been of interest to anthropologists.
Dissertation Overview by Chapter
This dissertation emerges from several specific anthropological subdisciplines
including the anthropology of biomedicine, the anthropology of autism, new biopolitical
theory, and the anthropology of youth. Chapter 1 will provide an overview of these fields
and the specific literatures within then most relevant to a study of autism and subjectivity.
Research question (3) emerged from a key literature on this topic, a collection of theories
I call “new biopolitical theory.” These theories – biosociality, biological citizenship, and
derived terms – draw from Foucault’s original formulation of biopolitics and update it for
the 21st century based on developments in the “bio” through genetics, epigenetics,
neurology, and the psy- sciences of psychology and psychiatry. Several studies find all or
part of these theories to be highly applicable in the case of autism-related subjectivities,
both of people with autism themselves and their families (e.g., Brownlow, Charlotte and
O’Dell, Lindsay 2013; Eyal et al. 2014; Orsini 2009). Though new biopolitical theories
have not been without criticism, my research focuses specifically on the geographic
dimension of criticism. Research on new biopolitical theory around autism and in other
situations has been geographically limited, with some scholars arguing that these theories
are applicable only in western (European and North American) states and healthcare
systems (Roberts 2008; Sleeboom-Faulkner 2011). This claim falsely homogenizes
“Europe and North America” into a monolithic concept. Through the exemplary case of
autism, the present study investigates the applicability of new biopolitical theory in Italy,
unquestionably western yet distinct from Austria and the United States where autism
26
spectrum conditions were first described. Studies of new biopolitical theory in Italy have
generated mixed results (Alcano 2009; Cola and Crocetti 2011). Therefore, this study
focuses on the key example of autism, to allow for a compelling implicit comparison with
the existent body of literature and determine if themes in the Italian context are more
concordant or discordant with findings in other settings.
Chapter 2 moves away from anthropological theory to provide the historical and
cultural background to this study. It begins by introducing the social history of autism as
a concept in the globalized complex of science and medicine. It then narrows this
discussion to a history of autism in Italy. The remainder of the chapter is dedicated to
explicating the history and current practices of managing autism in Italy. It explains what
resources are available to people with autism across the life cycle, both public and
private, to contextualize the presentation of collected data. Specific features of the Italian
health and human services system make it a particularly apt place to study new
biopolitical theory. Firstly, in the 1970s Basaglia famously initiated a world-renowned
movement of de-institutionalization (Scheper-Hughes and Lovell 1986). This movement
led to policies of social and educational integration, including hiring quotas for
employers (Crialesi et al. 2007:275), and the laws of integrazione scolastica mandating
the inclusion of students with disabilities in public schools (Canevaro and de Anna 2010).
Moreover, Italian medicine remains, for the most part, non-privatized; therefore patients
lack the consumer role seen in systems like U.S. healthcare (Cola and Crocetti 2011).
Relatedly, doctor-patient interaction can be characterized by a “social-embeddedness
narrative,” as in the case of non-disclosure of cancer diagnoses (Gordon 1990; Gordon
and Paci 1997). For these reasons, we might expect new biopolitical subjectivities to be
27
minimized. Biosociality is exclusive by its specificity, whereas social integration is
inclusive. A lack of a consumer role for patients may minimize their mobilization of lay
expertise, as would paternalistic doctor-patient relationships.
Having established this background information, the remaining chapters delve
into the study itself. Chapter 3 describes the research design, methods, and sample
characteristics, and describes the locations in which I conducted participant-observation.
Chapters 4-6 address description aims (1) and (2), divided between the three respondent
groups of professionals, parents of youth with autism, and people with autism
themselves. Chapter 4 is grounded in two subsections: “What is Autism?” and “How
Should Autism by Managed?” Chapters 5 parallels this structure, and Chapter 6
maintains a similar structure with subsections entitled “What is Autism” and “Interfacing
with Services.”
Chapter 7 and the Conclusion bring these groups back together. In Chapter 7, I
build upon the descriptive results in Chapters 4-6 and explore explicitly new biopolitical
theories, answering research aim (3). It is divided into three sections. “Defining New
Biopolitical Theory” explicates discrete themes in new biopolitical theory. “Complication
New Biopolitical Theory” investigates the relative weight of those themes in participant
narratives. “Whose New Biopolitical Theory” explores their variation based on
demographic factors including type of service accessed (Day, Residential, or
Social/Recreational) and characteristics of the participant/participant’s child.
In the conclusion, I present overarching themes that return to the literatures
described in Chapter 1. I articulate the significance of this study for the field of
anthropology by tying it to the four bodies of literature discussed in Chapter 2: the
28
anthropology of biomedicine, the anthropology of autism, new biopolitical theory, and
the anthropology of adolescence and youth. Overall, I argue two main points: (1) several
factors in the context of Italian health and human services and broader sociopolitical
context limit the extent to biopolitical modes of engagement are present in participants’
narratives; (2) exploring the ways that parents and professionals work within the health
and human services system to create opportunities for youth with autism to access
rhythms and materials of adulthood can challenge and expand working definitions of
adolescence in anthropology. I conclude with suggestions for future research, arguing
that a self-conscious cross-cultural study of autism that approaches the subject from a
meaning-centered perspective will be of interest to anthropologists, practitioners,
advocates, and people affected by autism through their own diagnosis or the diagnosis of
a loved one.
29
Chapter 1: Literature Review
Chapter Overview
In this chapter, I present an overview of the key literatures in anthropology that
gave rise to this dissertation topic and research design. I review the Anthropology of
Biomedicine, the Anthropology of Autism, the body of literature I call “New Biopolitical
Theory,” and the Anthropology of Youth. These literatures help develop and frame the
general research query, “how is autism defined, managed, and experienced in the Italian
context?” and give rise to its three specific research questions: (1) How is autism
conceptualized in Italy? (2) How do people labelled as autistici and their families define,
manage, and experience that diagnosis? (3) How do the discourses expressed by
professionals, people with autism, and their parents resonate, or not, with the body of
literature I call “new biopolitical theory”?
The first section of this chapter reviews the Anthropology of Biomedicine. This
literature shows that biomedicine is a cultural system, same as law, religion, and so forth.
Although biomedical research and practice are globalized enterprises characterized by the
flow of concepts, goods, and people, this globalization does not imply homogenization.
Rather, there are local biomedicines with diverse frameworks, bioethics, and practices.
One potential avenue of difference is historically Protestant versus historically Catholic
countries. Autism was first described in historically Protestant Austria and the United
States. This study seeks to understand how autism is defined, managed, and experienced
in a Western biomedical system different from these countries where autism spectrum
conditions were first described and later codified and globalized. This study looks at
historically Catholic Italy. We don't need to contrast “biomedicine” with “traditional
30
medical systems” in order to talk about the local particularity of biomedicines. This thesis
will describe some major features of the Italian biomedical and health services system as
it is relevant to autism.
The second section of this chapter reviews the Anthropology of Autism. I will
review the anthropology of autism, talking both about making the strange familiar (the
cross-cultural study of autism) and making the familiar strange (the cultural constructivist
study of autism, generally in the US). The field that Davidson and Orsini (2013) call
“critical autism studies” represents “making the familiar strange.” Closely linked to
science studies, communication studies, and history, such works investigate the historical
particularity of autism diagnoses and reports of an autism “epidemic.” In short, this body
of literature demonstrates the value of not taking anything for granted when it comes to
autism. The cross-cultural study of autism then makes the strange familiar. When it
comes to autism, some scholars broaden the notion of cross-cultural to include the idea
that people with autism participate in their own “autistic cultures,” which can themselves
be an object of study (e.g., Davidson 2008a). There is also a growing literature on the
cross-cultural study of autism as anthropologists typically mean it – the anthropology of
autisms around the globe. This dissertation most contributes to this latter literature by
expanding the cross-cultural study of autism, but is heavily informed by the former.
The third section of this chapter considers one important theory that makes the
familiar strange when it comes to autism – the collection of theories and literature I call
“new biopolitical theory.” These theories – biosociality, biological citizenship, and
derived terms – which draw from Foucault’s original formulation of biopolitics and
update it for the 21st century based on developments in the “bio” through genetics,
31
epigenetics, neurology, and the psy- sciences of psychology and psychiatry. They focus
on health identity and subjectivity practices. This section reviews these theories and the
key concepts including biomedicalization, molecularization, somatic expertise, individual
identities, collective identities and socialities, and bioeconomics. It reviews the uses of
these theories in the social study of autism, as well as the geographical limits of these
theories in studies addressing autism and new biopolitical theory more broadly. In doing
so, this literature overlaps heavily with the anthropology of biomedicine and the
anthropology of Europe through lines of local variation. Studies of new biopolitical
theory in Italy have generated mixed results. Therefore, this study focuses on the key
example of autism, to explore the geographical limits of new biopolitical theory with a
more nuanced and sophisticated operationalization of the theory, determining if themes in
the Italian context are more concordant or discordant with findings in other settings.
The fourth and final section of this chapter turns to the Anthropology of Youth.
Due to the focus of new biopolitical theories on identity and subjectivity, my research
design focuses on adolescents and youth on the autism spectrum. Adolescents and youth
are the best group in which to explore new biopolitical theory given that I was attempting
to understand its relevance to people with autism themselves. This study draws on both
the anthropology of adolescence (psychological anthropology) and youth cultures
(coming more out of British sociology). Both fields see identity as important for young
people, which is why I chose to study young people (new biopolitical theories are about
identity and subjectivity). My study is centered on autism-specific places, which comes
more from Youth Cultures, but it attends to young people's transitions into adulthood and
specific place in the life cycle, which comes more from the Anthropology of
32
Adolescence. Therefore, I argue it is fair to call this an Anthropology of Youth Study.
Questions of identity and life cycle remain central to this dissertation, which contributes
to the Anthropology of Youth by exploring one culture’s efforts to create opportunities
for access to adult roles for youth with developmental disabilities.
Section 1: The Anthropology of Biomedicine
Anthropologists have been interested in medical systems since the origins of the
field. While early anthropologists focused on the “ethnomedicines” and
“ethnopsychiatries” of non-professionalized and, generally, non-Western medical
systems (Rubel and Hass 1996; Hughes 1996), later scholars recognized and argued that
all medicine is ethnomedicine and all psychiatry is ethnopsychiatry (Gaines 1992a). In
other words, all medical systems are social systems which are situated within particular
local and historical contexts. At the same time, meaning-centered approaches in medical
anthropology focused on the lived experiences of illness, disability, chronicity, and
suffering more broadly. Scholars recognized that no matter how clear the biological basis
of an illness state (e.g., Down syndrome with its cause of a singular genetic mutation),
illness is defined, managed, and experienced in the social world (e.g., Rapp 2000a;
Scheper-Hughes 2001).
Autism does not have such a clear biological basis. Indeed, finding the cause(s)
takes up a good deal of research funding and energy. Autism remains what Silverman
(2012:6) calls “a contested category”. While Kleinman and Good (1985a:2) wrote that
depression is “considered mood, symptom, and illness,” Silverman (Silverman 2012:2)
writes, “autism refers to a symptom, a disorder, and a syndrome.” Such contested
categories have long been of interest to anthropologists. As in Kleinman and colleagues’
33
attention to anthropology of depression, a self-conscious Anthropology of Autism is
emerging.3
While the next section turns to a detailed discussion of the Anthropology of
Autism, this section addresses the anthropology of biomedicine more broadly,
demonstrating the ways in which it informs some aspects of the Anthropology of Autism
as well as this particular study. It begins with a brief historical overview of the
anthropological study of biomedicine, then considers in detail the problematicization of
mind/body dualism in the field – and related, the distinction between the anthropological
study of psychiatry and that of non-psychiatric medicine. Finally, it considers the local
particularity of biomedicine as revealed by this field. This study emerges from these
major points within the anthropology of biomedicine: it considers biomedicine a cultural
system; it explores autism’s problematic place between mind and body and between
medical fields; and it reveals the local particularity of the Italian biomedical system.
From the early days of anthropology, scholars have been interested in medical systems.
The study of “other” cultures’ medical systems became known as ethnomedicine (Hughes
1968; Rubel and Hass 1996) and relatedly ethnopsychiatry (Devereux 1980; Gaines
1992a; Hughes 1996). These systems were contrasted with “medicine” without the prefix,
described at first as “modern medicine” (Hughes 1968) and then variously as “scientific
medicine,” “cosmopolitan medicine,” “Western medicine,” “allopathic medicine” and
later Biomedicine, all meaning “the professional medicine of the West” (Gaines and
Davis-Floyd 2004). Starting in the 1980s, anthropologists turned more explicitly towards
the study of this latter system.
3
This paragraph is adapted from Cascio (2014a).
34
The use of “professional” in this context directly follows Arthur Kleinman’s
(1980) division of health care systems into “popular,” “folk,” and “professional” sectors.
The professional sector is comprised of “the organized healing professions” in contrast to
the individual, family, and community practices of the popular sector as well as the “nonprofessional, non-bureaucratic, specialist” folk healers. Professional medicine includes
not only the Western practices (biomedical and otherwise), but also professional
medicines based upon system such as Chinese, Ayurvedic, and Galenic medicine, which
Bates (1995) called “scholarly medical traditions” due to their reliance on the
interpretation of medical texts and Leslie (1976) called “great-tradition” medicines.
“Ethnomedicine” and “ethnopsychiatry” once referred primarily to the study of folk
medicines (e.g., Blum and Blum 1965; Blum and Blum 1970; Crapanzano 1973;
Hallowell 1961; Lebra 1976). Mol and Berg (1998) argue that “biomedicine” was in fact
a category invented by anthropologists as a contrast to these “ethnomedicines.” The
Anthropology of Biomedicine expands these terms to include professional medicine and
Biomedicine, studied similarly to “folk medicines” and shown to be equally “cultural.”
The distinction between (ethno)medicine and (ethno)psychiatry is fuzzy and
problematic, but has often been made (Hughes 1996). This distinction relates to that
between soma and psyche – body and mind – a particularly prevalent metaphor in U.S.
biomedicine (Kirmayer 1988). Early studies of professional medicines of East Asia were
particularly concerned with challenging this mind/body dualism through studies of
somatization (Kleinman 1977; Kleinman 1982; Kleinman and Kleinman 1985; Kleinman
1988; Ohnuki-Tierney 1984), “somatopsychic” medicine (Lock 1982), and the contrasts
between Western dualism and the conceptualizations of mind/body in these Eastern
35
Cultures (Marsella and White 1982). The conceptual divide between body and mind is
not shared cross-culturally, nor necessarily is a division between physical health and
mental health. Intellectual and developmental disabilities such as autism spectrum
conditions fall somewhat between them, often considered tenuously as part of psychiatric
classification schemas, yet distinct from mental illness (Eyal et al. 2010).
It was within the study of psychiatry moreso than non-psychiatric medicine that
anthropologists first contrasted practices between Western countries. Townsend (1978)
provided a detailed analysis of mental health conceptualizations among U.S. and German
patients, clinicians, and lay youth, finding that practitioners’ perspectives were more
closely tied to local lay perspectives than their fellow practitioners’ internationally. Such
findings paralleled studies in psychiatry, namely Kendell and colleagues’ (1971) research
on differing U.S. and British diagnostic practices using the same patient cases and
standardized nosologies (135). These studies further demonstrated the importance of
context to the diagnostic process, and de-homogenized the notion of Western medicine.
In the 1970s, Townsend (1978) argued that German psychiatry was more “biological”
than U.S. psychiatry. Other scholars also noted the strong influence of Freudian
psychoanalysis in U.S. psychiatry in the first half of the 20th century (Castel, Castel, and
Lovell 1982; Kennard 1959; Littlewood 2000). However, this influence began to wane,
such that by the time Castel and colleagues’ (1982) study of U.S. psychiatry was
translated from the French, three years after publication, the foreword had to note the
growing replacement of the psychoanalytic model with such organic and genetic models.
This shift can be linked to three interrelated forces:
1) the use of psychotropic medication began in the 1950s (Castel, Castel, and Lovell
36
1982; Garfield 1989; Lock and Nguyen 2011), which allowed for somatic
treatment of mental illness (Castel, Castel, and Lovell 1982; Kennard 1959) and
also sparked an interest in multi-sited clinical trials (Feighner 1989);
2) research diagnostic categories were standardized in the 1970s through heavily
German (Kraepelian) systems (Young 1995) such as the “Feighner Criteria”
which led to the “Research Diagnostic Criteria” and in turn the DSM-III (Lock
and Nguyen 2011; Spitzer 1997; Winokur 1997), which represented a radical shift
from psychoanalysis – in which the specific diagnosis mattered less because the
treatment was always the same – to a medical model of mental illness (Eyal et al.
2010; Feighner 1989); and
3) deinstitutionalization in the United States (Eyal et al. 2010; Castel, Castel, and
Lovell 1982; Shorter 1997) and elsewhere in Europe (Breemer Ter Stege and Van
Heugten 1986; Salvador-Carulla et al. 2005), perhaps most famously in Italy
(Burti 2001; Burti and Benson 1996; Bollini, Reich, and Muscettola 1988; Crepet
1990; Crepet and De Plato 1983; Jones and Poletti 1986; Santone et al. 2005;
Scheper-Hughes and Lovell 1986), began in the 1950s and a variety of treatments
proliferated to replace this one-size-fits-all approach, leading to greater
importance of diagnostic specificity (Eyal et al. 2010).
These changes ultimately led to the reclassification of psychiatry as even less distinct
from medicine, often as biological psychiatry based on the notion of mental illnesses as
brain diseases (Buchman, Illes, and Reiner 2011; Gaines 1992b) or simply biomedical
psychiatry, psychiatry practiced as a part of biomedicine (Fabrega, Jr. 1993). Since then,
anthropological study of comparative biomedicines, including psychiatries, has
37
blossomed.
The principle framework for the anthropology of biomedicine is interpretive. The
interpretive approach to biomedicine sees medical knowledge as problematic. It rejects
the notion that diseases are “natural” objective givens, sees ethnomedical knowledge as
culturally and historically situated, and grounded in experience-near realities (Gaines
1991). Important concepts in the interpretive approach to biomedicine include
Kleinman’s (1977; 1987; 1988) notion of disease/illness, Kleinman’s (1980) Explanatory
Models, Good’s (Good 1977; Good and Good 1982; Good 1994; Good and Good 1985)
Semantic Illness Networks, and Gaines’ (1991) Sickness Histories. These approaches
explicitly brought together ethnomedicine and ethnopsychiatry, seeing the
organic/psychological difference as constructed and the concepts as equally applicable to
psychological and physical disease entities (Kleinman 1980). Later research has shown
that just as depression, neurasthenia, and PTSD can be read as cultural constructs, so too
can atherosclerosis (Mol 1998; Mol 2000), asthma (Willems 1998), and fetal alcohol
syndrome (Armstrong 2003).
While the interpretive approach to biomedicine resists the notion that diseases are
objective physical entities onto which culture is added, this is not because they naively
disregard the physical world. Rather, the anthropology of biomedicine has been heavily
concerned with bodies through two concepts: the mindful body and local biologies. The
“mindful body” alludes to the previously problematicization of mind/body dualism;
Scheper-Hughes and Lock (1987) outline instead the three bodies: individual, social, and
body politic. This work has kept embodiment a central concern of much later medical
anthropology. “Local biologies” refers to both the simple fact that the entanglement of
38
biological and social processes leads bodies to vary cross-culturally and therefore
universalist claims about how the body works are suspect (Lock and Nguyen 2011; Lock
and Kaufert 2001) – a point also made about human psychology (Henrich, Heine, and
Norenzayan 2010) – and also to the observation that not only biomedicines but the
biologies on which they are based vary cross-culturally (Gaines 2005; Lock 1995).
The textbook history of Western Medicine holds that “rational” medicine was invented
by the Greeks, codified by Hippocrates. Anatomical developments occurred in
Alexandria, and the Greek Galen working in Rome set the standard through the Middle
Ages. After a period of limited professional medicine in early Christianity, translations in
Italy led to the Ars medica in the 11th century. Medical education began at Salerno, Italy,
and university medicine began in Paris and Montpellier, France in the 13th century. Italy,
France, and Spain dominated the field of medicine for several centuries as the traditions
spread across continental Europe and to England, where the move towards a modern,
secular medicine was symbolized in Harvey’s description of blood circulation. France,
Germany, the U.S., and the U.K. became dominant in the era of germ theory (Loudon
1997). The hospital system emerged in France and Germany and then spread across
Europe, the United States, their colonies, and elsewhere (Van Der Geest and Finkler
2004). The development of medicine and science are closely intertwined (Crosland 1976;
Huff 2003), although the directness of their link has been problematized (Canguilhem
1978; Gifford 1986; Keating 2003; Vogel and Rosenberg 1979). After World War II,
scientific research practices were largely standardized across Europe following the U.S.
form, which has been called hegemonic (Krige 2006). Science and technology are
therefore “global assemblages” (Collier and Ong 2005), networks of actors from diverse
39
locales working under the same assumptions and contributing to the same project (Latour
1993). Science and medical practices are mobile concepts in a globalized world, where
understanding the local variations in concepts of self and body affect definition and
practice (Crowley-Matoka and Lock 2006). As this brief history demonstrates, Italy holds
a clearly central position in the history of Western medicine. This dissertation study of
autism and medicine in Italy will contribute to the anthropological project of
demonstrating that the locally particularly and historically contextual reality of
biomedicine.
The Anthropology of Biomedicine has addressed the local particularity of
Western Medicines, often in a comparative framework. Scholars have described the
specific cultures of medicine and psychiatry in the U.S. (Young 1995), France (Dodier
1998; Gaines and Farmer 1986; Sargent and Larchanche 2007), Germany (Hirschauer
1998), the Netherlands (Oudshoorn and Somers 2007; Vermeulen 2004), and Italy
(Gordon 1990; Gordon and Paci 1997; Tanassi 2004; Yamaguchi 2004). They have
contrasted childbirth in the U.S., Holland, and Sweden (Jordan 1993); abortion in former
East and West Germany (Erikson 2003); IQ testing (Carson 2004), psychiatric diagnoses
(Gaines 1992b; Lloyd 2008), and leukemia/lymphoma diagnoses (Keating and
Cambrosio 2000) in the U.S. and France; drug regulation in the U.S., U.K., and West
Germany (Bodewitz, Buurma, and de Vries 1987); autism “brain policies” in the U.S.,
U.K., and Canadian (Baker and Stokes 2007); somaticization in Belgium and medicines
closer to the Mediterranean Sea (Van Moffaert and Vereecken 1989); and genetic testing
in Cyprus and Germany (Beck and Niewohner 2009). They have found overarching
similarities despite local particularities in MRI technology in Germany, Switzerland, and
40
the U.S. (Burri 2007).
One important axis of contrast – the one most germane to this thesis – has been
that of Northern vs. Southern or Mediterranean Europe (Gaines 1982; Markus and
Kitayama 1991). Within the Anthropology of Europe, the concept of a Mediterranean
cultural area has been contested, (de Pina-Cabral 1989; Herzfeld 1984), but remains
useful when used with greater reflexivity and local particularity (Bromberger 2006). Due
to the “rebordering” of the Mediterranean Sea as a boundary between regions rather than
a unifier (Suárez-Navaz 2005), this study takes a Southern European or Mediterranean
European perspective in selecting Italy as a potentially rich contrast with the Northern
European origins of the concept of ASD. Within political science, scholars have further
identified regional differences in social models within the EU. Aiginger and Leoni (2008)
identify Austria as a “continental corporatist,” emphasizing “employment as the basis of
social transfers”; whereas they identify Italy as “Mediterranean model,” in which “the
low level of social transfers is partly counterbalanced by the strong supportive role of
family networks.” These models impact how healthcare and social services function.
The history of ASD reveals a strong influence of Austrians: Leo Kanner, who developed
the idea of “autistic disorder”; Hans Asperger, for whom Asperger’s Syndrome is named;
and Bruno Bettelheim, who ran a famous school for children with autism on the basis of
the theory that autism is caused by emotionally distant “refrigerator mothers.” One Italian
anthropologist therefore calls Asperger’s Syndrome il male austriaco, “the Austrian
disease” (Cola 2012). This project seeks to explore how this Austrian and U.S. diagnostic
category is defined, managed, and experienced in a Southern/Mediterranean European
context.
41
The Anthropology of Biomedicine demonstrates that despite the globalization of
medical research and practice, local particularly remains paramount to the
anthropological study of biomedicine. Not all biomedicines are identical or
interchangeable, due to different conceptualizations of the body, different
conceptualizations of the psyche/soma divide, different bioethics, different legal systems,
different religious influences in biomedicine, different organization of subspecialties,
different biotechnologies, and so on. Key features in the Italian medical system will be
discussed in more detail in Chapter 2.
The Anthropology of Biomedicine, and its overlap with the Anthropology of
Europe, led me to focus on autism in Italy due to the presence of these locally particular
features and the potential for substantial contrast between Italian biomedicine and the
biomedicines of Austria and the United States where autism was first described. This
dissertation will contribute to the Anthropology of Biomedicine through its description of
the local features of the Italian context as they relate to autism. It will demonstrate that
the local particularity of the Italian medical system impacts the way people with autism
and their families define, manage, and experience this diagnosis.
Section 2: The Anthropology of Autism
In previous work (Cascio 2014a), I argue that the Anthropology of Autism is
characterized by, “1) A holistic view that considers the place of autism in the larger
sociocultural context,” “2) Attention to the local and historical particularity of the
concept of autism,” “3) Attention to the lived experience of people with autism and those
close to them” (Cascio 2014a:307). It has become a maxim in anthropology to say that
the field “makes the familiar strange and the strange familiar.” Two threads within the
42
anthropological study of autism encapsulate these two trends in anthropology. The field
that Davidson and Orsini (2013) call “critical autism studies” represents “making the
familiar strange.” Closely linked to science studies, communication studies, and history,
such works investigate the historical particularity of autism diagnoses and reports of an
autism “epidemic.” In short, this body of literature demonstrates the value of not taking
anything for granted when it comes to autism. The cross-cultural study of autism then
makes the strange familiar.
With respect to autism, some scholars broaden the notion of cross-cultural to
include the idea that people with autism participate in their own “autistic cultures,” which
can themselves be an object of study (e.g., Davidson 2008a). There is also a growing
literature on the cross-cultural study of autism as anthropologists typically mean it – the
anthropology of autisms around the globe. This section briefly reviews both trends, and
situates my study within the literature.
Critical Autism Studies
Like earlier literatures on the anthropology of depression (Kleinman and Good
1985b) and schizophrenia (Jenkins and Barrett 2004), the emergent anthropology of
autism unpacks the ways in which the term cannot be taken for granted as an objective
and universal entity, but must be understood as culturally and historically constructed as
are all illness experiences. Autism and the broader Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD)
also share characteristics of Estroff’s “I am” illnesses (Estroff 1993) and Dumit’s “sociomedical disorders” (Dumit 2000). The construction of ASD has been addressed rather by
sociologists (Eyal et al. 2010) and communication scholars (Nadesan 2005). Nadesan
(Nadesan 2005) argues that ASD are what Hacking calls “niche disorders.” Hacking
43
vehemently disagrees (Hacking 2010),4 although he considers autism a “human kind”
(Hacking 1995) or more recently a “moving target” (Hacking 2007) that is “made up”
(Hacking 2006b).
The social scientific study of autism addresses a variety of themes and has
prompted several reviews in the early years of the 21st century (Cascio 2014a; Silverman
2008a; Solomon 2010). Solomon’s (2010) review of autism and the anthropology of the
senses identifies three major contributions of anthropology to the study of autism: 1)
anthropology conceptualizes intersubjectivity and empathy in a practice-based way, not
assuming the anthropologist preternaturally can think like the people he or she is trying to
study, but rather stressing careful attention and observation; 2) anthropology takes a
hermeneutic, interpretive stance focused on lived experience; 3) anthropology’s meaningcentered ethnographic methodology attends to everyday practices. Silverman’s (2008a)
review of such social science “fieldwork on another planet” identifies four major areas of
study that such scholarship has addressed: 1) Autistic cultures 2) Theorizing treatment
(perceptions of efficacy change over time. Should study “how particular therapies emerge
as preferred options for particular contexts, how techniques are designed, and what types
of acquired expertise are required to successfully administer therapies.” 3) autism and
subjectivity. 4) Social movements. In an edited volume, Davidson and Orsini describe the
central features of their “critical autism studies” as a careful attention to power relations,
advancement of enabling (not deficit-focused) narratives of autism, and commitment to
inclusive and non-reductive frameworks (Davidson and Orsini 2013:12).
4
I respectfully disagree with his disagreement (Cascio 2013), for reasons that will be addressed more fully
in Chapter 4.
44
This section respects the insights of these former reviews while addressing the
contributions I find in the social science literature on autism before and since these
works. It addresses specifically contributions in the critical social history of autism; the
study of disability and society with its attention to power relations; studies of the lived
experience of both parents and people with autism; the study of autistic cultures,
communication, and sociality; and the study of autism as a source of identity for both
those on the spectrum and their families.
Anthropologists and other social scientists have produced several detailed social
histories of autism, focused primarily in the United States. These works have
demonstrated the way that the emergence of autism is linked to broader historical and
intellectual trends. Book length manuscripts provide detailed accounts of the emergence
of the autism label from particular historical trends including the emergence of childhood
psychology and compulsory education (Nadesan 2005); deinstitutionalization (Eyal et al.
2010); and scientific movements and technology (Silverman 2012). Grinker (2007) traces
the emergence of an autism “epidemic” due to changing definitions and understandings,
but also attempts to gather what little information existed at the time on autism crossculturally. His work will be discussed also in the following section on cross-cultural
autism studies. Shorter works have similarly unpacked the role of broader economic
forces (Bumiller 2013; Nadesan 2013). They have also presented ethnographic research
on scientists and practitioners (Cascio 2015a; Eyal et al. 2014; Fitzgerald 2014; Lappé
2014). As I observe elsewhere (Cascio 2014a), these texts show “that progressive
narratives of the ever onward march of science do not accurately represent the changing
trends in the treatment and management of autism. Rather, what treatment programs and
45
research initiatives take hold depends on the larger scientific-political context.” These
social histories of autism science, scientists, and treatment serve as one of the greatest
contributions to the making strange of familiar discourses about autism by placing
science, scientists, and treatment in historical context rather than considering them
“objective.”
Anthropologists and other social scientists have also studied autism in the context
of contemporary and historical relationships between “disability and society,” between
people identified as disabled and the broader society that identifies them as such. Several
studies address the impact of broader gender politics on aspects of autism, which is
overwhelmingly diagnosed in boys and men (Fombonne 2005). Some argue that this
diagnostic difference may reflect differences in the way autism manifests based on
socialization (Davidson 2008b) or is observed by parents and physicians with gendered
expectations, drawing on and perpetuating gender stereotypes (Davidson 2008b; GillisBuck and Richardson 2014). Moreover, girls and women with autism may face more
challenges “enacting normalcy” in a non-autistic world based on gender stereotypes and
expectations such as participating in gossip talk (Davidson 2008b). Similar observations
have also been made for diagnostic differences between different racial or ethnic groups
(Begeer et al. 2009; Liptak et al. 2008; Mandell et al. 2007; Mandell et al. 2009) or
socioeconomic status (Liptak et al. 2008) in several contexts.
Many studies in this category address the idea of inclusion for people with autism
in peer and community groups such as school, work, and recreation. While many studies
in education and public policy address these topics using metrics relevant to those fields
(Boutot and Bryant 2005; Carter et al. 2008; Owen-DeSchryver et al. 2008; Symes and
46
Humphrey 2010), there are also several experiential quality studies that focus on the
experiences of people with autism with such peer and community settings (Humphrey
and Lewis 2008; Humphrey and Symes 2011; Ochs et al. 2001). Demographic
differences come into play here as well, as life cycle expectations in the United States in
particular are often based on specific white middle-class values of autonomy and
independence that do not apply to all cultural groups within the United States and
elsewhere (Magaña and Smith 2006; Smith and Routel 2009). Another major area of
study under the rubric of disability and society is that of representation. Several scholars
have analyzed the representation of autism and people with autism in various media
(Hacking 2009b; Hacking 2010; Murray 2008; Osteen 2008). Others have focused on
specific metaphors describing people with autism as various tropes such as robots or
empty fortresses (Broderick and Ne’eman 2008; Danforth and Naraian 2007; Dinishak
and Akhtar 2013; Fein 2011; Hendriks 2012; Waltz 2003). Studies in the category of
disability and society challenge naturalizing and individualizing narratives of deficit
located in the person with the diagnosis, consideration interactions between the individual
and the broader society that impact identification of disability, social inclusion of the
person so identified, and representation of the disability and the person so labelled within
the broader context.
The social study of autism, and especially the anthropology of autism, has studied
the topic not only at these macrological levels, but also “on the ground,” at the level of
lived experience. They employ “experience-near” (Geertz 2000) strategies to understand
the daily lives and subjectivities of people affected by autism – both those with the
diagnosis and their families. Studies with parents of people with autism address various
47
facets of their use of scientific knowledge, including decision-making about having future
children based on genetic responsibility (Raspberry and Skinner 2007; Raspberry and
Skinner 2010) and doubting vaccine safety (Kaufman 2010); parents’ relationships with
professionals and service providers (Valentine 2010); their experiences as research
participants (Lappé 2014); and their grassroots activism (de Wolfe 2014; Ryan and Cole
2009) Studies with siblings have explored their definitions of autism and sibling
relationships (Hwang and Charnley 2010; Olney and Kim 2001; Orsmond, Kuo, and
Seltzer 2009). Studies with people with autism themselves have explored how they
described themselves, understood disability, and whether or not they identified as having
a disability (Cola 2012; Daley and Weisner 2003; Humphrey and Lewis 2008; Jones
2012; Shtayermman 2009), as well as their experiences with autism pride communities
(Bagatell 2007; Bagatell 2010). In considering the lived experiences of both people with
autism and their families, gender remains a prominent concern. Not only is autism most
often diagnosed in boys and men, as discussed above, much of the responsibility for
caretaking falls on women – mothers and other women. Several texts have considered
this gendered labor (Bumiller 2009; Silverman 2012). This work has explored “‘autism
moms’ and dads who ‘fix things’ (Silverman 2012), speakerphone dads (Grinker 2007),
and other gendered tropes of lived and performed experience including refrigerator
mothers, mother warriors, autistic computer geeks, and autism dads (Jack 2014). This
work reflects the anthropological commitment to understanding autism, and all of human
experience, on the ground and enmeshed in its social context and social relationships.
These scholars who study people with autism take a variety of perspectives. One
perspective which I highlight here is especially important to the anthropology of autism:
48
the study of “autistic cultures” and related autistic sociality and autistic communication.
These studies seek to understand autistic ways of being. They tend to start from a
presumption of competence (Biklen and Attfield 2005), recognizing people with autism
as social and communicative agents despite stereotypes to the contrary. In this way, they
recognize autism as a way of being, in some ways analogous to the different cultures
anthropologists have always studied. However, instead of being unified by geography,
religion, or lineage, people with autism are unified as a type of imagined community
(Anderson 2006) linked by features of autism. The most prominent, but certainly not
only, way of understanding this grouping is neurological – autism is a form of
“neurodiversity” that links people with autism together into a cultural group, different
from “neurotypicals” who do not share these neurological features (Baker 2006; Boundy
2008; Brownlow 2010; Bumiller 2008; Cascio 2012; Fenton and Krahn 2007; Glannon
2007; Hart 2014; Kapp et al. 2012; Ortega 2009; Valtellina 2011). Studies of autistic
cultures have researched groups of people with autism both online (Davidson 2008a) and
offline (Bagatell 2007; Bagatell 2010), though both highlighting the role of the internet in
facilitating this community-building (see also Davidson 2008b). Several studies also
address a notion of “autistic sociality” and communications styles, not necessarily social
and communication deficits, particularly through the UCLA Ethnography of Autism
Project (Kremer-Sadlik 2004; Ochs et al. 2001; Ochs et al. 2004; Ochs and Solomon
2004; Sirota 2010; Sirota 2004; Solomon 2004; Solomon 2008; Sterponi 2004; Sterponi
and Fasulo 2010). This project focuses on the linguistic and communication styles of
people with autism, it aligns well with the agendas of prominent autism researchers with
autism such as Michelle Dawson who contribute to the study of autistic intelligence
49
(Dawson et al. 2007; Rogers and Dawson 2009; Soulières et al. 2011). Indeed, ‘insider’
narratives, in a sense, have also played a role in the anthropology of autism as scholars on
the autism spectrum also write about their experiences (Prince 2010; Prince Eddings
2013). These studies elaborate a non-deficit narrative of autism, as Davidson and Orsini
advocate “critical autism studies” do, and rethink the possibilities for anthropological
engagement (Solomon and Bagatell 2010). They provide fertile ground for understanding
the lived experience of people with autism.
The presence of autism as an identity has also been widely documented. Autism
identity politics can be compared to previous associations such as the Lesbian Gay
Bisexual Transgrender and Questioning community (Davidson and Henderson 2010) and
Deaf Culture (Davidson and Henderson 2010; Friedner 2010; Senghas and Monaghan
2002). Autism serves as an identity not only for those with the diagnosis, but also their
family members, as part of what Stuart Murray (2008) calls “the family with autism.”
However, not all parents and patients access identity narratives equally. Juliette de Wolfe
(2014) employs the concept of “identification” (Lave and Wenger 1991), demonstrating
that parents may identify more or less with autism – or with the role of being an “autism
warrior” and having an “autism lifestyle” – over different times and places. Often,
scholars discussing autism as an identity employ the frameworks of biosociality and
biological citizenship, which will be discussed in more detail in the next section on “New
Biopolitical Theory.” For now, we turn to the other half of the anthropology of autism:
making the strange familiar through the cross-cultural and global study of autism around
the world.
Cross-Cultural Studies of Autism
50
The bulk of Anglophone social science literature on autism has focused on major
Anglophone countries: the United States, Canada, United Kingdom, and Australia. A few
works have offered comparisons between these countries, particularly with respect to
policy (Baker 2007; Baker and Steuernagel 2009; Baker and Stokes 2007). However, a
growing cross-cultural literature is emerging. An early text compared parental
understandings of autism between Brazil, Greece, Italy, and Germany (Probst 1998). In
2007, anthropologist Roy Richard Grinker’s book Unstrange Minds provided an
overview of autism around the globe, demonstrating cultural variation in how it was
identified, diagnosed, and treated. He presented information on the U.S. and other
countries including South Africa, South Korea, and India. Anthropologists and other
scholars have investigated autism with particular attention to local particularity in Brazil
(Block and Cavalcante 2012; Rios and Costa Andrade in press), Cuba (Sotgiu et al.
2011), France (Chamak 2008; Chamak et al. 2010; Chamak and Bonniau 2013; Orchard
2013), India (Daley 2002; Daley 2004; Daley and Sigman 2002; Mehrotra and Vaidya
2008; Sarrett 2015), Israel (Bilu and Goodman 1997; Vogel and Reiter 2003; Vogel and
Reiter 2004), Italy (Cola 2012; Cola and Crocetti 2011; Russo, Capararo, and Valtellina
2014; Sotgiu et al. 2011; Valtellina 2011; Valtellina 2015), Morocco (Haldane and
Crawford 2010; Hart 2014), Singapore (Poon 2011), South Africa (Grinker et al. 2012),
and South Korea (Grinker and Cho 2013; Grinker et al. 2012; Kang-Yi, Grinker, and
Mandell 2013).
These studies have highlighted several importance differences between the way
autism is defined, managed, and experienced in different social contexts. Scholars have
explored differential diagnoses, such as the use of Reactive Attachment Disorder rather
51
than autism diagnoses in South Korea (Grinker 2007), as well as new definitions such as
South Korean “border children,” the term mothers and teachers use to describe children
with symptoms of autism spectrum disorder who are academically successful (Grinker
and Cho 2013). They have also explored the different salience of various symptoms in
different contexts (Daley 2004). Autism is also managed differently in different contexts.
Several studies have focused on the transition from psychoanalysis to a medical model of
autism, particularly in France (Chamak 2008; Chamak et al. 2010) and Brazil (Block and
Cavalcante 2012; Rios and Costa Andrade in press), as well as other aspects of
professionals’ definitions (Daley and Sigman 2002). They have also looked at parents’
and patients’ experiences with getting diagnoses (Chamak et al. 2010; Cola 2012; Daley
2002) and with different forms of disability rights movements shaped by local contexts
(Chamak 2008; Chamak and Bonniau 2013; Orchard 2013). They have student parentchild interactions in a variety of contexts (Hart 2014; Sarrett 2015; Sotgiu et al. 2011).
Other studies have looked at the way people with autism fit into religious traditions,
specifically Orthodox Judaism, by exploring the role of facilitated communication in
making meaning of autism (Bilu and Goodman 1997), the meaning of Bar/Bat mitzvahs
to youth with disabilities (Vogel and Reiter 2003) and their parents (Vogel and Reiter
2004).
Social science research on autism in Italy, though limited, has addressed several
important aspects of the field that impact this dissertation. Scholars comparing Italy and
Cuba found that Italians with autism had larger social networks due to “technical
figures,” such as therapists (Sotgiu et al. 2011). Anthropologist Mirna Cola (Cola 2012;
Cola and Crocetti 2011) has conducted ethnographic research with people with
52
Asperger’s Syndrome participating in patients’ organization, exploring the various
meanings of the Asperger’s label while noting that people with Asperger’s in her study
did not necessarily use Asperger’s as a primary identity label. Other studies have
explored the empowerment narratives around Asperger’s in the country (Valtellina 2011).
Indeed, the emerging field of Italian Disability Studies (D’Alessio 2011; Medeghini and
Valtellina 2006) has contributed much to the Italian social study of autism, including the
introduction of neurodiversity perspectives (Narzisi and Muratori 1981; Valtellina 2011)
and attempts to trace the history of people with autism to institutions prior to
deinstitutionalization (Russo, Capararo, and Valtellina 2014). Finally, an unpublished
thesis explicates central themes in autobiographical novels by parents of children with
autism, noting that they all seek to give encouragement to parents though in very
different ways; and that thus far autobiographical novels have been the only genre of
autism novels in Italy (no autism fiction, for example) (Ravanelli 2013).
Section 3: New Biopolitical Theory
One approach that appears time and time again in the social study of autism is that
which I call “new biopolitical theory.” Social scientists have a long a history of interest in
the impact of medical institutions on individuals’ identity and subjectivity – and indeed
on the relationships between institutions and individuals broadly. This study considers
subjectivity “the agonistic and practical activity of engaging identity and fate,”
specifically “the inner lives of subjects” as it relates to conceptualizations of self and
identity, conceptualizations of autism, and the relationship between the two (Biehl, et al.
2007:5). Anthropologists have approached health-related subjectivities in many ways
(e.g., Biehl, et al. 2007; Whyte 2009). One prominent field of study draws on Foucault’s
53
concepts of biopower and biopolitics. In History of Sexuality Volume 1 (1990), Foucault
describes two poles of “biopower”: “anatomo-politics,” referring to clinical interaction
with individual bodies; and “biopolitics” referring to state administration of populations
of bodies. Both techniques use the body as source of political control. This description of
biopower was based upon the technologies of the time, particular the medical institutions
Foucault so influentially theorized (Foucault 1994; Foucault 2006). “New biopolitical
theory” is “new” with respect to Foucault’s biopolitical theories because of the changes
in technology described in this subsection.
Scholars have identified autism as an exemplary case of such theories, specifically
biosociality (Buchman, Illes, and Reiner 2011; Friend 2014; Navon and Eyal 2014;
Silverman 2008b), biological citizenship (Orsini 2009), geneticization (Bumiller 2009),
and biopolitical subjectivities about the brain (Fein 2011; Ortega and Choudhury 2011),
although Silverman recants this position in her later work (Silverman 2012). Greenhough
(2011) argues that autism subjectivity goes “beyond” biological citizenship to the idea of
autistic culture or identity. She discusses how a biological citizenship approach might
view famous scientist with autism Temple Grandin, compared to how she says she views
herself. This literature has found social organization around autism as exemplary of new
biopolitical modes of engaging, both among individuals so diagnosed (Fein 2011; Friend
2014; Orsini 2009; Ortega 2009; Ortega and Choudhury 2011; Silverman 2008b) and
their parents (Friend 2014; Orsini 2009; Raspberry and Skinner 2010; Silverman 2008b).
Others have argued that while some parents of children with autism embrace Rose and
Novas’s (Rose and Novas 2005) activist role, others find parental engagement models
overwhelming (Valentine 2010).
54
In order to resolve the issue of new biopolitical theory’s applicability, this section
breaks down the study of new biopolitical theory. It begins by reviewing the emergence
of these new theories in the wake of the Human Genome Project and the Chernobyl
disaster. It then explores four specific themes: molecularization, somatic expertise,
individual identities, and collective identities and socialities. It also addresses criticisms
and limitations of these theories that emerge from the literature. This section then turns,
as did Section 2, to the cross-cultural study of new biopolitical theory. It explores its
geographical limits.
Overview of New Biopolitical Theory
In new biopolitical theory, Foucault’s concept of biopolitics has been updated to
account for changes in the life sciences that occurred in the late 20th century, namely the
focus on genetics and molecular aspects of human bodies (Kay 1993; Keller 2000; Simon
2008). After the Human Genome Project, scholars have argued that nature and society are
both artificial, are both constructs, and both subject to human control (Cambrosio,
Young, and Lock 2000; Jasanoff 2004; Rabinow 1999; Rheinberger 2000). This
molecularization of life led therefore a reformulation of Foucault’s biopolitics into a
“politics of life itself” (Rose 2007a; critiqued in Braun 2007; Raman and Tutton 2010).
The new biopolitical theory encompasses Petryna’s “biological citizenship” (Petryna
2002) and Rabinow’s “biosociality” (Rabinow 1999). The term biological citizenship
comes from Adriana Petryna’s study of Chernobyl survivors in the Ukraine, and has been
expanded by Nikolas Rose and Carlos Novas, who describe it as, “All those citizenship
projects that have linked their conceptions of citizens to beliefs about the biological
existence of human beings, as individuals, as families and lineages, as communities, as
55
population and races, and as a species” (Rose 2007:440). It includes not only the
imposition of citizenship obligations on individuals (responsibilities) but also the
individual enactment of that citizenship upon the state (rights). It is both individualizing
and collectivizing. The individualizing aspect of biological citizenship encompasses a
new somatic expertise in which individuals must “know and manage the implications of
one’s own genome” and body (Rose 2007:134). The collectivizing aspect is biosociality,
which refers to “the likely formation of new group and individual identities and
practices” (Rabinow 1999:413). The term comes from Paul Rabinow’s wordplay and
reversal of “sociobiology” in the 1990s. He states:
If sociobiology is culture constructed on the basis of a metaphor of nature, then in
biosociality nature will be modelled on culture understood as practice. Nature will
be known and remade through technique and will finally become artificial, just as
culture becomes natural [….] This remark involves a large number of issues, but
the only one I will underline here is the likely formation of new group and
individual identities and practices arising out of these new truths. There already
are, for example, neurofibromatosis groups who meet to share their experiences,
lobby for their disease, educate their children, redo their home environment, and
so on. That is what I mean by biosociality. I am not discussing some hypothetical
gene for aggression or altruism. Rather, it is not hard to imagine groups form
around the chromosome 17, locus 16,256, site 654,376 allele variant with a
guanine substitution. Such groups will have medical specialists, laboratories,
narratives, traditions, and a heavy panoply of pastoral keepers to help them
experience, share, intervene, and “understand” their fate. (Rabinow 1999:413)
Although these terms arise from specific historical moments – the tragedy of Chernobyl
and the optimism of the Human Genome Project – the theories have had staying power
and have been expanded to include theories such as genetic citizenship, therapeutic
citizenship, pharmaceutical citizenship, neurochemical and neurostructural selfhood, and
so on. In short, this literature describes new modes of citizenship engagement (both rights
and responsibilities) and new forms of collective and individual subjectivities and
identities.
56
Governmentality is a larger theory that umbrellas over new biopolitical theory.
Particularly, several authors connect biological citizenship with neoliberalism and the
neoliberal subject (Ajana 2012; Briggs and Hallin 2007; El-Haj 2007; SleeboomFaulkner 2011). Along with biomedicines and biosocialities, I argue we should
dehomogenize our discussion of neoliberalism and talk about neoliberalisms. While
Sleeboom-Faulkner (2011) links new biopolitical theory to western neoliberalism,
insights from the Anthropology of Europe and the Mediterranean show that it is more
appropriate to talk about western neoliberalisms in the plural.
With new biomedical practices comes a new role for biomedical patients. Rose
(Rose 2007a) identifies a shift from Parsons’ sick role (Parsons 1951) to the
protoprofessional role of patients as collaborators partly responsible for their own
wellness. This protoprofessionalism is linked to the role of associations in directing
research, with 1980s gay organizations’ AIDS activism as template (Rose and Novas
2005), as patients and their caretakers become collaborators in the medical-research
industries (Bach 1998; Baggott and Forster 2008; Heath 1998; Huyard 2009; Rabeharisoa
and Callon 2004). The gay rights movement and AIDS activism are themselves both
modelled on the U.S. Civil Rights movement of the 1960s, whose influence on later
identity politics movements – including those related to medicine – should not be under
stated. The new biopolitics, however, is differentiated from the study of identity politics
more broadly through the consideration of biopower (Braun 2007; Whyte 2009). Patients
also have the opportunity to take this role sooner, through genetic susceptibility and
presymptomatic diagnoses (Rose 2007b; Rose 2007a).
In addition to autism, scholars have explored the applicability of new biopolitical
57
theory to a variety of conditions understood as biological. They have looked at medical
conditions and labels such as hearing impairment or membership in the “Deaf”
community (Friedner 2010), HIV/AIDS (Alcano 2009; Beckmann and Bujra 2010;
Cassidy 2010; Cataldo 2008; Cuevas Valenzuela and Pérez Zamora 2011; Misra 2006;
Robins 2006; Wilhelm-Solomon 2013), Hepatitis C (Orsini 2008), mild sickle cell
anemia (Fullwiley 2010), inherited blood disorders (Beaudevin 2013), multiple sclerosis
(Block et al. 2011), breast cancer (Gibbon 2008), Alzheimer’s (Burke 2008), chimerism
(Norton and Zehner 2008), gambling addiction (Vrecko 2008) and a variety of rare
genetic disorders (Fitzgerald 2008). They have also studied the biopolitics of medical
products and services such as circumcision (Carpenter 2010), Viagra (Asberg and
Johnson 2009), cryonics (Romain 2010), genetic testing (Curnutte and Testa 2012;
Ducournau et al. 2013; Felt and Müller 2011; Lee and Crawley 2009; Sommer 2010), and
new reproductive technologies (Bharadwaj 2008; Brekke and Sirnes 2011; Nahman
2008); as well as the experiences of research participants (Johnson 2013). Several
scholars have focused on the biomedicalization of race under the new biopolitics (Bliss
2011; El-Haj 2007) as well as the way race (El-Haj 2007) and gender politics are or risk
being reproduced. Another aspect of subjectivity under new biopolitical theory is the
responsibility of optimization, being the best possible such as through “frequent sipping”
of bottled water (Race 2012). They have also explored the biopolitical subjectivities
outside of realms traditionally considered biomedical, such as sexuality (Martucci 2010),
climate change (Whitington 2013) biometrics (Ajana 2012; Mordini and Ottolini 2007),
and the microbiopoliticals of raw milk (Kurtz, Trauger, and Passidomo 2013; Paxson
2008). Scholars have also linked biosociality and biological citizenship to a whole realm
58
of bioeconomics, the treatment of human tissues as goods and their mobility, particularly
in cases such as biobanking (Mitchell 2012), and the flow of information through
biocommunicability (Briggs and Hallin 2007). Through a close reading of this literature, I
have identified four major themes: molecularization, somatic expertise, individual
identities, and collective identities and socialities. I turn to these themes in these
following paragraphs.
Molecularization
Molecularization is a “‘style of thought’ of contemporary biomedicine” which
“envisages life at the molecular level, as a set of intelligible vital mechanisms among
molecular entities that can be identified, isolated, manipulated, mobilized, recombined”
and so on (Rose 2007:5-6). It refers to the conceptualization of life at the minute level of
genes, proteins, and enzymes (Rose 2007a:12), related to the discoveries of molecular
biology (Kay 1993).
There are several forms of molecularization, perhaps most prominently
geneticization (Lippmann 1991). As Lippman explains:
Today’s emphasis on genetic causes and genetic understanding seems to be part
of a more expansive process I have called ‘geneticization’ (and have begun to
describe elsewhere). Those in charge of health-related naming and helping
activities increasingly now reduce differences between individuals to their DNA
codes, define most disorders and behaviors, as well as physiological variations, as
at least in part genetic in origin, advocate the adoption of interventions that
employ genetic technologies to manage problems of health and, in general,
(incorrectly) equate human biology (humanity?) with human genetics, implying
that the latter acts along to make each of us the organism she or he is. (Lippmann
1991:85)
Geneticization is also linked to subjectivities based around genetics, including genetic
citizenship (Heath, Rapp, and Taussig 2004) and genetic responsibility (Raspberry and
Skinner 2010). Furthermore, “Packaging human health problems – as well as a whole
59
range of human variability – as ‘genetic’ blurs the distinctions between disease and
illness, disability and handicap” (Lippmann 1991:87). The blurring of this distinction is
particularly prominent in autism, which occupies an unusual niche somewhere between
mental illness and mental retardation (Eyal et al. 2010), somewhere between psychology,
psychiatry, neurology, and biomedicine.
Geneticization clearly emerged from the optimism of the Human Genome Project.
Particularly after the Human Genome Project failed to bring about the momentous change
in our understanding of the human condition that its early proponents and detractors
anticipated, some scholars have found this discourse limited (Lock 2008; Lock, Lloyd,
and Prest 2006). Eyal and colleagues (2014; Hart 2014) argue that several aspects of
autism research, treatment, and advocacy, especially the “low tech” quality of many
interventions strategies, challenge biomedicalization and geneticization. However, as
recently as 2009, Bumiller identified important trends in these domains that strongly
followed biomedicalization and geneticization even in the absence of clarity about autism
genetics. Hedgecoe (2001) uses the term “enlightened geneticization” to refer to scientific
discourse that prioritizes genetics while allowing for a role for non-genetic factors and
not remaining limited to the notion of a “gene for” a disease or a straightforward
discourse of “genetic diseases.”
Molecularization also encompasses several neurological discourses, particularly
the concept of mental illnesses as “brain diseases” (Buchman, Illes, and Reiner 2011;
Rose 2007a). As Rose explains (Rose 2007a:220), “In this way of thinking, all
explanations of mental pathology must “pass through” the brain and its neurochemistry—
neurons, synapses, membranes, receptors, ion channels, neurotransmitters, enzymes,
60
etcetera.” Neurological molecularization or ‘neurologization’ includes discourses of both
neurochemistry and neurological structure. Scholars have linked these discourses to the
respective subjectivities of neurochemical (Rose 2003; Ortega 2009) and neurostructural
(Fein 2011) selfhood.
Somatic Expertise
Somatic Expertise refers to “new ways of governing human conduct” (Rose
2007:6) which encompass somatic technologies of both patients/parents and
professionals.
Professionals exercise somatic expertise through specializations on “particular
aspects of our somatic existence” and “advise and guide … individuals and families as
they negotiate their way through the personal, medical, and ethical dilemmas that they
face” (Rose 2007:6). In doing so, they act as what Rabinow (1999:413) calls “pastoral
keepers” who “help [patients and families] experience, share, intervene, and ‘understand’
their fate.” Pastoral keepers whose work becomes particularly known may be the
destination for what Rose (2007:6) calls “pilgrimages of hope,” in which patients and
families travel to access cures or treatments.
Patients/parents exercise somatic expertise through lay expertise. Similar to
Rabeharisoa, Moreira, and Akrich (2014), I understand lay expertise to operate among
two lines, which I call experiential expertise and patient literacy. Rabeharisoa and
colleagues (2014) explain this division for organizations:
Secondly, patients’ organisations’ concerns with disease and its exploration has
entailed a redistribution of competencies and prerogatives between patients and
credentialed experts. Certain patients and activists acquired scientific and medical
knowledge, becoming what Epstein (1995) labelled ‘lay experts’, i.e. credible
interlocutors of specialists. Patients’ organisations also collected, formalised and
circulated patients’ experience as a legitimate body of ‘experiential knowledge’
61
(Arskey 1994; Borkman 1976) on their conditions, and became what Rabeharisoa
& Callon (2004) called ‘experts of experience’. This two-fold role played by
patients’ organisations, as ‘lay experts’ and ‘experts of experience’, has led to
new forms of cooperation between patients and specialists in the production,
discussion and dissemination of knowledge. This transformation extended the
repertoire of styles of mobilisation enacted by patients’ and users’ organisations,
adding to forms of contestation new modalities of articulation that bring together
these organisations and specialists to negotiate the scope of epistemic arenas for
their conditions.
I note a similar two-fold role, though also for individuals and not limited to organizations.
I use the term “Experiential Expertise” (Borkman 1976; Hughes 2009; Rabeharisoa,
Moreira, and Akrich 2014; Rabeharisoa and Callon 2004) to describe the ways in which
patients/parents challenge medical authority and assert that they may know better than the
doctors by virtue of their personal and familial experiences. I take the term from
Borkman (1976), who explains:
Experiential knowledge is truth learned from personal experience with a
phenomenon [….] Besides experiential knowledge, there is experiential expertise
[…] which refers to competence or skill in handling or resolving a problem
through the use of one’s own experience [….] Thus experiential expertise can
serve as a basis for leadership, a higher status in the group, and a source of
authority for decision making.
Patients/parents may also exercise lay expertise by internalizing, not challenging, medical
authority. They do this through patient literacy, reading up on the diagnosis and
cultivating medical expertise. I use this term rather than “lay expertise” to avoid the
ambiguity of the origin of this expertise, dividing it based on the source of the
authoritative knowledge (Davis-Floyd and Sargent 1997). Moreover, it is also important
to understand the nuance of the mobilization of lay expertise and its relationship to
professional expertise. Hughes (2009) contrasts the lay expertise of new biopolitical
theory (biosociality) with that of the “social model stalwarts” of the Disabled Peoples’
Movement:
62
If the [Disabled People’s Movement] valorizes the experiential knowledge of
disabled people, biosocial associations are more likely to embrace a more
complex epistemological position that has been described as a ‘coalition or
hybridisation of experiential knowledge, lay expertise and counter-expertise’
(Hess 2004, 697).
One important concept related to somatic expertise is that of “somatic individuality,”
referring to “beings whose individuality is […] ground within our fleshy, corporeal
existence, and who experience, articulate and judge, and act upon ourselves in part in the
language of biomedicine” (Rose 2007a:26). One such form of somatic individuality is the
cerebral subject, “an anthropological figure that embodies the belief that human beings
are essentially reducible to their brains” (Ortega and Vidal 2007). Brekke and Sirnes
(2011) 2011 present instead the concept of “hypersomatic Individuals” who are more
soldier than citizen. This metaphor harkens back to militarization metaphors elsewhere in
biomedicine, most notably “fighting” cancer (Erwin 1987). It appears also in autism
studies, as parents – particularly mothers – take on identification with a role as “autism
warriors” (de Wolfe 2014). Niewohner (2011) explores molecularization under
epigenetics, claiming that it relies not on somatic expertise but somatic sociality,
explaining “People do not need to translate their somatic self-understanding into a way of
life any longer [….] I suggest the term somatic sociality to mark this phenomenon.
Somatic sociality means sociality understanding and reproducing itself on the basis of
biological knowledge about its epigenetic effects on the individual body. In other words,
somatic sociality is a form of sociality where the collectivizing momentum is provided by
molecular understandings of social life itself. It is not individuals with the same
polymorphism understanding themselves through that polymorphism or forming a
collective on the basis of that polymorphism. It is individuals slotting into forms of social
63
life that molecular biology produces as forms of social life that are good for your body
and your health, for example, you have friends but do not expose yourself to large socioeconomic differences.” In both somatic individuality and somatic sociality, nature is
remodeled on culture. However, “Somatic sociality refers not to the collectivization of
individuals on the basis of biological knowledge of a shared phenotype. Instead, the
notion of somatic sociality marks instances and forms of sociality and of social life that
are directly modelled on nature. Particular forms of social life come to be known and
practised increasingly through their somatic, that is, ‘natural’ consequences.”
Somatic individuality blurs the line between professional and patient/parent.
Patients and family members become experts in their condition of concern (Novas and
Rose 2000; Rose 2007a:134). This type of patient responsibility involves a role
significantly from Talcott Parsons' classic (1951) sick role, as in genetic responsibility
where “the patient is to become skilled, prudent, and active, an ally of the doctor, a
protoprofessional, and to take a share of the responsibility for getting themselves better”
(Rose 2007a:110). Patients/parents are responsibilized in that they are expected to study
and use this medical expertise. They are “urged to become active and responsible
consumers of medical services and products” (Rose 2007a:4), to collaborate with
professionals, and to become protoprofessionals themselves. Moreover, they must take
responsibility for themselves outside of strict medical expertise, demonstrating that they
are the right kind of patient to be deserving of medical care. As Robins (2006) about
responsibilized ARV patients in South Africa, “In terms of this call for a paradigm shift,
clients would be entitled to free government health care, including ARV drugs, but they
would also need to demonstrate that they were ‘responsibilized clients’— specifically
64
through treatment adherence, disclosing their HIV status, using condoms, abstaining from
alcohol abuse and smoking, and having healthy diets and lifestyles.” Responsibilized
patients use “technologies of the self” (Foucault 1988) to monitor and intervene on their
own body-selves (Murray 2009; Race 2012; Taussig, Rapp, and Heath 2003). Taussig,
Rapp and Heath (2003:59) succinctly define these technologies as “the practices by
which subjects constitute themselves, and work to improve themselves, while living
within institutional frameworks of power.” These subjects are responsible for consuming
and understanding biomedical knowledge and applying it to themselves on a daily basis.
They are also responsible for making informed consumer decisions about biomedical
services, products, and strategies.
Illness Identities
“Illness identities” refers to the mobilization of medical/psychiatric labels as
identity categories. The literal definition of identity is “sameness,” the recognition of
similarity between self and others (Bucholtz and Hall 2006; Sökefeld 1999). In new
biopolitical theory as identification of biological sameness leads to the formation of
biosocial groups (Rabinow 1999). However, anthropologists understand the subjective
experience of identity as more complex than sameness/difference (Bucholtz and Hall
2006). This study recognizes identity as something that a reflexive self (following
(Hallowell 2010) enacts through agentive self-presentation in social contexts (Sökefeld
1999). Furthermore, those employing biosocial identities do not simply accept biology as
a given; they contribute to the construction of the biologies on which these identities are
based (Wehling 2011). The bases of categories are constantly redefined in what Hacking
calls the “looping effect” of “making up people,” categories such as “people with autism”
65
(Hacking 2007). The use of autism-as-an-identity has been amply documented (Bagatell
2010; Chamak 2008; Kapp et al. 2012; Orsini 2009; Ortega 2009; Ortega and Choudhury
2011). Ortega explores one form of illness identity specifically relevant to people with
autism: “cerebral subjectivation.” This term refers to brain discourses in which “people
define themselves as autistics … because their brains are ‘wired’ in an atypical way”
(Ortega 2009). Autism functions as an identity not only for those diagnosed, but for the
whole family. I use Stuart Murray’s (Murray 2008) term “the family with autism” to refer
to identification as an “autism parent” – an individual identity based around autism for
parents of people with the diagnosis, not people with the diagnosis themselves. I refer
here to parent identification as a certain type of parent – and autism parent or, as Juliette
de Wolfe recently wrote, “autism warriors.” I consider this membership in an imagined
community.
Collective Identities and Biosocialities
Collective identities and biosocialities refers to membership in less imagined
communities and more support groups and advocacy organizations. Collective identities
and biosocialities refers to collectivities around conditions, risks, or experiences
understood to be biological. Rose describes three forms of biocitizenship: informational,
which I use to mean the role of collectivities and parent networking in sharing
information both scientific/medical and administrative; rights biocitizenship in which
patients/parents “[campaign] for better treatment, ending stigma, getting access to
services, and like” (Rose 2007a:135); and digital biocitizenship, in which patients/parents
use the internet to find information, resources, and connections.
Not all patients can access biological citizenships and citizenship collectivity
66
claims. For example, several scholars have explored the limits of new biopolitical modes
of engagement for stigmatized or socially less desirable groups such as first generation
Korean-Americans (Kim, Haney, and Hutchinson 2012), Nicaraguan immigrants in Costa
Rica (Goldade 2009), Hepatitis C patients in Australia (Fraser 2010) and the UK
(Rhodes, Harris, and Martin 2013) who are constructed as “bad” citizens and limited in
their access to new biopolitical modes of engagement.
Cross-Cultural Studies of New Biopolitical Theory
As discussed in Section 1, biomedicines vary from country to country, context to
context. Similarly, biopolitics vary. New biopolitical theory has been found useful in the
United States (Block et al. 2011; Carpenter 2010; Norton and Zehner 2008; Rapp 2000b;
Rapp 2000a; Raspberry and Skinner 2007; Raspberry and Skinner 2010), Canada (Orsini
2008; Orsini 2009), the United Kingdom (Carpenter 2010; Gibbon 2007; Gibbon 2008;
Roberts 2007), New Zealand (Fitzgerald 2008), Sweden (Asberg and Johnson 2009),
Norway (Brekke and Sirnes 2011), Germany (Guell 2011; Kampf 2010), Austria (Felt
and Müller 2011), the Ukraine (Petryna 2002), and Italy (Alcano 2009). It has been found
to not be applicable in other settings, such as in Mozambique where HIV/AIDS
organizations compete and instead of biosociality there is “biosociopathy” (Kalofonos
2010), and in Tanzania where the role of the biological is not as important as pre-exist
social networks, making “(bio)sociality” a more appropriate term (Marsland 2012) Some
scholars have therefore identified a geographical focus for new biological theory, linking
biosociality to “Europe and North America” in contrast to its absence in Ecuador
(Roberts 2008), or contrasting “Western” bio-citizenship and neoliberal governance with
China (Sleeboom-Faulkner 2011). Biosociality has also been reworked in India, where
67
Bharadwaj finds “bio-availability” more useful (Bharadwaj 2008). However, others have
associated biosociality with “Europe and North America” yet nonetheless argued for its
applicability in Brazil (Cataldo 2008). Certainly, new biopolitical theory has also been
explored outside Europe and North America in such countries as Chile (Cuevas
Valenzuela and Pérez Zamora 2011), China (Lora-Wainwright 2009), Israel (Nahman
2008), Oman (Beaudevin 2013), Senegal (Fullwiley 2010), Singapore (Reubi 2010),
South Africa (Robins 2006), Tanzania (Beckmann and Bujra 2010), Uganda (WilhelmSolomon 2013), the Gambia (Cassidy 2010), and Burkina Faso (Nguyen 2005); as well as
in India (Ecks 2005; Misra 2006); and on-line networks extended to support parents’ in
Latin America as well as Europe, Australia, the US, and Canada (Rapp 2003).
Moreover, these theories have been challenged at least in the Anglophone world.
Anthropologists have found striking absences of biosociality in U.S. rare genetic disease
organizations (Panofsky 2010) and among U.S. research participants in an enriched-risk
cohort study of autism (Lappé 2014), not to mention varying levels of participation in
biosocial organizations by social position (Rapp 2000a); they have also noted the absence
of geneticization concepts for Alzheimer’s in the U.S., U.K., and Canada (Lock 2008;
Lock, Lloyd, and Prest 2006); Hacking notes that Canadian transhumanists seem to resist
biosocial identities (Hacking 2006a). Similar to Marsland’s (2012) (bio)sociality, Celia
Roberts proposes the hyphenated term bio-sociality in her study of hormone replacement
therapy, “to insist that the implosion of the biological and the social is not as advanced or
wide-ranging as [other] work sometimes implies” (2007:21). They have taken issue with
the “political economy of hope,” highlighting instead its “darker side” – fear and new
obligations (Brekke and Sirnes 2011; Charles 2013). Scholars in Italy have argued that
68
Italian biological citizenship is more akin to Petryna’s in Ukraine (Petryna 2002) than
Rose’s (Novas and Rose 2000; Rose 2007a; Rose and Novas 2005) “advanced liberal
societies” (Metzler 2007), and that biosociality is limited by the lack of patients’
consumer role (Cola and Crocetti 2011). In accounting for variations, Beck and
Niewohner suggest the term “bio-cultural intimacy” to refer to a theory of biosociality
that takes historical and cultural contingencies better into account (Beck and Niewohner
2009). Others, however, question whether the new genetic sciences and resultant
subjectivities are really all that new and propose instead a plurality of politics of life
(Raman and Tutton 2010). Still others question the “citizenship” of biological citizenship
if people are not able to exercise citizenship capabilities (Árnason 2013).
Another challenge to new biopolitical theory comes in part from the wide range of
approaches to its study in both the social sciences and humanities. Particularly with the
range of studies drawing from published materials such as advertising or specific targeted
communities such as online forums, literature on new biopolitical theory often gives the
materialist impression that new biomedical technologies themselves lead to new identities
and subjectivities. Ethnographic studies complicate this relationship by demonstrating
variability in the uptake of new biopolitical theory by demographic and other factors
(Rapp 2000a), by exploring new biopolitical themes in the production (not just
consumption) of biomedical knowledge and technologies (Eyal et al. 2014; Fitzgerald
2014; Lappé 2014), and by linking new biopolitical modes of engagement to historically
relevant modes of engagement in a particular locality (Marsland 2012). This dissertation
enriches new biopolitical theory by exploring the applicability of the new biopolitical
themes listed above “on the ground,” offline and in the day to day lives of people with
69
autism and their families who are actively participating in autism-centric activities.
The applicability of the new biopolitics is therefore muddled. It may be local, it
may depend upon the aspect of post-Human Genome medicine under consideration, or it
may be nothing at all. It is therefore worthy of investigation. More specifically, just as
anthropologists have found that biomedicines vary by locality, so too may biosocialities
vary or be absent in different biomedical contexts.
Several factors make Italian medicine particularly well-suited for this study.
Firstly, in the 1970s Basaglia famously initiated a world-renowned movement of deinstitutionalization (Scheper-Hughes and Lovell 1986). This movement led to policies of
social and educational integration, including hiring quotas for employers (Crialesi et al.
2007:275), and the laws of integrazione scolastica mandating the inclusion of students
with disabilities in public schools (Canevaro and de Anna 2010). Secondly, Italian
medicine remains, for the most part, non-privatized; therefore patients lack the consumer
role seen in systems like U.S. healthcare (Cola and Crocetti 2011). Finally, doctor-patient
interaction can be paternalistic, as in the case of non-disclosure of cancer diagnoses
(Gordon 1990; Gordon and Paci 1997). For these reasons, we might expect new
biopolitical subjectivities to be minimized. Biosociality is exclusive by its specificity,
whereas social integration is inclusive. A lack of a consumer role for patients may
minimize their mobilization of lay expertise, as would paternalistic doctor-patient
relationships.
Section 4: The Anthropology of Youth
In order to explore health identities and subjectivities around autism, this study
focuses on adolescents and adults ages 14-34 labelled with autism spectrum conditions.
70
My research design identifies adolescents and youth as the target population for this
study due to specific research question (3) on new biopolitical theory. As discussed in
Section 3, new biopolitical theory is concerned with subjectivity, specifically sociality
and identity, around autism (Fein 2011; Ortega 2009; Rabinow 1999; Rose 2007;
Silverman 2008). Following Erikson (1968), anthropologists have found adolescence to
be near-universally a time of both increased sociality with non-family peers, and identity
development. This makes adolescence the ideal stage in which to address these questions.
Therefore, this study draws heavily from the anthropological fields which address this
age group. This section very briefly reviews the Anthropology of Adolescence, Youth
Cultures, and the Anthropology of Youth. It also reviews studies of youth with autism
that are relevant to the major themes in the Anthropology of Youth.
Human development generally and adolescence in particular have been central
concerns in anthropology since Mead’s famous (Mead 2001), but debated (Freeman
1983; Holmes 1988; Orans 1996), study of Samoan adolescence. This work provided an
ethnographic veto to Stanley Hall’s theory of universal “storm and stress” in adolescence
(Hall 1904). Though there was a pause in attention to adolescence directly following
Mead’s work, later psychological anthropologists and anthropologists of human
development paid greater attention to adolescence (Anderson-Fye 2010; Brown and
Larson 2002; Fong 2006; Fussell and Greene 2002; Mortimer and Larson 2002; Korbin
and Anderson-Fye 2011; LeTendre 2000; Parikh and Adams 2005; Weisner and Lowe
2005), particularly following the series Adolescents in a Changing World project
(Burbank 1988; Condon 1988; Davis 1999; Hollos 1989) and the Adolescent
Socialization Project (Schlegel and Barry III 1991).
71
The life course approach of the Anthropology of Adolescence is often contrasted
with the approach of Youth Cultures (Bucholtz 2002; Eyre, Hoffman, and Millstein 1998;
Lave et al. 1992), generally associated with British social anthropology and cultural
studies. Many studies focus on media and popular culture/subculture (Hebdige 1991;
Kinsella 1995). Whereas the life course approach is concerned with developmental
stages, socialization, and transition, the Youth Cultures approach is concerned with
adolescents in the present, irrespective of developmental trajectories. It tends to be
associated with place, often schools (Bucholtz 1999; Bucholtz 2001; Corsaro 1996;
Corsaro and Molinari 2000; Davis, Watson, and Cunningham-Burley 2000). This
scholarship is often concerned with identity (Bucholtz 1999; Bucholtz 2001; Bucholtz
and Hall 2006; Mendoza-Denton 1996; Ortega and Choudhury 2011; Vestel 2009) and
sexual culture (Yan 1999).
Bucholz proposes a synthesis which she has called “the anthropology of youth”
which respects youth as agents beyond being unfinished adults, while still attending to
diachronic factors (Bucholtz 2002; Cole 2004).
Anthropologists generally define adolescence as the time between physiological
puberty and the assumption of full adult social roles. The anthropology of adolescence
has addressed many domains within this life stage, including peer interaction, identity,
sexuality, and ritual. It is complemented by the field of “youth cultures” and the newer
“anthropology of youth” synthesis, which focus more on adolescents as “youth” engaged
in cultural practices that are significant in their own right, rather than only as stepping
stones to adulthood (Bucholtz 2002). In the case of developmental disorders such as
autism, it is understandable that attention would focus on issues of development. Many
72
studies of adolescents with autism are aimed at increasing future function through focus
at this time, therefore taking an approach most similar to the anthropology of
adolescence.
Autism spectrum conditions have often been considered disorders of childhood,
although attention has more recently been turned towards adults. Adolescence, as a life
stage somewhere in the middle, often gets overlooked. Scholarship on autism and
adolescence is therefore rather specialized. This paper provides a brief review of such
scholarship. It is divided into sections based on topics of particular anthropological
interest: rites of passage into adolescence, identity development, peer relationships,
sexuality, and the transition to adulthood. Each section couches literature on autism
within an anthropological discussion of adolescence and youth.
Rites of Passage
Rites of passage have long been of anthropological interest, and clearly relate to a
life stages approach. Some have claimed difficulty in identifying clear rites of passage in
contexts such as the United States, making transitions between life stages difficult to
demark socially (children, tweens, teens, young adults, etc.). Some have pointed to events
like Prom. One clear rite of passage exists in the Jewish religious tradition, both in Israel
and among Jewish populations living in other countries. This is of course the Bar or Bat
Mitzvah, which occurs at age 13 and officially signals a Jewish person’s entrance into
social and religious adulthood. However, the adulthood of an initiated person has been
contested. The rite of passage may perhaps more accurately be thought of as entrance into
adolescence. A small collection of writing addresses the experience of the Bar/Bat
Mitzvah for persons diagnosed with autism and similar conditions, and focuses on
73
strategies for empowering young people with disabilities to participate in the ritual and
the meaning of their participation for the youth themselves and for their parents (Hyman
2009; Vogel and Reiter 2003; Vogel and Reiter 2004).
Identity
Erikson noted the importance of identity development in adolescence (generally
ages 12-18). Anthropologists have agreed with this focus and studied identity extensively
in this life stage and in peer cultures studies. As discussed above, the role of autism as an
identity has been widely documented (Bagatell 2010; Nadesan 2005; Orsini 2009; Ortega
2009). In these cases, autism is not only something someone “has” following APA
person-first language, it is also an identity akin to GLBT or Deaf (Davidson and
Henderson 2010). Many people taking autism as an identity prefer therefore to use the
term “autistic” (Davidson and Henderson 2010). Several studies have addresses
important aspects of autism and identity in persons with autism in a more Youth Cultures
framework, so to speak, without attention to life cycle.
Ortega (2009) discusses the use of brain discourses by self-advocates that express
“cerebral selfhood,” consistently largely of the metonymous use of “my brain” for “I.”
Fein (2011) elaborates upon this in her discussion of “neurostructural selves,” drawing on
data from adults self-advocates and those who work with adolescents with Asperger’s
syndrome. Both Ortega (2009) and Fein (2011) stress that the use of such brain
discourses present autism spectrum conditions as “morally innocent.” In other words, the
problem behaviors of autism are rooted in brain structure, and do not reflect individual
failures to overcome them the way that “psychiatric” conditions might (Fein 2011).
Ortega and Choudhury (2011) explain that these self-concepts lead to “neurological
74
identities” based on the discourse that brain differences are natural not pathological. This
use of neuroscientific discourse explicitly contrasts with typically-developing
adolescents’ rejection of neuroscientific discourses about their deficient brains (Ortega
and Choudhury 2011).
Humphrey and Lewis (2008) used semi-structured interviews, participant diaries,
and participant drawings to understand the subjective experience of pupils with
Asperger’s syndrome in inclusive educational settings in the UK. They found
“constructing an understanding of AS [Asperger’s Syndrome]” to be a central theme in
student data. Some students reported that having the label of Asperger’s Syndrome made
them feel “not normal” and fear being seen as “retarded,” while others saw Asperger’s as
a valuable part of “who they were,” particularly with peer support (Humphrey and Lewis
2008).
Bagatell (2007) presents a person-centered ethnography of Ben, a 21-year old
adolescent college student diagnosed with high-functioning autism. Bagatell (2007)
addresses Ben’s identity development through 9 months of fieldwork and retrospective
data gained from Ben’s storytelling. Earlier in his adolescence, Ben tried to conform to an
idea of normal, particularly in his school settings. However, he experienced severe stress
and suicide ideation that led to hospitalization. After this event he became involved in the
“Aspie” community of self-advocates and took up new vocabularies and a new identity as
an “Aspie” – a person with Asperger’s syndrome or, in Ben’s case, high functioning
autism. The Aspie world allowed Ben to stop “pretending” to be normal and embrace the
way his mind worked. However, Ben experienced tension “orchestrating voices” of both
the Aspie world and the mainstream world in which he attended college. Ben later
75
attempted suicide a second time. Bagatell’s (2007) in-depth exploration of Ben’s identity
development demonstrates that orchestrating such voices takes hard work, and that all
identities are in a state of constant revision.
Peer Relationships
Anthropologists have identified peer relationships as a cross-cultural and indeed
cross-species near-universal characteristic of adolescence. In adolescence, humans and
other species intensify peer relationships, especially homosocial relationships. Large
cross-cultural studies have found that boys tended to leave the home in age-sets but girls
tended to remain in the home and have relationships across ages (Schlegel and Barry III
1991). Although researchers have found that the social and interpersonal skills of persons
with autism improve with age (Mesibov and Handlan 1997), some studies show the gap
between individuals with autism and typically developing peers widens at adolescence as
these skills become more complex (Duncan and Klinger 2010; Locke, et al. 2010), and
others find that reciprocal friendships are more common when youth with autism are
younger (Orsmond, Krauss, and Seltzer 2004). As Howard and colleagues (2006) note in
their case study of a 12 year old home-schooled boy in the US about to enter public
school, “adolescence may be a particularly challenging time for people with ASDs
because of the increasing importance of intimate relationships.” An exploratory sample of
US adolescence with Asperger’s Syndrome found high levels of peer victimization and
stigma for participants across educational settings, by self and parental report
(Shtayermman 2009). More in-depth qualitative research with UK students with
Asperger’s confirmed that peer victimization is high, although peer support also impacts
the lives of students in inclusive educational settings (Humphrey and Lewis 2008). This
76
subsection explores peer relationships across a variety of settings.
One of the first peers US children often know are in the home, their siblings.
Orsmond, Kuo, and Seltzer (2009) based their study of sibling relationships in the context
of autism on findings in typical psychology that sibling relationships tend to decrease in
adolescence when this outside peer relationship becomes more important, and then
increase in later adulthood. Orsmond and colleagues (2009) lamented that previous
research on sibling relationships when one sibling has autism have focus on childhood or
a range of ages. The authors wanted to know how the sibling relationship changes over
time when one sibling is labeled with an autism spectrum condition, and explicitly
compared adolescent and adult sibling dyads. They administered questionnaires
measuring positive affect and shared activities. Both groups reported similar positive
affect. The diversity of shared activities varied based on the genders of the siblings
among adults, but not among adolescents. These findings suggest that the sibling
relationship was more stable over time in terms of closeness than in the literature on
typically developing adolescents (Orsmond, et al. 2009).
More studies of peer interaction among adolescents labeled with autism spectrum
conditions have taken place in institutional settings, such as schools. Compulsory
schooling lengthens adolescence, brings age-mates together, and intensifies peer group
formation (Brown and Larson 2002). For individuals with autism, an important facet of
compulsory schooling has been national trends towards inclusive education (Begeny and
Martens 2007), particularly following the United Nations Convention on the Rights of
Persons with Disabilities which stated the right to inclusion in free and compulsory
education (Nations 2006). Several scholars of education have investigated the
77
experiences of children and adolescents with autism in inclusive settings in the US
(Boutot and Bryant 2005; Carter et al. 2008; Ochs et al. 2001) and UK (Humphrey and
Lewis 2008; Humphrey and Symes 2011; Symes and Humphrey 2010). A US study
(Carter et al. 2008) of students with developmental disabilities in inclusive classrooms
found that social interaction between those with and without disabilities was higher in
small group instruction without support teachers, but academic outcomes were better
during one to one or small group instruction with general or special educators nearby.
Another US study (Ochs et al. 2001) found that the role of other children may be in fact
more important than educators in facilitating the social inclusion of children with autism.
Peer training programs tap into this potential (Owen-DeSchryver et al. 2008). A UK
study used spot observations of 38 secondary school students with autism spectrum
conditions from 12 mainstream schools and compared their peer interaction patterns with
students with dyslexia and students with no identified special education needs
(Humphrey and Symes 2011). The students with autism spectrum conditions spent more
time in solitary pursuits, less time in co-operative interaction, and more time reacting to
aggression from peers than participants in the other two groups. Students with autism
spectrum conditions also engaged in less rough/vigorous play and experienced more
(positive) social initiation as well as (negative) verbal aggression from peers. The authors
found that students with autism spectrum conditions may have recognized neither social
initiations nor bullying, given their lack of response to either. They suggest increased
social skills interventions in these specific areas (Humphrey and Symes 2011).
Locke and colleagues (2010) similarly investigated peer interaction among
adolescents in an inclusive US school. Their 7 high-functioning participants were all
78
enrolled in a drama class at mainstream high school along with 13 typically developing
peers. The authors gave all students survey measures of loneliness and social networks
asking them to list their friends. Adolescents with autism spectrum conditions reported
more loneliness than their typically developing peers. Adolescents labeled with autism
spectrum conditions tended to list another adolescent with autism as a “best friend.”
These friendship dyads were comparable to typically developing friendships in most
domains but “poorer” in terms of companionship and helpfulness. However, the authors
argue that such friendships may be characterized as appropriate for persons with autism
and be equally developmentally useful as the more companionate and helpful
relationships of the typically developing students (Locke, et al. 2010). Similar to
Humphrey and Symes (2011), the authors argue for increased social skills interventions
(Locke, et al. 2010). Both studies found that inclusion, though based on sound theoretical
goals, was not sufficient in and of itself to accomplish strong social networks. Locke and
colleagues less so than Hymphrey and Symes assume social networks as indeed the goal,
as they allow for the conceptualization of other types of friendships as valid.
Researchers have also investigated peer interaction in specialized institutions.
Poon (2011) reported on several measures of activities and participation for adolescents
diagnosed with autism spectrum conditions in a special school setting in Singapore. The
study found that adolescents had greater strength in “activities” (i.e., self-care and general
tasks) than “participation” (i.e., interpersonal interaction, community and social life),
possibly due to difficulties in generalization (Poon 2011). Poon (2011) therefore found
that a drawback to a specialized, as opposed to inclusive, program for adolescents is that
skills learned in controlled settings may not be generalized to home and community
79
settings.
Sexuality
In addition to homosocial relationships, adolescence is often associated with
sexual relationships and subjective experiences of sexuality. Herdt suggests the process
of sexuality development begins around 10 years of age (Herdt 1987/2005; McClintock
and Herdt 1996). Previously, popular and scholarly literature failed to acknowledge or
outright denied the sexuality of persons with disabilities (see Tissot 2009 for a review).
However, newer research has confirmed that many persons with autism do in fact desire
sexual and romantic relationships (Hellemans, et al. 2007; Ousley and Mesibov 1991),
and new studies are emerging that focus on sexual development in adolescence
specifically. Much of this literature has come from a service provision and program
development perspective.
Sexuality can be risk for persons with disabilities. Individuals with autism are
both at risk of abuse and risk inflicting abuse on others, therefore increasing the need for
education (Tissot 2009). Parents participating in a focus group on sexuality education in
the US expressed the same concerns and came to the same conclusion that education
could help (Nichols and Blakeley-Smith 2010). Parent reports also indicate more
“inappropriate courting behaviors,” even stalking, among adolescents and adults with
autism, suggesting a need for social skills interventions relating to sexuality (Stokes, et al.
2007).
Melone and Lettick (1983) and Tissot (2009) both describe the development of a
sexual education program for students with autism at specialized, residential schools.
However, Melone and Lettick (1983) were writing in the United States in 1983 and
80
Tissot (2009) in the United Kingdom in 2009. Therefore, although the authors described
similar goals with similar participants, Melone and Lettick (1983) reads more as an
artifact. Tissot (2009) identifies “views of society” as the most challenging obstacle to
providing sexual education to individuals with disabilities. Despite Melone and Lettick’s
(1983) relatively progressive goal of supporting individuals with autism to develop solo
sexuality, the authors discuss their participants in much the same way as Tissot (2009)
criticizes. Melone and Lettick (1983) describe in no uncertain terms why their residents
are prohibited from sexual activity: because it would be dangerous, because the students
would be emotionally or cognitively incapable of relationships, or to a lesser degree
because the students expressed no interest in it. While disinterest would certainly be a
good reason, the authors presume, rather than demonstrate, the incompetence of their
clients, going so far as to suggest that a student with “near-normal sexual feelings”
should not remain at the institution. Melone and Lettick (1983) make clear that “this is
not a program for individuals who can engage in intimate sexual relationships”
(1983:176).
Tissot (2009) criticizes earlier literature for similar assumptions of asexuality.
Tissot’s (2009) school also prohibits paired sexual activity, but by national law rather
than the individual school board. However, Koller (2000) draws on Melone and Lettick
nearly 20 years later in a review of successful sexual education programs. Koller (2000)
also summarizes a similar program in Denmark, developed in the 1990s, which still treats
paired sexuality with reservations, but with openness to supporting it through education
rather than rejecting the development of such feelings as in Melone and Lettick (1983).
Despite their differences, both programs focus on teaching the what, when, and where of
81
sexuality, with a particular focus on the concept of “private” activities. Tissot (2009) calls
this “establishing a sexual identity;” Melone and Lettick describe it as “understanding …
their own sexuality.” Koller’s (2000) review focuses on adolescents’ “self-esteem” and
“healthy self-image.” These goals are all individual and treat sexuality as a component of
an individual identity. The limitations of this approach should be obvious: as many
persons with autism desire sexual relationships (Bagatell 2007; Hellemans, et al. 2007).
The following paragraph describes the few interventions that discuss sociosexual skills,
rather than solo sexuality.
Duncan and Klinger (2010) discuss a social skills intervention program for
adolescents with autism in an outpatient clinic in the US. This program did include
training regarding “Friendships and Dating in High School” but the authors did not
comment on the characteristics of the participants in terms of social functioning or
activities of daily living. However, Nichols and Blakeley-Smith’s (2010) focus group
with parents of children with autism in the US found that regardless of the child’s ability
level, parents expressed a desire for fulfilling relationships as part of their goals for
sexuality development programs. The authors then developed a parent training program
that addressed these goals. These programs directly contrast with the residential programs
in which education did not address sexual relationships. It is worth noting that Tissot
(2009) treats this absence more as a constraint of the setting than the participants.5
Transition to Adulthood
5
The language of the article reflects sympathy for the predicament of the case study in which the problem
behavior was an intimate relationship. E.g., “Eventually Colin was offered an adult placement…. John
found this exceptionally hard…. Unfortunately at the time of this writing, they have not seen each other
since leaving Queensgate School.” Tissot (2009) also notes that the main problem came from John’s
parents distress at the homosexual nature of the relationship specifically, not the sexual intimacy generally.
82
Autism spectrum conditions are generally diagnosed in early childhood, and
research tends to focus on either this life stage or adulthood (specifically the transition to
adulthood). Studies often group adolescents and adults together without differentiating
between the stages. Perhaps this is because it is so difficult to determine when someone
with a developmental disability develops into different life stages. Anthropologists define
adolescence as the period between physical maturity and social maturity defined as “the
assumption of full adult roles.” Many studies of adolescents and adults with autism focus
on outcomes (for a review see Levy and Perry 2011), on how successful the assumption
of roles such as independent living actually is. These studies therefore assume an agebased marker of adulthood, which can be successful or unsuccessful but nevertheless is
adulthood; adulthood is not defined by the assumption of full adult roles but outcomes for
age-defined adults are defined by the extent of these assumptions.
A specific set of studies defines adulthood as post-schooling. Hendricks and
Wehman (2009) provide a review of outcomes that defines adulthood in this way. The
transition out of high school is particularly important for adolescents with autism because
the public school district provides many services that may or may not be replaced by
other services afterwards. For many adolescents in the United States, post-secondary
education is an important additional educational stage. Camerena and Sarigiani (2009)
provide data on interviews with high-functioning adolescents with autism currently in
high school regarding their aspirations and expectations for college.
Developmental disabilities such as autism provide a challenge to the
anthropological definition of adulthood in that people with developmental disabilities do
not necessarily attain “full adult roles” and yet do reach adulthood (for a review of
83
adulthood and aging in autism see Wright et al. 2013). Literature on acquired physical
disability has explored the “erosion of full adult personhood” due to the limitations of
physical disability (Luborsky 1994). A study of women who had ostomy surgery explores
their “personhood realignment” strategies, attempting to reclaim full adult personhood.
The author argues that people do not fully regain it, but gain it in the absence of certain
specific characteristics (Ramirez et al. 2014). Limited work has been done on this process
at all, and especially so in people born with developmental disabilities like autism,
though there are compelling exceptions, particularly Edgerton’s (1971; 1984) compelling
exploration of adults with intellectual disabilities and the way they craft adult roles for
themselves, and newer work (Mehrotra and Vaidya 2008; Olney and Kim 2001). This
dissertation will explore the way various individual and institutional actors in the Italian
context work to provide opportunities for full adult roles to people with autism by
investigating the experiences of youth aged 14-34 in autism-specific contexts.
Conclusion
This chapter has situated my dissertation research within four key literatures: the
anthropology of biomedicine, the anthropology of autism, new biopolitical theory, and
the anthropology of youth. This dissertation derives from and contributes to each of these
literatures. It derives from the anthropology of biomedicine in its recognition of
differences in biomedicines around the world, and its choice of Italy as a fieldsite heavily
influential in the history and development of Western biomedicine, yet distinct from
Austria and the United States where autism was first described. It contributes to the
anthropology of biomedicine by providing a detailed description of the Italian context
and its impact on people with autism and their families. It derives from the anthropology
84
of autism in its choice of autism as a research topic and its respect of the local and
historical particularity of autism and focus on non-deficit focused narratives. It
contributes to the anthropology of autism by providing rich ethnographic data on the
Italian context and internationalizing the conversation. It derives from literature on new
biopolitical theory in its choice of theoretical framework, and contributes to this literature
by operationalization, challenging, and expanding new biopolitical theories. Finally, it
derives from the Anthropology of Youth in its choice of age group and autism-specific
setting. It contributes to the Anthropology of Youth through an investigation of meaning
in the life cycle and the creation of opportunities for full adult roles for people with
developmental disabilities in Italy.
85
Chapter 2: Background
Chapter Overview
This chapter provides background information on autism in Italy, setting the stage
for this research study. It begins with discussion of the history of autism with particular
emphasis on social histories. It then presents a brief historical overview of autism in Italy,
including the history of psychiatry and health and human services more broadly. It then
provides an overview of the management of autism services in Italy during the data
collection period, including scholastic and other institutional paths for persons with
disabilities. This chapter will set the stage for the presentation of data collected from
autism professionals, parents, and people with autism themselves in the following
chapters.
Section 1: History of Autism
In the early decades of the 21st century, autism has received immense and growing
scholarly, professional, and popular attention. Indeed, the history of autism has been
rehearsed so many times that it has truly developed into, as Solomon (2010) aptly notes,
an “origin story.” The story goes something like this: in the 1940s a scientist in the
United States (Leo Kanner) and another in Austria (Hans Asperger) independently
observed two different groups of young people and independently used the term
“autistic” to describe them (Asperger 1944; Kanner 1943). The term derived from Eugen
Bleuler’s (1950) descriptions of schizophrenia and referred to self-absorption (Hacking
2010). However, due to language barriers during and after the Second World War, these
works were not unified until the 1980s, when Lorna Wing translated Asperger’s work
into English (Wing 1981). The syndrome which now bears his name was considered
86
similar enough to Kanner’s syndrome to combine them into the umbrella term “autism
spectrum disorders,” or ASD in the DSM-IV despite the fact that – as Wing (1981)
discusses in her paper – Asperger himself argued that his syndrome and Kanner’s autism
were different. Diagnoses under this umbrella are applied to people, often in childhood,
with impairments in social interaction and communication and restricted, repetitive
interests and behaviors. ASD has long been a “classification in motion,” in Hacking’s
(2010) words. It was called childhood schizophrenia until it was noted that children with
autism did not grow into adults with schizophrenia but rather, adults with autism. For
similar reasons, the term “infantile autism” was abandoned, and it was re-conceptualized
as a pervasive, also in the sense of life-long, developmental disability.
As of this writing, the United States Centers for Disease Control and Prevention’s
most recent reports indicate a prevalence of all autism spectrum conditions at 1 in 68
(Baio 2012). Syntheses of international reports place conservative estimates at 13 per
10,000 for autistic disorder, 21 per 10,000 for PDD-NOS, 2.6 per 10,000 for Asperger’s
Syndrome, and 0.2 per 10,000 for childhood disintegrative disorder, with a best estimate
of 60 per 10,000 for all autism spectrum disorders together (Fombonne 2005; see also
Fombonne 2003). Studies consistently find four to six times more males with autism than
females (Baio 2012; Fombonne 2003; Fombonne 2005), with the ratio even wider among
people with higher levels of support needs (Fombonne 2005).
Epidemiological information on autism in Italy – as in much of the world – is
difficult to find. Arduino and Latoni (2009) review several epidemiological studies from
Piedmont, Marche, Tuscany, and Emilia Romagna from the mid-2000s, with prevalence
rates ranging from 1.3 to 2.5 per 1000 minors, whereas a survey by the Italian Institute
87
for Political and Social Economic studies (EURISPES) found a prevalence of 6-10 cases
of autism in 10,000 births (Meroni 2008). One reason for the difficulty in identifying
prevalence rates in Italy is the history of epidemiology as a field of study, which is rather
new in Italy and very regionalized (Saracci 2011; Saracci, Terracini, and Merletti 2011).
Many Italian sources cite U.S. or international prevalence rates (e.g., Levi 2005).
Those who identify the difference – namely, that Italian rates are so much lower than
those in the U.S. and elsewhere – assign different meaning to them. Some (Arduino and
Latoni 2010; Levi and Bernabei 1997; Meroni 2008) argue that Italy is under-diagnosing
autism, supporting this argument by conducting systematic testing in populations with
other diagnoses (such as intellectual disability) to identify overlooked cases (Arduino and
Latoni 2010). Notably, scholars in the United States have argued that U.S. prevalent rates
are too high, a case of “diagnostic substitution” where people who would previously have
been diagnosed with intellectual disability are now diagnosed with the more desirable
autism label (Rosenberg et al. 2009; Shattuck 2006; Wazana, Bresnahan, and Kline
2007). Others (Dufault et al. 2012) take the differences in prevalence at face value, and
look for causal reasons the Italian prevalence is lower.
Anthropologists and social historians have done much to contextualize this story,
as reviewed in Chapter 1. Notably, the anthropology of biomedicine and other social
studies of science have complicated the very notion of diagnosis and epidemiology,
noting factors such as local biology (Lock and Kaufert 2001), which call into question
whether prevalence rates for presumedly biological conditions that are based on a study
in one location can be assumed to apply globally, and looping effects (Hacking 1995),
which notes that the definition of a category changes as its membership changes, and so
88
on, cyclically (Kirmayer 2006). Verhoeff (2012) questions whether autism can be
considered a “natural kind” or an entity with its own essence at all, reviewing such
conflicts as the “fractionation” of the core symptoms of autism (which do not correlate
with each other in a single person) and the lumpers versus splitters debates in
classification.
The newest step in the history of autism spectrum conditions is their
reclassification in the DSM-5 as a singular “autism spectrum disorder” (ASD) that
encompasses and streamlines the diagnostic criteria for four of the five DSM-IV-TR
disorders, and excludes one, Rett Syndrome, due to its easily pinpointed genetic etiology.
The DSM-5 is nothing if not controversial, and the removal of the term “Asperger’s
Syndrome” is one of the heavily debated issues. While some people with Asperger’s may
get a diagnosis of ASD, research indicates that people previously classified with
Asperger’s may be excluded from the DSM-5 diagnosis of ASD (McPartland, Reichow,
and Volkmar 2012), possibly falling instead under the new “social communication
disorder” (Frazier et al. 2012) but possibly receiving no diagnosis at all. Some critics of
the new system fear that people who do not qualify for a diagnosis might therefore no
longer qualify for services that would be helpful for them.
Although the DSM is an important resource in Italy, the Azienda Sanitaria Locale
(ASL, Local Health Unit) where I conducted my fieldwork relied on the ICD-10 or even
ICD-9 for classification. The use of the ICD-9 has caused difficulties getting a diagnosis
of Asperger’s, because the term is not directly used therein. Much of the work of
Asperger’s advocacy organizations in Italy (e.g., Gruppo Asperger) over the past decade
has been the struggle for recognition and diagnosis. The importance of Asperger’s and
89
diagnostic changes in the DSM-5 is discussed in more detail in Chapter 4.
In Chapter 1, a review of the anthropology of biomedicine revealed that despite
globalized medical research and practices, and despite the popular notion of a
homogenous “Western medicine,” local particularities remain important. The above
history does not include much about Italy. The standard story talks about the United
States and Austria. A closer look reveals an even stronger presence of Austria, as not
only Asperger but also both Kanner and Bettelheim were Austrian born. Indeed, one
Italian anthropologist (Cola 2012) calls autism il male austriaco (the Austrian disease) in
reference to this geographical locus of authority.
There are, however, a few notable moments in the history of autism that are
specifically Italian. One theory of autism that has captured scientific and public attention
in the early 2000s links autism to “mirror neurons,” motor command neurons which fire
when watching someone else’s action (rather than performing it oneself) and therefore
facilitate empathy and imitation (Ramachandran and Oberman 2006). These neurons
were first identified at the University of Parma, Italy, and scientists in Italy and elsewhere
continue to study them and their role in the brains of people with autism (Rizzolatti and
Fabbri-Destro 2010).
Historically, the Italian physician Sante de Sanctis provided, at the turn into the
20th century, one of the earliest descriptions of children who would now be labeled
“autistic” (Feinstein 2010; Levi and Bernabei 1997). De Sanctis has also been called the
“padre della neuropsichiatria infantile italiana” (father of Italian pediatric
neuropsychiatry) (Cimino and Lombardo 2004), which is the primary field that addresses
autism in Italy (Levi and Bernabei 1997). De Sanctis’s role transitions us into the next
90
section, which takes us further back into Italian history to understand the emergence of
this field of neuropsychiatry, as well as the distinct discipline of psychiatry, and the role
of these fields and other actors in managing institutions, deinstitutionalization, and
community mental (and developmental) health care.
Section 2: History of Italian Autism Services
To understand the history of autism in Italy, it is necessary to understand not only
the history of neuropsychiatry, but also the history of institutions and psychiatry. Scholars
have argued that before deinstitutionalization, people with autism in Italy were placed in
mental institutions (Nardocci 2009; Russo, Capararo, and Valtellina 2014). Indeed, this
argument aligns with Eyal and colleagues (2010) argument that the autism label emerged
after global deinstitutionalization, when specific diagnoses became more important due to
the variety of treatments instead of one treatment – institutionalization – for everything.
Autism provides a specific example of a consequence of this change, discussed more
broadly in Chapter 1. However, Eyal and colleagues do not specifically address Italy
despite the widespread popularity of its deinstitutionalization movement among global
professional circles (WHO Working Group on Changing Patterns in Mental Health
1980).
Mental institutions in Italy have a long history. The first national law to address
these institutions was passed in 1904, although Italy has increasingly been
institutionalizing people with mental illness and disabilities from about 1860 to 1960 and
both Church charities and local governments has created various types of services
(Donnelly 1992). Burti (2001) traces Italian psychiatry back to the 1789 S. Bonifacio
Hospital in Florence and the “enlightened principals of moral treatment.” Previously, care
91
for people with mental illness was mostly under the domain of the Church (Burti and
Benson 1996). Indeed, much of Italian healthcare before the mid-20th century was
organized by the Church (Lo Scalzo et al. 2009). The 1904 Law 36 made psychiatric
hospitals a matter of state security, designed to protect society from “dangerous” people.
It only allowed for involuntary admissions (Burti 2001; Donnelly 1992). The 1904 law
remained the most important legislation governing mental institutions into the 1960s,
leading into 1978’s famous Basaglia’s law, mandating deinstitutionalization (Burti and
Benson 1996; Burti 2001; Bollini, Reich, and Muscettola 1988; Crepet and De Plato
1983; Crepet 1990; Jones and Poletti 1986; Santone et al. 2005; Scheper-Hughes and
Lovell 1986).
These reforms were enacted differently in different places. Generally, the law was
enacted more successfully in northern and central regions of Italy than southern. (Bollini,
Reich, and Muscettola 1988; Donnelly 1992).6 Donnelly (1992:96–97) also notes a
division between its implementation in smaller towns versus larger metropolitan areas.
Whereas deinstitutionalization was accomplished most successfully in smaller towns, in
larger metropolitan areas a division emerged between inpatient and outpatient services,
which served largely different clienteles. Section 3 will turn to the management of autism
services specifically following deinstitutionalization in my northern, primarily urban
fieldsite. The remainder of this section looks at the parallel histories of the professional
fields that dealt with institutions, deinstitutionalization, and community mental health
Per Italy’s National Institute for Statistics (Italian National Statistical Institute (Istat) 2013), Italy can be
divided into three geographical regions of “North,” “Centre,” and “South and the Islands.” North includes
Piemonte (Piedmont), Valle d’Aosta, Liguria, Lombardia (Lombardy), Trentino-Alto Adige, Friuli-Venezia
Giulia, Veneto, and Emilia-Romagna. Centre includes Toscana (Tuscany), Lazio, Umbria, and Marche.
South and the Islands includes Abruzzo, Molise, Compania, Basilicata, Puglia, Calabria, Sicilia (Sicily),
and Sardegna.
6
92
care: neuropsychiatry and psychiatry. It then describes the emergence of a “niche” for
autism within Italian medicine and Italian health and human services.
The Italian medical system distinguishes between neuropsychiatry and psychiatry,
neuropsichiatria infantile and psichiatria. Neuropsichiatria infantile (child
neuropsychiatry), abbreviated NPI but sometimes referred to simply as neuropsichiatria
(neuropsychiatry), addresses neurological, psychiatric, and developmental problems in
children under age 18. Psichiatria (psychiatry) treats adults after reaching the age of
majority on their 18th birthday. As such, it is tempting to simply distinguish child and
adult psychiatry. However, neuropsychiatry and psychiatry actually have distinct origins
and practices which make the transition to legal adulthood anything but simple (Nardocci
2009). As the names imply, neuropsychiatry links neurology and psychiatry. Adult
psychiatry, however, does not. Participants – both professionals and parents – often noted
that neuropsychiatrists took a more holistic view of the child, family, and context.
Psychiatrists, on the other hand, were often criticized for not knowing anything about
autism, and for only wanting to prescribe medications. I stress the differences between
psychiatry and neuropsychiatry in Italy both because they were vitally important to my
participants and because they provide the most concrete and relevant example of the
differences in biomedicine between Italy and other Western biomedical contexts where
discussion of psychiatry are very different.
Italian psychiatry has its roots in the early 19th century (Donnelly 1992). Historian
Michael Donnelly (1992) provides a detailed history of Italian psychiatry and the 1978
deinstitutionalization law and the status of its enactment through the early 1990s. He
demonstrates how early Italian psychiatrists designed the field to be “organicist,” “bio-
93
determinist,” and “positivist” – directly opposed to the “spiritualist” approach of the
Church. This decision was both scientific and political, as the Church and other
professional groups had their own stakes in the care of the mentally ill (Donnelly
1992:29). The Italian psychiatric association, in contrast the French, barred psychologists
(Donnelly 1992:29). These were the psychiatrists working in mental institutions through
the 1960s. This approach located the origin of psychiatric illness firmly in the biological,
leaving no room for either psychoanalytic or social approaches to understanding or
treating mental illness.
In the 1960s, a younger generation of psychiatrists emerged, most prominently
Basaglia. These psychiatrists aligned themselves with phenomenology and existential
psychiatry, which Donnelly (1992:43) argues may have been the only real alternative to
the positivism and bio-determinism of psychiatry at the time in Italy. It was within this
framework that Basaglia and his colleagues conducted their pilot experiments in Trieste
and elsewhere, gradually empowering patients, closing institutions, and “socializing”
psychiatry. They crystallized their ideas into the ideology of Psichiatra Democratica
(Democratic Psychiatry). These efforts led to the passage of “Basaglia’s Law,” the 1978
Law 180. Democratic Psychiatry derived in part from a Sartrean philosophy, seeing the
“mental patient” construct as a manifestation of contradictions in society and the asylum
as a means of removing these contradictions from view, maintaining the illusion of order.
In this approach:
[W]hat it requires is not sympathy for the mentally disordered or
acknowledgement that they have been badly treated, degraded, abandoned. The
point is that the category ‘mental patient’ itself should not exist; the indignation
arises not against the fate of actual suffering people, but against the generally
mechanism of ‘splitting’, which is the Manichean fault of existence. (Donnelly
1992:54)
94
The conflict between positivist and interpretivist psychiatry did not involve the sort of
speculating on the “reality” of mental illness as in the UK and USA; there was no conflict
between medical and social models of psychiatric disability nor influence from outside
professions like social work (Donnelly 1992:59). The ideology of Democratic Psychiatry
aligned closely with the students’ and workers’ movements of the 1960s – these patients
were emarginati (marginalized) just as students, workers, immigrants, displaced persons,
and so on – and therefore gained support from activists in these sectors as well (Donnelly
1992; Scheper-Hughes and Lovell 1986). These approaches later gave way to
psychodynamic and psychoanalytic therapies, which previously did not have much of a
foothold in Italy due to the psychiatric association’s strong rejection of non-organicist
approaches. Feinstein (2010) argues these approaches were more welcome in the northern
regions closer to France.
Most people with autism are diagnosed in early childhood, and it is the field and
the administrative units of neuropsichiatria infantile, childhood psychiatry, that governs
the care of children with autism. The website of the Società Italiana di Neuropsichiatria
dell’Infanzia e dell’Adolescenza (SINPIA –Italian Society for Child and Adolescent
Neuropsychiatry) explains that they field addresses four main areas of child and
adolescent health: neurology, neuropsychology, psychiatry, and rehabilitation. Per their
website (Società Italian di Neuropsichiatria dell’Infanzia e dell’Adolescenza (SINPIA)
n.d.), the conditions and disorders they address can be divided as follows, with their text
in the first column and my translations in the second:
95
Figure 1: Domains of Child Neuropsychiatry
Neurologia
epilessie, paralisi cerebrali infantili,
distrofie ed altre malattie neuromuscolari,
distonie, cefalee, malattie
neurometaboliche, sindromi genetiche,
tumori, encefaliti, etc. [sic]
Neuropsicologia
insufficienza mentale, disturbi specifici
dell’apprendimento (dislessie,
disortografie, discalculie), disturbi del
linguaggio (disfagie, balbuzie, ritardi di
linguaggio ecc)
Psichiatria
Autismo, psicosi, depressione, anoressia,
bulimia e altri disturbi del
comportamento alimentare, disturbi
ossessivo-compulsivi, disturbi del sonno,
disturbi d'ansia, fobie, malattie
psicosomatiche, disturbi relazionali,
disturbi di personalità, disturbi del
comportamento in genere, inclusa
l’iperattività con deficit di attenzione, etc;
enuresi ed encopresi; maltrattamento e
violenze sui minori e loro conseguenze
Riabilitazione
Abilitazione e riabilitazione dei disturbi
neuromotori, cognitivi, neuropsicologici e
psichiatrici
Neurology
Epilepsy, cerebral palsy, muscular
dystrophy and other neuromuscular
disease, dystonia, migraines,
neurometabolic diseases, genetic
conditions, tumors, encephalitis, etc.
Neuropsychology
Intellectual disability, specific learning
difficulties (dyslexia, dysorthography,
dyscalculia), language impairments
(dysphagia, stuttering, language delay,
etc.)
Psychiatry
Autism, psycosis, depression, anorexia,
bulimia and other eating disorders,
obsessive-compulsive disorder, sleep
disturbances, anxiety disorders, phobias,
psychosomatic illnesses, relational
disorders, personality disorders, general
behavioral disorders including attention
deficit hyperactivity disorder etc., enuresis
and encopresis, mistreatment of and
violence against minors and their
consequences
Rehabilitation
Abilitation and Rehabilitation of
neuromotor, cognitive,
neuropsychological, and psychiatric
disorders
Autism is placed within the category of “psychiatry,” and most people with autism enter
into the care of psychiatrists when they reach 18. Neuropsychiatry serves minors,
whereas psychiatry serves adults. The transition from the care of neuropsychiatry to
psychiatry can be rather abrupt and leave young adults with autism in a “black hole”
(Molteni 2011:106). An epidemiological study has shown that there are more individuals
under 14 then over 14 with an autism diagnosis due to differing diagnostic practices in
these fields and the idea that autism is only “infantile,” demonstrated that adult psychiatry
96
does not necessarily recognize or address autism in adults who had been served by
neuropsychiatry in childhood (described in Moderato 2008:46); see also Arduino and
Latoni 2009; Molteni 2011:36–37).
While childhood neuropsychiatry is indeed the counterpart to adult psychiatry,
more than just the age group served differentiates these fields. If Italian psychiatry has its
roots in Basaglia and the ideology of democratic psychiatry, neuropsychiatry has its roots
in Sante de Sanctis (psychiatrist), Giuseppe F. Montesano (psychopedagogue), and Maria
Montessori (pedagogue, creator of the “Montessori school” model used also in the United
States) and therefore bridges psychiatry and pedagogy (Bracci 2003; Migone 2014).
Neuropsychiatry emerged from the work of psychiatrists, psychologists, and pedagogues,
initially as a subfield of psychiatry itself (Migone 2014; Storia » Società Italiana Di
Psichiatria n.d.). However, the field became so distinct that ultimately neuropsychiatry
created an independent professional association, SINPIA, separate from the psychiatric
association, SIP (Storia » Società Italiana Di Psichiatria n.d.).
Child neuropsychiatry – in the sense of the science of neurology and/or psychiatry
in childhood and adolescence – has its roots in the 1800s as part of a broader movement
across Europe and the United States (Bracci 2003) alongside greater attention to children
and childhood (see Nadesan 2005). Italian neuropsychiatry first emerged at the turn of the
20th century. Psychologist and psychiatrist Sante de Sanctis, who provided one of the first
descriptions of children that some now recognize as having autism (Levi and Bernabei
1997), has also been called the father of Italian neuropsychiatry. De Sanctis penned the
book Neuropsichiatria infantile (1925) that gave the field its name, describing his
observations of children then known as frenastenici, a term used to refer to cognitive
97
disabilities and distinguished from mental illness (Cimino and Lombardo 2004:15).
Similarly to the organicist psychiatrists Donnelly describes at this time, De Sanctis never
joined the psychoanalytic movement in Italy, although reportedly he was often asked
(Cimino and Lombardo 2004:14). Even into the late 20th century, some neuropsychiatric
units were under the same scrutiny from democratic psychiatry as institutions (Bracci
2003:15). Just as psychiatry took an interpretive turn in the 1960s and 70s with
deinstitutionalization, so did neuropsychiatry. Fiorani (2011) traces this turn to the period
immediately following World War II, in which several Italian psychiatrists were trained
abroad in various countries and returned to Italy with these influences. Fiorani (2011)
cautions against a reading of strict opposition between this new generation and the
previous (like the general story of Basaglia’s generation in the history of psychiatry), but
stresses the connections between this emerging field of child neuropsychiatry and the
precursors of the early 20th century.
Giovanni Bollea has also been called the father of neuropsychiatry for his role in
establishing the professional after World War II (Fiorani 2011; Migone 2014). Bollea
(1960, cited in Fiorani 2011) explained his legacy as such:
Sessanta anni fa, nel 1900, Sante De Sanctis apriva in Roma il primo
Ambulatorio di neuropsichiatria infantile; dopo trent’anni, nel 1930, Cerletti a
Genova e De Sanctis a Roma, inauguravano i primi due Reparti infantili
universitari. Dopo altri trent’anni circa la Gazzetta Ufficiale dell’8 ottobre 1959
sanciva la richiesta della Facoltà Medica di Roma di mettere tra le materie di
insegnamento universitario la neuropsichiatria infantile e qualche settimana più
avanti il Consiglio di Facoltà, su proposta del mio direttore prof. Gozzano,
affidava a me il primo incarico italiano di questa nuova materia.
Sixty years ago, in 1900, Sante De Sanctis opened in Rome the first ambulatory
service for child neuropsychiatry; after 30 years, in 1930, Cerletti in Genoa and
De Sanctis in Rome started the first two university childhood departments. After
nearly another 30 years the Official Gazette of October 8, 1959 sanctioned the
request of the Medical Faculty of Rome to have child neuropsychiatry among the
maters taught at university and some weeks layer the Faculty Council, at the
98
suggestion of my supervisor Professor Gozzano, entrusted to me the first Italian
post for this new material.
Fiorani (2011) traces the emergence of child neuropsychiatry to a broader climate
of increased attention to childhood and human development post-World War II. Fiorani
(2011) also traces the use of the term neuropsychiatry (as opposed to “pedopsychiatry”
which he says is used all’estero, abroad) to Bollea’s desire to honor the distinctly Italian
tradition and legacy following Sante de Sanctis.
Migone (2014) argues that child neuropsychiatry has taken more influence from
French psychoanalytic schools, whereas adult psychiatry has taken more influence from
first German and then Anglo-Saxon psychiatries. Migone further explains:
La psichiatria dell'infanzia e dell'adolescenza in Italia quindi è stata
caratterizzata da un ridotto uso di farmaci (se paragonata agli Stati Uniti), e da
un diffuso uso delle psicoterapie ad orientamento dinamico, individuali e
familiari (dalla metà degli anni 1970 si diffonderà la terapia sistemica).
Estremamente importante è l'attenzione alla famiglia e all'ambiente sociale per
comprendere il caso clinico durante l'età evolutiva.
Child and adolescent psychiatry in Italy is therefore characterized by a reduced
use of medications (if compared to the United States), and by a diffuse use of
dynamic psychotherapy, both individual and family therapy (from the mid-1970s
systemic therapy spread). The attention to the family and the social environment
is extremely important for understand the clinical case during the developmental
years.
The field can thusly be distinguished from psychiatry. It is known for being
multidisciplinary and working in equipe, teams of psychiatrists, psychologists, social
workers and so on which Bracci (2003:3) calls “l’elemento metodologico più importante
e caratteristico” (the methodological element most important and characteristic). It
incorporates psychoanalysis, psychotherapy, dynamic psychology, psychological testing,
social interventions, and so on (Fiorani 2011). Neuropsychiatry incorporates
psychologists and psychological approaches, but the history of psychology distances it
99
from psychiatry. Particularly in the fascist and post-war eras, psychology was considered
a part of philosophy and not clinical practice (Fiorani 2011). Neuropsychiatry was also
influenced by philosophy through Maria Montessori as she left child psychiatry for the
field of philosophy and strict pedagogy (Migone 2014).
This integrated nature of childhood neuropsychiatry creates a very different
environment than the singular field of psychiatry for adults, or indeed the singular field of
psychiatry for children and adolescents. As Bracci (2003:19) explains:
La debolezza dei Servizi di NPI è più evidente laddove si prendano in
considerazione patologie psichiatriche acute o che necessitano di un ricovero
ospedaliero urgente. Raramente questi casi trovano collocazione presso le
Strutture di N.P.I. e ciò per vari motivi: inidoneità strutturali, impedimenti
logistici, scelte gestionali, confessionali per citarne solo i più comuni.
La gestione delle acuzie psichiatriche, soprattutto in età prepuberale e in
adolescenza, così come le gravi crisi depressive con rischio suicidiario o i gravi
l’alimentazione, è ampiamente demandata alle strutture psichiatriche.
The weakness of Child Neuropsychiatry Services is most evident wherever one
considers acute psychiatric pathologies or things that require urgent
hospitalization. Rarely in these cases does one find arrangements in Child
Neuropsychiatry Services and this is for various reasons: unideal structures,
logistical difficulties, managerial choices, and sectarian reasons to name only the
most common.
The management of acute psychiatric needs, especially in prepubescents and
adolescents, such as serious episodes of depression with suicide risk or serious
eating disorders, is largely deferred to psychiatric structures.
A full comparison of the histories and practices of neuropsychiatry and psychiatry is a
dissertation unto itself, but to set the stage for the ethnographic data to follow I need to
stress that my participants identified a sharp difference between childhood
neuropsychiatry and adult psychiatry in practice, which I argue emerges somewhat from
these different historical trajectories.
These histories heave led to the current situation in Italy. Parents, professionals,
and youth I interviewed often argued that there is little public awareness of autism in
100
Italy. Autism is certainly not unknown in Italy, as the Italian language newsfeed I set up
during my field work made clear to me, but the perspectives presented in this dissertation
are those of people who are much more heavily invested in and aware of autism than the
typical person. Autism in Italy has not, for example, produced a genre of “autism fiction”
as it has in Anglophone settings (Hacking 2010; Ravanelli 2013). The remainder of this
dissertation describes the emergence of a niche for autism and the way autism is
conceptualized by professionals, parents, and youth operating within the niche. In order
to set the stage, the final section of this chapter turns to the management of Italian autism
services in the framework of generally disability services in Italy.
Section 3: Management of Italian Autism Services
The deinstitutionalization movement led to policies of social and educational
integration. In place of the older institutions, people with disabilities are served by a
highly regionalized national health care system in which the local health unit assigns
individuals with disabilities a level of invalidità civile (colloquially called simple
invalidità, invalidity) that grants rights to certain financial benefits (Crialesi et al. 2007).
This section discusses the current management of autism services in my fieldsite. It
begins with a discussion of the function of the Azienda Sanità Locale (ASL, Local Health
Units), the rights granted to people with disabilities and their families under Italian Law
104, and the range of professionals and paraprofessionals involved in this work. It then
discusses educational services in Italy, explaining the workings of the Italian school
system in general, the rights granted to individuals with disabilities, and the educational
figures youth with disabilities often encounter. Finally, it addresses settings outside of
schools in which people with disabilities can receive services. In total, this section sets
101
the stage for readers to understand the environment in which my participants operated,
preparing readers to delve into the ethnographic data presented in the rest of this
dissertation.
The deinstitionalization policies of Law 180 were rolled into Law 833, which
created the Servizio Sanitario Nazionale, the Italian National Health Service (Cosmacini
2011; Donnelly 1992). This service provides free, universal coverage for health care (Lo
Scalzo et al. 2009). In 1999, Law 229 set the standards for the Servizio Sanitario
Nazionale to support health for all humans regardless of economic circumstances
(Cosmacini 2011). The administration of health services is regionalized through aziende
sanitarie locali (ASL), which Lo Scalzo and colleages (2009) translate as “local health
enterprises.” These ASL coordinate public health services and specialist ambulatory
services, which they either provide themselves or accredit public or private facilities to
provide (Lo Scalzo et al. 2009). Dedicated multidisciplinary teams within ASLs
coordinate mental and developmental health services (Lo Scalzo et al. 2009). However,
many Italians (including many of my participants) pay privately for services due to long
waitlists and dissatisfaction (Lo Scalzo et al. 2009).
For individuals with disabilities, ASLs assign a level of invalidità civile (civil
invalidity/disability). Law 118 of 1971 defines invalidità civile as “the right to receive
financial benefits when the disability is a consequence of biological damage with no
reference to general self sufficiency,” in the words of Crialesi and colleagues (280). This
invalidità is assigned on a level from 0-100 and grants various supports. Someone with a
level over 33 can access work support programs. More importantly to my participants,
someone with a level over 75 can receive pension benefits which help address costs of
102
living (though parents often still end up paying out of pocket) (Crialesi et al. 2007:280).
Financial benefits of pensione (pension) and indennità di accompagnamento
(accompaniment allowance) are paid by the Istituto Nazionale della Previdenza Sociale
(National Institute of Social Security).
The most important healthcare law for my participants was Law 104, passed in
1992. This law established a specific health care program for ASLs to deliver
rehabilitative care to people with disabilities (Lo Scalzo et al. 2009). This law established
the rights of people with disabilities, formalizing older provisions and setting the ground
for future ones (Crialesi et al. 2007:274). Subsequent legislation further specified rights
for people with disabilities and their parents. In 2000, Law 388 established several such
regulations, including leaves up absence from work for parents, additional pension for
workers with disability levels over 74%, employment quotas for businesses to hire people
with disabilities, specific assistance at schools, equal access to public transportation,
special parking reservation, among others (Crialesi et al. 2007:275–6).
Several professionals and paraprofessionals are involved in the execution of these
services. Professionals in my fieldsite considered equipe, teams, to be the gold standard
for coordinating services for people with autism. These equipe consisted of several types
of professional figures, including neuropsychiatrists, psychiatrists, psychologists,
professional educatori, rehabilitative therapists, professional nurses, ausiliari socioassistenziali, social workers, pedagologists, supervisors, and responsible officers of the
service itself. They may also include idiosyncratic team members, such as gym teachers,
depending on the needs of the person and service (Di Furia and Montanari 2003:197).
These professionals work in a variety of contexts in schools, hospitals, social services,
103
and the community. The rest of this section addresses important institutional contexts for
people with autism.
The Italian school system is divided into several levels, diagrammed in Figure 1. I
have created this figure using the terms of categories that were important to my
participants,7 and refer therefore to asilo nido, scuola materna, scuola elementare, scuola
media, scuola secondaria, and the university levels of laurea and laurea magistriale.
These terms will appear through the following chapters of data presentation. In the next
several paragraphs, I will draw from secondary source material to explain in more detail
what these terms mean, and what alternate more technical) terms exist. Note that
schooling is obligatory and compulsory at the elementary school and middle school
levels, comprising 8 years from approximately ages 6-14 (UNESCO-IBE 2010). In 2006,
the age for the end of compulsory schooling was changed to 16, therefore extending into
the first two years of secondary school (UNESCO-IBE 2010).
Figure 2: The Italian Educational System
A similar and useful figure can be found in UNESCO, p. 14. However, UNESCO’s figure does not
include any schooling before scuola maternal, whereas the vast majority of my participants attended asilo
nido. Furthermore, UNESCO’s figure only uses English terms, and includes more information than is
relevant for our discussion.
7
104
Up to 3 Years
Asilo Nido
(Nursery School)
3 mos-3 yrs old
3 Years
Scuola Materna
(Preschool)
3-6 yrs old
Scuola
Elementare
(Elementary
School)
5 Years
6-11 yrs old
3 Years
Scuola Media
(Middle School)
11-14 yrs old
5 Years
14-19 yrs old
3 Years
19-22 yrs old
2 Years
22-24 yrs old
Liceo
(Lyceum/High
School)
Istituto Tecnico
(Technical
Institute)
Laurea (Bachelor)
Istituto
Professionale
(Vocational
Institute)
IFTS/ITS
Laurea
Magistrale
(Master)
Asilo nido can be glossed as nursery school and is available to children age three months
to three years. The words literally mean shelter or haven (asilo) and nest (nido). Although
early childcare services in Italy have a longer history stretching back to fascism and
105
before then to Church charity, asili nido as they are known today began with the passage
of Law 1044 in 1971, which provided state funding to set up municipal (comunale) asili
nido available to all children. However, asili nido have limited space. Attendance is
neither compulsory nor guaranteed. Attendance is also not free, although it is heavily
subsidized. Asili nido are not technically part of the educational system, but administered
by the Ministry of Health. They are therefore more custodial than educational institutions
– at least as imaged by the Law 1044 – but there has been a trend since 1971 towards
more a more educational role. Children with disabilities are given top priority for
admission (though this could be both for custodial and educational reasons) and staff are
known as educatori, derived from the same root word as education (Corsaro and Emiliani
2014). Asili nido can be either public or private. Many of my participants specified that
their children attending asili nido run by le Suore (the Nuns).
Scuola Materna translates to maternal school and can be glossed as preschool.
Most of these schools are public. In 1968, Law 444 introduced state run scuole materne,
and in 2003 Law 53 and in 2004 Legislative Decree 59 integrated scuole materne into the
education system as part of scuola dell’infanzia (infancy school/pre-primary school)
(UNESCO-IBE 2010). These schools are not obligatory, but they are free so long as they
are public (either a state school or a school that has been recognized by the state after
meeting state requirements) (UNESCO). Some students with disability certificates take
an extra year of scuola materna, colloquially called the anno di saldatura (welding year –
a year that welds a bridge between pre-primary school and primary school) (Leonardi
2008:38).
Scuola elementare, elementary school, is obligatory and free. It lasts five years.
106
These years are referred to as prima elementare, seconda elementare and so on through
quinta elementare, therefore first grade, second grade, and so on through fifth grade.
Scuola media, middle school, is also obligatory and free. It is part of the primary
education cycle. It is more properly called scuola secondaria di primo grado, but this
formal change is very recent, dating from a 2004 law (Treccani, n.d.), and many
documents in fact still refer to it as scuola secondaria di primo grado (ex scuola media) –
Lower Secondary School (Ex Middle School). Many participants used the term scuola
media. Scuola media is comprised of three years – prima media, seconda media, and
terza media (first, second, and third grade). The third year of middle school ends with
state exams which grant a diploma and access to upper secondary school.
Scuola secondaria, secondary school, is compulsory only for the first two years. It
is most properly known as scuola secondaria di secondo grado (upper secondary school).
These schools generally take five years. The first two years are compulsory and free. The
remaining three years cost. Secondary schooling divides students into different academic
or vocational paths: liceo (Lyceum), istituto tecnico (technical institute), and istituto
professionale (professional institute). There are several types of liceo, which are currently
in flux following reforms starting in the 2010-2011 school year (UNESCO-IBE 2010).
Under the reform there are six types of liceo. Liceo classico (classic lyceum) is dedicated
to the humanities and ancient languages, liceo scientifico (scientific lyceum) to the
natural sciences, liceo linguistico (linguistic lyceum) to modern foreign language, liceo
artistico (artistic lyceum) to the fine arts, liceo delle scienze umane (human sciences
lyceum) to the social sciences, and liceo muscicale e coeutico (music and dance lyceum)
to music and dance. After five years, students take state exams which grant a diploma di
107
istruzione liceale (lyceum diploma) (Leone 2010a). There have been various
configurations of liceo over past years, but the gist remains that these schools last five
years and focus on the arts, sciences, and humanities.
There are two types of istituto tecnico – economico (economics) and tecnologico
(technology). Economic schools focus on administration, finance, marketing, and
tourism. Technology schools address a range of areas including mechanics, computer
science, and agriculture. At the end of five years, students take the state exams and
receive the diploma di istruzione tecnica (technical institute diploma) (Leone 2010b).
There are also two types of istituto professionale – servizi (service) and industria ed
artigianato (industry and craft). Service schools include agricultural and rural
development services, socio-sanitary services, hospitality services, and commercial
services. Industry and craft schools include production schools and maintenance and
technical assistance schools. At the end of five years, students receive the diploma di
istruzione professionale (professional institute diploma). Studies can also receive
qualifiche (qualifications) at the end of the third year and diploma professionali
(professional diplomas) at the end of the fourth year. Students at these schools do not
necessarily take the state exams, though they may (Leone 2010c; UNESCO-IBE 2010).
The above discussion makes much reference to diplomas, called diploma in Italian or
colloquially maturità. Students who pass the state exams receive diplomas. However, not
all students with disabilities do sit for the exams or receive diplomas. While students with
disabilities do receive diplomas at the end of the scuola media as long as they fulfil their
individualized educational plans (IEPs, in Italian, PEI), at the end of secondary school
there are two paths. Some students follow a path that can grant them a regular diploma,
108
whereas others follow a path that grants an attestato di frequenza (certificate of
attendance) at the end of the academic cycle (Ministero dell’Istruzione, dell’Università e
della Ricerca [MIUR] 2012).
Post-secondary education in Italy is split similarly to secondary education.
Following the 1999 Bologna Reform, Italian universities follow a 3+2 system similar to
many other places in Europe. In most fields, the first three years grant a laurea similar to
a U.S. bachelor’s degree, and a second two years grant a laurea magistrale similar to a
U.S. master’s degree (UNESCO-IBE 2010). Instead of the university path, students may
also enroll in various types of vocational post-secondary education, such as Istituti
Tecnici Superiori (Higher Technical Institutes).8 Participants reported that there are even
some post-secondary vocational programs specific for people on the autism spectrum.
In 1977, Italy passed Law 517 which abolished “the widespread use of special schools”
(Cornoldi et al. 1998). In 1992, Italy enacted Law 104, which provides for the education
of all students (including students with disability) in the same public school system –
including post-secondary education. This inclusive education system is called
Integrazione Scolastica (Educational Integration). Educational integration coincided with
deinstitutionalization in Italy and reflects broader national goals of social integration
(Canevaro and de Anna 2010). English language literature has identified the Italian
educational system as a model for inclusive education (Begeny and Martens 2007).
Although the term integrazione is often glossed as inclusion, D’Alessio (2008:57–58)
explains that they are not exactly synonymous:
Inclusive education and integrazione scolastica present many similarities as they
are both dynamic processes contributing to the realisation of a more democratic
8
See http://www.indire.it/its/
109
society. Integration, however, is usually used to refer to the education of disabled
students while inclusive education is concerned with all pupils. Despite the scarce
literature about inclusive education in Italy, inclusion seems to be intended as a
more radical and socio-political project that engages society as a whole and
considers integration as just part of its project (Barberio, 2002). Nevertheless,
previous Italian literature often seems to mistake inclusive education as a
synonym of integration.
Although this dissertation does not focus on schooling, the school environment
remains important to parents and youth with autism even after they have graduated or
otherwise left school. Chapter 5 in particular will address parents’ experiences with
school, which touch upon the particularities of Italian integration.
Several professional and paraprofessional figures are important to enacting
integrazione scolastica. First are the regular classroom teachers. Teachers whose
classrooms have a student with a disability are capped at 20 students, and usually there is
no more than one student with a disability in the class (Cornoldi et al. 1998). To help the
class run smoothly, the classroom receives a certain number of hours a week of assistance
from an insegnante di sostegno (support teacher) per student with a disability (Cornoldi
et al. 1998). In theory, the support teacher serves as an additional resource in the
classroom for all students, and facilitates integration. In practice, support teachers often
serve more as aides to the specific student (D’Alessio 2008). Students with disabilities
may also interact with another educational figure whose role is, in fact, to serve as this
sort of individual aide – the educatore (educator or tutor, from here on out referred to in
the Italian). ISTAT uses the terms assistante educativo culturale (cultural education
assistant) and assistente ad personam (personal assistant) to refer to professionals who
help students one on one with issues of personal autonomy (ISTAT 2014), but my
participants talked primarily about educatore.
110
Though the Italian system has been heralded as a model of inclusion, it is not
without problems. Studies indicate that teachers are supportive of inclusion, but do not
feel that they receive enough support to implement it practically (Cornoldi et al. 1998).
Researchers have also addressed specific recommendations for including students with
autism in integrazione scolastica, including using un approccio ecologico (an ecological
approach) (Farci 2005) and the implementation of the TEACCH approach (Panerai et al.
2009).
Though the school environment is overwhelmingly important for Italian youth,
including youth with disabilities, it is by no means the only educational or institutional
setting that impacts their lives. The rest of this section considers these other institutional
contexts, outlining the “institutional path” of someone with a disability certificate in Italy.
Crialesi and colleagues (2007) identify 19 types of services available to people with
disabilities in Italy: comunità socio educative per minori, comunità socio-riabilitativa per
minori, comunità alloggio per minori, comunità familiare per minori, altre strutture per
minori, Residenza Sanitaria Assistenziale, istituti per minori, Private Psychiatric
Inpatient Facilities, Comunità socio-riabilitativa per adulti, Non-Hospital Residential
Facilities, Comunità familiare per adulti, Residenza assistenziale per autosufficienti,
Residenza socio-sanitaria, altre strutture per adulti, comunità alloggio per adulti,
comunità socio-riabilitativa per anziana, comunità alloggio per anziana, and Residenza
Sanitaria Assistenziale. My discussion herein addresses only those types of services that
were commonly mentioned by and important to my participants.
111
Figure 3: The Institutional Path of Someone with a Disability Certificate in Italy
• Early Intervention
Pre-SchoolAge Minors
School-Age
Minors
Younger
Adults - Day
Younger
Adults Residential
• Assistenza Domiciliare
• Special Educational Programs
• Comunità Socio-Educativa/Comunità Familiare
•
•
•
•
Centro Aggregazione Disabili
Centro Formazione Autonomia
Centro Socio-Educativo
Centro Diurno Disabili
• Comunità Protetta/Appartamento Protetto
• Communità Alloggio Socio Sanitaria
• Residenza Sanitario Assistenziali per Disabili
• Residenza Sanitario Assistenziale per Anziani
Older Adults
Many Italians with autism participate in some sort of early intervention program
before the age of compulsory schooling (age 6). Common interventions include ABA,
TEACCH, logopedia (speech therapy), psicomotricità (psychomoro therapy) and
psicoterapia (psychotherapy). These interventions continue as children get older, but are
112
most common among children under 8 years old (Fondazione Censis 2012).
Several services exist for school-aged children outside of those described above.
Children may have assistenza domiciliare (home help), in which an educatore comes to
their own home to provide educational interventions or therapies, facilitate social
activities, go on outings, and so on. This type of assistance is not necessarily limited by
age, and younger or older individuals with disabilities may also access the service
colloquially known as il domiciliare or by its acronym, ADH (assistenza domiciliare
handicap – home help for people with disabilities). In addition to support teachers and
educatore at school, some schools use specialized programs targeted at helping children
with autism, particularly those using the TEACCH model (Panerai et al. 2009). Finally,
there are also some residential services for minors. Comunità Socio-educative per minori
(socio-educative communities for cildren) are for children ages 6-18, and typically serve
children ages 15-17 (Crialesi 284). These services are residential and also provide
educational support. Comunità Familiare per minori are similar. There are also other
types of residences for children but they don’t generally serve minors with intellectual
and developmental disabilities.
There are four main types of day centers that serve adults with disabilities,
sometimes including late adolescents generally starting no younger than 16. Centri
Aggregazioni Disabili (Centers of Aggregation for People with Disabilities) provide
places for adults with disabilities to get together and socialize, while also promoting
autonomy and providing individualized guidance for individuals with disabilities to
navigate the broader community and integrate into its social fabric. Servizi di Formazione
all’Autonomia (Services for Autonomy Training) provide people with disabilities training
113
aimed at social and workplace inclusion, and limit attendance to five years. Centri SocioEducativi (Socio-Educative Centers) provide a place for individuals with disabilities and
higher levels of support needs to go during the weekdays and provide a range of
activities. Centri Diurni Disabili (Day Centers for People with Disabilities) provide
similar services from roughly 9AM to 4PM weekdays, but for people with even higher
levels of support needs (Servizi per Disabili n.d.). My participants explained that Centri
diurni disabili had more staff than centri socio-educativi based on the staff:attendee
support ratio specified by ASL, operationalizing this higher level of support need. Of all
these day services, I only knew of CDD that were autism specific during my time in the
field; services for people with lower levels of support need were not autism-specific.
However, as we will see in the following chapters, there were autism specific social and
recreational activity groups that were not necessarily municipal institutions like those
discussed here.
In addition to day services for adults with disabilities, there are also several types
of residential services. Comunità Protette (sheltered communities) are a type of
“assistance care” for people with disabilities with lower levels of support needs, generally
those who need more healthcare assistance than social assistance in tasks of living
(Crialesi 283). Crialesi and colleagues differentiate comunità protette from residences
that involve more social care, variously named casa alloggio, casa famiglia, or
appartamenti autonomi. These communities are for people with higher levels of support
needs. The names and specific divisions between these types of services vary regionally.
My participants talked about comunità protette (protected communities) and
appartamenti protetti (protected apartments), small apartments which served people with
114
relatively low levels of support needs but offered some staffing during part of the day to
help people with disabilities navigate independent living. They also talked about
Comunità Alloggio or Comunità Alloggio Socio-Sanitario (CSS) (Community Housing or
Socio-Sanitary Community Housing). These slightly larger apartments (sometimes up to
10 residents) have full-time staff who provide more services to residents with disabilities
and coordinate other vocational, educational, and social activities in the surrounding
community. Participants also talked about Residenze Sanitario Assistenziali per Disabili
(Assisted Living for People with Disabilities), which served a larger number of
individuals with disabilities who needed a lot of socio-sanitary care in activities of daily
living (Servizi per Disabili n.d.). I knew of both CSS and RSD that were autism-specific,
as well as several short term residential opportunities for weekends or short weeks away
to practice residential living skills.
Adults ages 65 and older with disabilities attend various types of facilities for the
elderly including comunità socio-riabilitativa per anziani and comunità alloggio per
anziani, comunità familiare per anziani, residenza assistenziale per autosufficienti, and
residenza socio-sanitaria. (Crialesi et al. 2007). Adults with disabilities whose needs
resembled those of the elderly may attend Residenze Sanitario Assistenziali per Anziani
(Assisted Living for the Elderly) even at younger ages. Others may continue to attend the
same residences as when they were younger adults, listed above. (Servizi per Disabili
n.d.).
People with autism encounter several different types of professionals in these
settings. Psychologists and psychiatrists work at or with these services. As in school
settings, educatori are extremely important. They work in day centers and residential
115
centers as well in home help situations. Day and residential centers may also employ staff
who attend to the sanitary and health needs of the structure and individuals and have a
range of specific certifications allow some variation in what they can and cannot do:
Ausiliario Socio Assistenziale, Operatore Socio Sanitario, and Assistente Sociosanitaria.
While in theory educatori provide educational services and these other staff provide
sanitary and assistive services, many staff in both roles frequently stressed to me that in
practice everyone works together on their common goals with people with disabilities.
Most of these services are not autism specific. However, a niche for autism is emerging.
This dissertation takes place within that niche, exploring autism-specific services for
adolescents and youth on the autism spectrum. The settings in which I conducted
participant observation were day centers, residential centers, and social/recreational
activity groups. The rest of this dissertation provides an experience-near account of these
settings, drawing also on interviews with autism professionals, parents of attendees with
autism, and attendees with autism themselves. The following chapter describes research
design and methods, and describes in more detail the specific services in which I
researched.
116
Chapter 3: Research Design
Section 1: Conceptual Framework
How is autism conceptualized in Italy? How do people defined as autistici and
their families define, manage, and experience that label? How does new biopolitical
theory apply, or not, to social organization around autism in Italy? As described in
Chapter 1, these questions emerge from meaning-centered traditions in anthropology and
therefore require a meaning-centered research design. This study of autism-specific
settings in a region of northern Italy seeks to answer these questions while generating a
thick description of the lived experiences of key stakeholders affected by autism: autism
professionals, parents whose children have autism, and people with autism themselves.
The present study takes an interpretivist framework. Because I ask how autism is
defined, I do not begin with a pre-set definition. I did not perform ADOS or similar
evaluations on my participants to “confirm” their diagnosis before moving forward with
the interview. Instead, I operationalized autism by asking the staff to identify participants
with autism whose parents and/or themselves would be eligible for interviews. I did not
exclude individuals who “self-diagnosed” themselves with an autism spectrum condition,
that is, did not have an official diagnosis from a licensed practitioner. This phenomenon
has been much discussed in the literature (e.g., Cola 2012; Hacking 2010; Kapp et al.
2012), particularly in the case of Asperger's Syndrome. Excluding such individuals as not
“really” having autism imposes, rather than explores, the definition. As will be seen in the
following chapters, these decisions allowed me to focus my interviews on understanding
why and what that term means to them, and access narratives of disagreement over a
particular individual's diagnosis, both between parent and staff and even between staff
117
and diagnosing professionals.
This study investigates new biopolitical theory, which engages with the broader
concepts of identity and subjectivity. Many choices in the research design emerge from
the anthropological literature on these concepts. Adolescents and youth constitute the best
age group in which to study such questions because identity has been recognized as an
important component of this life stage (Bucholtz 2002; Erikson 1968). More specifically,
research has suggested that autism identity discourses may be most meaningful starting at
adolescence (Baker 2006). The choice to research in autism-specific sites isolates autism
discourses in a place where autism is actively and already the central focus. It both selects
a location in which new biopolitical subjectivity is more likely to occur (social
organization around autism) and avoids essentializing the identities of participants to only
those of “people with autism” by focusing on spaces where the theme of autism is already
central. In this way, the research design acknowledges that identity is a function of the
situation, not the person (Bucholtz and Hall 2006). This study can therefore be called a
part of the anthropology of youth (Bucholtz 2002), a field reviewed in Chapter 1, because
it considers the lives of these adolescents and youth as it relates to life stages and
transitions out of childhood and/or into adulthood (common to the anthropology of
adolescence), but takes a place-based approach that allows the researcher to focus on the
specific issue and/or identity of interest to the research questions (common to youth
cultures).
This study takes a cross-sectional approach, which allows for a broad picture of
the social world of autism in Italy while still remaining focused on autism-specific
spaces. It allows for many important dimensions of comparison. Demographic
118
dimensions of comparison are certainly important. Many scholars have noted that
neurodiversity narratives in particular are more important or appropriate to people who
are ‘high functioning,’ although there are also ‘low functioning’ individuals who support
neurodiversity and ‘high functioning’ individuals who do not (Fenton and Krahn 2007;
Ortega 2009). The aggregated data set will allow for comparisons based on demographic
factors of both parents and participants including age, sex, religion, and domains of
functioning (specifically verbal/non-verbal). Moreover, the cross-section of autismfocused sites allows for comparison between different types of autism spaces with
different goals.
However, this cross-sectional design also excludes many important dimensions of
comparison. I do not have the data to compare the experience in autism-specific settings
with mixed-disability or fully integrated settings, except through the narratives of parents
who had experiences in multiple settings. A systematic comparison would be fruitful and
is a direction for future research I propose in the conclusion. I have also focused on only
one age group, and can draw conclusions only on the behaviors and perspectives of
parents of adolescent and adult children. A comparison with parents of younger children
in the same region would be fruitful as well.
In terms of cross-cultural comparisons, I only collected data in Italy and therefore
do not directly relate participant responses to a comparable sample in other countries.
However, I do relate their responses to themes in the literature, seeking to determine of
my participants' responses are more concordant or more discordant with themes in the
literature on new biopolitical theory derived from a range of settings. In doing so I follow
the example of Bonaccorso (2009) in her study of new reproductive technologies in Italy,
119
exploring the proposition of previous scholars that Northern and Southern European
approaches may differ with respect to their conceptualization of biomedicalization. I do
not explicitly compare Italian perspectives on the definition, etiology, and treatment of
autism with U.S. sources, except when writing about how my participants discuss these
sources.
Section 2: Methods
Data were collected using participant-observation, interviews, and one focus
group. In this section, I begin by describing site selection and participant observation,
then subdivide the discussion of sampling and data collection for each of three
respondent groups: (1) professionals who work with autism; (2) parents of youth with
autism; (3) youth with autism themselves. All parts of the study were approved by the
Case Western Reserve University Institutional Review Board.
Participant-observation
Based on my pilot research, I identified three main categories of services
available to people with autism spectrum conditions: Day Centers, Residential Centers,
and Social/Recreational Groups. A more detailed description of these service types is
available in Chapter 2. To obtain a cross-section of these experiences, I conducted
participant-observation and recruited interviewees from three Day Centers, Two
Residential centers (which also offered day services), and two Social/Recreational
Groups. Organizations were included if they marketed themselves specifically to people
with autism and had attendees within the desired age group of 14-34. I gained entry to the
organizations via the coordinating staff. I did not exclude any organization that was
interested in participating.
120
Participant observation aimed at gaining an experience-near perspective,
accessing the subjective experience of attendees with autism through joint participation,
and building rapport to set the basis for future interviews. Observations focused on
activities undertaken, social interaction between the people with autism, role of other
participants (aides, therapists, etc.), topics of conversation, and conceptualizations of
autism referenced. The specific activities hosted by each site as well as my role as a
researcher varied substantially. Therefore, detailed descriptions of the fieldsites are
presented in Section 3, below.
Staff and Other Professionals
From each group I interviewed two staff members (total N=14). I sought staff at
two levels of administration: one close to the founding and administrative structure of the
organization and one close to the daily operation and client-focused aspects of the
organization. I interviewed coordinators, educatori, and personal care aides. I further
contextualized my findings with interviews with other autism professionals and advocates
working in the area (N=31). This sample was obtained through chain referral. I did not
exclude any professional or advocate who wanted to participate. All professionals worked
in Italy and spoke Italian, but three were born outside the country.
Staff and other professionals were interviewed using the same interview guide,
with questions focused on the organization's history, daily operations, and goals; the
definition, diagnosis, and treatment of autism; the professional's relationship with parents
and connection to other services; and personal history of involvement with autism
services. Detailed interview guides can be found in the appendices. Interviews were
audio-recorded when the professional consented, and lasted between 17 and 257 minutes.
121
Table 1: Demographic Profile of Interview Participants (Professionals)
Profession
Coordinatore (Coordinator)
Educatore (Tutor/Aide)
Neuropsichiatra infantile (Child
Neuropsychiatrist)
Psichiatra (Psychiatrist)
Psicologa (Psychologist)
Insegnante (Teacher)
Pedagogista (Pedagogue)
Other/Volunteer Position
Presidente dell'associazione (President of
Advocacy Association)
Psicomotricista (Psychomotor Therapist)
Ausiliario Socio-Assistenziale (Social Services
Auxiliary)
Consulente (Consultant)
Insegnante di Sostegno (Support Teacher)
Logopedista (“Speech Language Therapist”)
Pediatra (Pediatrician)
Riabilitatrice Psichiatrica (Psychiatric
Rehabilitation Therapist)
TNPEE (Neuropsychomotor Therapist)
8
7
5
5
5
3
3
2
2
2
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
Age
Range
Mean
Median
24-71 Years
47 Years
45 Years
Female
Male
31
14
Sex
Parents/Guardians
Client eligibility was the basis for both client and parent recruitment. Eligible
clients were those between 14 and 34 years of age with a diagnosis (or self-diagnosis) of
an autism spectrum condition as identified by the staff. The specific ages I use as
boundaries emerge from the Italian context. At age 14, young people (including those
with disabilities) exit middle school and begin high school and/or early admission into
122
day centers. Many Italian municipalities (e.g., Romano and Natilli 2011) consider all
young people under 35 to be “youth,” and there are special scholarships and initiatives
“for youth” meeting this age requirement (Comune di Milano 2012).
When possible, I selected three verbal and three non-verbal participants with the
goal of interviewing the verbal participants in addition to their parents, and interviewing
only the parents of the non-verbal participants. Language functioning is arguably the
most significant factor distinguishing level of severity of ASD (Twachtman-Cullen and
Twachtman-Reilly 2007). At sites in which fewer than 3 non-communicative clients were
present, a greater number of client interviews were conducted. At sites in which fewer
than 3 sufficiently communicative clients are present, only parents were interviewed. This
classification of verbal or non-verbal was initially determined by staff. However, in one
case staff and parent disagreed, and I have classified the respondent’s child as verbal
following the parent’s assessment and the centrality of the child’s (limited) speech to the
parent’s narrative.
Both parent and youth were interviewed using semi-structured guides loosely
based on the McGill Illness Narrative Interview (Groleau, Young, and Kirmayer 2006)
and Kleinman's Questions (Kleinman, Eisenberg, and Good 1978), with additional
questions of my own design addressing the choice of and experiences with services.
Parent interviews included additional questions on pregnancy and early childhood.
Interview guides were modified during the course of the research to improve clarity,
minimize respondent fatigue, and respond to respondent priorities. Detailed interview
guides can be found in the appendices.
I conducted a total of 38 interviews with parents/guardians. In 26 interviews only
123
the mother participated, in three only the father, and in nine both parents. Interviews were
audio-recorded when the parent consented, and lasted between 46 and 250 minutes. Two
interviews were with the non-biological parents of the child with autism, and questions
on pregnancy and birth were adjusted to ask about their knowledge of these
circumstances not their direct experience. Both adopted children were born out of the
country and adopted by Italian parents. Five other parents were born out of the country,
though due to Italian wartime migration (see for example Schneider 2000), some were
born to Italian parents and could therefore claim Italian citizenship.
124
Table 2: Demographic Profile of Interview Participants (Parents)
Age
Range
Mean
Median
39-79 Years
56 Years
55 Years
Female
Male
35
12
Sex*
Religion
Catholic
Catholic, Non-Practicing
Other Christian
Other Non-Christian
Atheist
30
3
2
2
1
Income
€0 - €15.000
€15.000 - €28.000
€28.000 - €55.000
€55.000 - €75.000
Più di €75.000
Don’t Know/No Response/Not Asked
4
10
8
3
5
8
Type of Site from which Recruited**
Day Center
Residential Center
Social/Recreational Group
16
11
12
Age of Child
Range
Mean
Median
14-33 Years
23 Years
22 Years
Sex of Child
Female
Male
10
28
Child Verbal?
Yes
No
27
11
* Total exceeds total number of participants due to the nine couples.
** Total exceeds total number of participants because one participant attended two sites.
125
Youth with Autism
I conducted a total of ten individual or group interviews with youth with autism
diagnoses or self-diagnoses. Interviews were audio-recorded when the youth consented,
and lasted between 23 and 95 minutes.
Previous research with adolescents and young adults with autism and
developmental disabilities in institutional settings has found focus groups a useful tool
because clients may be more comfortable speaking in groups (Llewellyn 2009; Ruef and
Turnbull 2002). I therefore intended to use focus groups in this study as well. However, it
was found unfeasible both because many sites did not have enough eligible participants
to form a productive group and because, as will be detailed in Chapter 6, many of the
youth with autism I met were less, not more, comfortable interacting with each other than
with me or with staff. The one focus group I did conduct was at a Social/Recreational
Activity Group, and had four participants, only two of whom stayed for the entire 92
minutes. Individual interview and focus group questions can be found in the appendices.
Demographic data on participants in individual and group interviews are
aggregated together in the following table. Two of the youth reported receiving a small
income from their work at internships. Four youth were actively students. Three youth
did not report schooling or employment (the three attending day centers).
126
Table 3: Demographic Profile of Interview Participants (People with Autism)
Age
Range
Mean
Median
14-20 Years
18 Years
18 Years
Female
Male
2
8
Sex
Religion
Catholic
Catholic, Non-Practicing
Other Christian
Other Non-Christian
None/Don’t Know/No Response
5
1
1
1
2
Type of Site from which Recruited**
Day Center
Residential Center
Social/Recreational Group
3
0
7
Youth and parents were both recruited based on youth characteristics and in this
sense the interviews were paired. While most parent interviews did not have a
corresponding interview with that parent’s child, all youth interviews did have a
corresponding interview with that youth’s parent or parents. The interview topics and
questions largely overlapped, asking about experiences in obtaining a diagnosis,
characteristics they associate with the diagnosis, general life experiences, and experiences
and opinions about organizations in which they participate. Interviews with parents had
additional questions youths’ early lives and the parents’ experiences with obtaining a
diagnosis. The overlap in questions allows for some meaningful contrast between
parental narratives and youth narratives. I do not directly compare what one youth said
about him or herself with what that youth’s parents said about him or her; rather,
127
responses are aggregated. The parent/guardian interview is also intended to contribute to
the understanding of the experiences of people with autism, and so I draw on information
from parent interviews in Chapter 6, which is dedicated to youth, both to further
contextualize the narratives of youth interviewed and to comment on the experiences of
youth who were not interviewed.
Data Analysis
Data were analyzed in several ways which mutually reinforce each other and the
validity of the findings in this study. Data analysis methods included: (1) Inductive
coding; (2) Deductive coding; (3) Frequency tallies of certain inductive codes; (4)
Average percentage coverage per interview of deductive codes; (5) Pearson’s correlations
of various indicator variables with the output variable of percentage of each interview
coded with deductive codes. Coding was performed with the assistance of NVivo 10
qualitative data analysis software. Statistical analysis was performed with the assistance
of SPSS 22 statistical analysis software.
Methods (1) and (2) provide rich qualitative data into participant experiences
relative to the research questions. Methods (3), (4), and (5) provide metrics that offer
some quantitative support to thematic findings. The purpose of these data analysis
strategies is not to engage in a quantative research design, but to bolster the qualitative
analysis through greater transparency of the numerical aspects of the coding schemes, to
identify relevant avenues for qualitative exploration that thematic coding along may not
have uncovered, and – in the case of method (5) – to generate hypotheses for future
research.
Inductive, emergent coding was performed to answer research questions (1) and
128
(2). For emergent coding, all data were first coded openly (Emerson, Fretz, and Shaw
1995), directed by theoretical interest in themes regarding autism meaning and social life,
including: definitions and conceptualizations of autism; goals of individuals and
organizations; and identity as defined by subjective reports of feelings of group
membership and presentation of self. This initial coding generated a list of emergent
codes, which were grouped and organized into a codebook, available in the appendices.
Deductive, a priori coding was performed to answer research question (3). For a
priori coding, coding categories were created based off of the literature on biosociality,
biological citizenship, and new biopolitical theory (LeCompte and Schensul 1999; Weiss
1995), detailed in Chapter 7. All data were then coded using this codebook, also available
in the appendices. Both supporting evidence and negative cases were sought. I
determined the frequency and overlap of themes and negative cases (Chapter 7, Section
2) using nVivo 10 query tools.
For both inductive and deductive coding, I maintained a “coding notes” notebook
in addition to maintaining the two codebooks referenced above. In this document I
maintained a detailed log of my coding progress and process. I noted which themes
emerged from my preliminary work with the data as I collected and created it; catalogued
themes that were added, dropped, changed, combined with another, or split into multiple
through this process; documented details of the recoding I conducted to ensure
consistency of the coding over time; and explained rationales for coding decisions. This
documentation process enhances the validity of these analytical methods by increasing
transparency of the coding process and providing a concrete reference for me to ensure
my own consistency in coding over time. The process of coding and recoding over time
129
also ensures this consistency over time and across documents, as documents that were
coded earlier were recoded to ensure their consistency with changes to the coding schema
made later.
In Chapters 4-6, I address several aspects of research questions (1) and (2)
through tables reporting frequency tallies of various codes regarding characteristics,
causes, and treatments of autism as well as some aspects of parents’ interactions with
services (such as how they heard about the service and their goals). These tables were all
constructed in a similar manner, drawing from emergent coding. As can be seen in the
codebook in Appendix 2, emergent codes included “What is Autism,” “What Causes
Autism,” “Treatment and Management,” and a code dedicated to “Services” including
subcodes “Heard About” and “Goals for Participation.” After initial coding to identify
segments of fieldnotes and interviews (as appropriate) that discussed these topics, I coded
subthemes under each code relating to specific characteristics, causes, or treatment
strategies, and again for specific ways parents heard about services and specific goals
they mentioned. For characteristics, causes, and treatment I then classified each response
based on the relevant axes of the commentary: in short, whether the participant supported
or did not support the characteristic, cause, or treatment they mentioned with respect to
autism. As the chapters below show, these responses were not always binary. Intermediate
responses are explained in more detail with respect to each specific table.
In Chapter 7, I report on the details of the a priori codebook of new biopolitical
themes. I compare the relative strength of each theme in participant narratives by
averaging the percentage of each interview coded with each theme.
Finally, I determined the association between themes and participant
130
characteristics using SPSS 22 statistical analysis software. Pearson’s correlations with
different themes were performed to understand if any systematic differences can be found
between those who exhibited narratives concordant with new biopolitical and those who
did not on the basis of several key demographics, described in more detail in Chapter 7.
Details of these models and the operationalization of variables are presented in Chapter 7
with the results.
Dissemination of Results
I have taken many steps to share the results of this study with Italian collaborators
and an Italian audience more broadly. The most transparent of these steps is the research
website I maintain in Italian at arielcascio.wordpress.com. I use this website to post
announcements of upcoming presentations and publications deriving from this research,
including this dissertation. I strive to provide translations of English language abstracts
into Italian to make my progress more accessible to an Italian audience, and I provide
more detailed Italian language summaries for some works. I have also shared copies of
some works directly with research collaborators, including the first version of this
completed dissertation and an Italian language summary of it. In these ways I seek to
disseminate my research in a manner that increases transparency, accessibility, and
communication with specific individuals and organizations in Italy as well as an Italian
audience more broadly.
Limitations
Some limitations bear noting. Firstly, data were collected by a single researcher,
which brings several advantages in terms of the depth of understanding of the fieldsite,
strength of rapport, and richness and validity of qualitative data. However, it also raises
131
the possibility for error and imprecision in coding. I have minimized this possibility
through recoding the earlier coded sections to ensure consistency with coding practices at
the end of the analysis and through careful maintenance of a codebook detailing my
decision-making process. Nonetheless, the possibility for error and imprecision remains.
For this reason and the sampling limitations described above, findings from this study
should be considered tentative and hypothesis-generating, not definitive or hypothesistesting. Secondly, any quantification strategy for the presence of thematic codes in
interviews may be limited in these semi-structured interviews, because several other
factors may impact the length at which an interviewee speaks. For example, parent
interviews conducted at the site from which they were recruited were shorter than
interviews conducted at other locations (the parent’s home, their workplace, or a public
place).9 Additional variation occurs in data collection variability in that some interviews
were not audio recorded, and some interviews were audio-recorded and then spot
transcribed such that portions of the spoken interview not relevant to the research topics
were not transcribed. However, comparing the presence of themes using percentage of
the interview coded attempts to mitigate these differences by looking at the proportional,
not absolute, weight of themes. For this reason as well, findings from this study should be
considered hypothesis-generating, not hypothesis-testing. Finally, this research design
which intentionally seeks people participating in autism-specific services is limited in
that it does not speak to the experiences of all people affected by autism in Italy, but only
9
Of the parent interviews, excluding those unrecorded interviews whose length of time was not
noted, 20 were conducted at the site from which they were recruited, eight were conducted at
home, four in a public place, and two at the parent’s place of work. Length of interview was
significantly (p < .012) shorter at the Site. Of the youth interviews for which times were recorded,
five were conducted at the site and three in other locations (the youth’s home, the anthropologist’s
home, a public place). The difference in interview length was not significant for youth interviews.
132
this particular group. Morever, it addresses the way autism is defined, managed, and
experienced by people directly involved in the social world of autism, either personally or
professionally, and therefore not necessarily reflective of all Italians’ perspectives. The
results should be interpreted as such.
Section 3: Description of Fieldsites
Over the course of 11 months, I conducted participant observation at seven sites:
three Day Centers, two Residential Centers, and two Social/Recreational Groups. My
experiences at each site were unique, therefore I describe then in detail in this section.
Variability in the amount of time spent per site derived from the staff's availability, the
number of hours in which the site was open per week, and the presence of holidays
during the observation period. I dedicated 2-3 days per week to each site. Table 4 details
the time I spent at each site.
133
Table 4: Time Spent at Each of 7 Sites
Site
Weeks
Avg. Hours per Week
Total Hours
Day Center 1
4
9
36
Day Center 2
11
12
131*
Day Center 3
13
14
178*
Residential Center 1
19
6
117
Residential Center 2
8
18
148
Social/Recreational
Group 1
14
7
102
Social/Recreational
Group 2
18
14
252
Total Hours
965
* Day Centers 2 and 3 total hours include interview times as interviews were conducted
during regularly scheduled observation hours.
Day Center 1
History and Administration
Day Center 1 is a Centro Diurno Disabili (Day Center for People with
Disabilities), accredited by the municipality. Day Center 1 was the first day center that I
visited, and I am grateful to the staff for their patience with the many types of ignorance I
brought with me, linguistic and technical. Day Center 1 is semi-residential, meaning that
it provides full-time service nearing 40 hours a week. It hosts up to 15 adults with autism,
providing both vocational/abilitative and personal care services. It was founded in the late
1990s by a Cooperativa, and employs educatori as well as personal care aides (generally
certified as ASA – Ausiliario Socio Assistenziale, Social and Assisted Care Aide; OSS –
Operatore Socio Sanitario, Social and Health Care Operator). The cooperativa that ran
Day Center 1 also provides services outside the day center both for people with autism
(evaluations, services for children, services for schools) and in mixed disability groups
134
(another day center, trial weekends in apartment living, vacation and free time activities).
Day Center 1 documents refer to the people with autism who use their services as ospiti,
as will I in this section.
My Typical Week
As in many sites, at Day Center 1 was generally described as “tironcinante,” an
intern. I conducted this internship for approximately 9 hours a week, for a period of four
weeks, per my agreement with the staff. I took notes, spoke with the staff, set up and
helped run workshop activities, accompanied the ospiti who left for external workshops,
and learned to help some of them with their needs on these trips.
Before the first day, I was advised that the ospiti didn't interact much with each
other, but that there were group activities and I was welcome to observe. I was also
advised that, as a novità (a novelty; a new feature of the environment), the ospiti might be
upset by my presence. Therefore, the staff would insert me “piano piano” (gradually), at
first just observing and then getting more directly involved. I was still surprised at how
quickly I was integrated into the setting. With some of the ospiti, I interacted from the
first days. Others I came to know more gradually, and still others I hardly interacted with
at all due to their difficulty with such an invasive presence.
Each day, I entered around 9am just as accoglienza (welcome) began. Some ospiti
generally arrived before me, but many arrived after. Ospiti arrived on pulmini (Pullmans;
shuttle buses) provided by the municipality, or with their parents. One or two parents
might poke their heads into the room to say hello or talk to the staff.
Accoglienza consisted of tea and biscotti.10 One of the ospite set the table from a
10 Biscotti refers to cookies broadly in Italy, not just the hard sticks of bread available in U.S. cafes.
135
box staff have prepared full of instructions and the necessary components in the
appropriate quantities. He also set up a daily calendar indicating the date, day of the
week, and weather. Another ospite set up a similar board on the other side of the room.
Once weekly, I accompanied a group of ospiti to percorso motorio (obstacle
course) at a nearby gym. When we returned, the room was set up for an activity called
catena (chain; assembly line), a collaborative exercise that all of the ospiti completed
together. On another day, the entire morning was dedicated to a creta (ceramics) outing
for six ospiti, and I accompanied them three of my four weeks. The ceramics workshop
was located across town, and five of the ospiti took public transit with the staff in order to
arrive. The sixth met us at the creta workshop, then we all traveled back together.
Staff ran two workshops focused on domestic tasks. I always assisted with the
first, Attività Domestiche (Domestic Activities), in which six ospiti hung laundry, cleaned
a table, and swept a floor in that order. After Domestic Activities, there was a brief break
and then a slightly different group of six ospite joined a different staff member in a
different room for Riordine Centro (Cleaning up the Center). Rather than working in
sequence one at a time, several ospite worked concurrently on separate tasks as assigned
by the staff, such as putting away dishes, folding and putting away laundry, putting away
groceries in fridge, freezer, or shelf as appropriate, and setting the table.
On the days I did not go to this workshop, I stayed in the main room with other
ospiti who worked on lavoro indipendente or tempo libero. Lavoro indipendente
consisted of cognitive and motor tasks, even computer skills, that the ospiti had
previously completed with the help of operatori until they had mastered them. Tempo
libero activities were often puzzles, games, or magazines. Almost all of the activities
136
were donated or constructed from donated and recycled materials such as old magazines
and plastic food containers.
Day Center 1 also engaged its clients in several other activities in the afternoons
and other days, including group tempo libero of listening to music chosen in turns (those
who can choose their own favorite songs, the others with input from parents and staff
who know them well), a social skills workshop involving a simulated trip to a store with
a staff member as shopkeeper, group games such as Tombola11 with both social and
cognitive skills goals, trips to a swimming pool, another Cleaning Up activity located in
an apartment run by the Cooperativa that increases the generalizability of domestic skills,
and a “manuality” workshop in which the ospite created objects they could take home.
Day Center 1 also organized activities for the parents, including group plenaries
two or three times a year, individual meetings to discuss individuals educational plans
and outcomes at least twice a year, and parent counseling services.
Day Center 2
History and Administration
Day Center 2 is also a Centro Diurno Disabili, run by a cooperativa and recently
accredited by the local health board. It is housed in a large building that is also home to
another CDD and a short-term residential setting for emergency relief. There are 16 utenti
at this CDD. The CDD at which I worked was dedicated to autism and similar
pathologies. This particular project of the cooperativa was founded in the early 2010s as
11 Tombola is a matching game, often translated as Bingo. However, I have chosen to leave it untranslated
here. Although the rules are substantially similar, in every iteration of Tombola I have seen played, the goal
was to fill the entire board, not simply one of the winning combinations of bingo. Moreover, the game does
not including the calling out of “tombola!” as in the U.S. bingo version. Finally, Italians recognize a very
important difference in the relative roles of Bingo and Tombola – Bingo is a gambling game played in
casino settings, Tombola a recreational game among friends a family.
137
an experimental program, and accredited shortly thereafter. Day Center 2 documents refer
to the attendees with disabilities primarily as utenti, which is the term I will use herein.
However, some of the documents in the structure most directly aimed at the utenti use the
word ospite, such as the sign for their bathrooms.
My Typical Week
At Day Center 2, I served as a volunteer. I stayed primarily in the dining room and
the two rooms generally used for free time and group activities, talking with the staff and
utenti, observing, and taking notes. I also assisted the staff psychologist on some days
with the activities she conducted.
I stayed at Day Center 2 all day twice a week. Once weekly the staff psychologist
was also present, and I sometimes accompanied her to work in other rooms. I also stayed
late to attend weekly équipe (team)12 meetings in which all the staff met to discuss
specific cases, upcoming special events, and operational details included proposed
changes.
Each day I arrived around 9:00 or 9:30. The utenti were beginning to arrive,
mostly by Pullman. The first activity was accoglienza, an early morning snack of waters
and cookies, and creating the agendas. Staff worked with the utenti to have them create
their own agendas for the day. One morning’s agenda involved an excursion to the
swimming pool for about half the utenti, an activity on which I never accompanied them.
The swimming group ate lunch early. In the morning, a subset of the utenti participated in
a cooking workshop and made the panini for this meal. Other lunches were delivered by a
caterer. Another morning activity was chorus, in which some utenti from this CDD joined
12 A French loan word.
138
those from other CDD for chorus practice in another part of the building. Other stayed in
the CDD and worked on free time activities such as puzzles and individual games, or had
group activities such as body painting in which utenti and operatori paint one another, an
activity that engaged both relational skills, sensory integration, recognition of colors and
body parts, and self-determination as the utenti made several choices regarding who
painted whom with what color and where.
During the morning activities, utente left one by one to work with a dedicated
staff member in the spazio lavoro. The work tasks were set up in discrete and clearly
labeled boxes, and the utente had a quiet, clean space in which to work from left to right
on completing the tasks (such as sorting or dexterity skills). Although a staff member was
present to help, the goal was for each person to complete the task as independently as
possible.
Staff described the lunch set-up as “self-serve.” Utenti in turn prepared
themselves a lunch tray and asked the kitchen staff for first and second courses. At the
dessert course, staff generally brought the dessert around and asked each person to
choose between at least two options of fruit or sometimes pudding. Utenti also cleaned
their trays in turn. After lunch, utente assisted staff in cleaning the kitchen, in shifts
divided by day. They dried dishes, wiped down the table, swept, and the like.
During the shorter afternoon portion of the day, utenti divided into two rooms
(generally divided based on level of functioning/support needs) and worked on free time
activities or talked with staff who filled out their diaries. One of the utente who had
started taking guitar lessons on the weekends might show off what he learned to one or
both of the rooms. Sometimes they worked on specific projects such as gifts to take home
139
for holidays or decorations for the center. Some utenti went outside the playground right
across the parking lot and easily visible from the center's balcony or shopping trips to a
grocery store across the street to get ingredients for the pastry baking activity or premio
(rewards) that the utente received for good behavior overall or after specific activities
such as Tombola. Tombola were the last activities of the day. Utenti played in two groups,
one seated around the table and one as a Tombola al piede (Tombola on foot) out in the
hallway.
The other day, utente did not go swimming, so everyone ate the catered lunch,
still in two groups. The main physical exercise of the day was instead a percorso motorio
(an obstacle course) either at a gym a short drive away or in the hallway. After lunch, a
staff member might host a guided relaxation session for some of the utenti, focused on
audio and tactile sensory experiences.
Day Center 2 also offered regular parent meetings including a general assembly
meeting and parents' discussion group.
Day Center 3
History and Administration
Like the other two, Day Center 3 is a Centro Diurno Disabili. It is run by a
cooperativa which also runs a day center that is not autism-specific, and several services
for people with disabilities, the elderly, children, and infants in the region. The autismspecific CDD was founded in the mid-2000s due to the growing number of people with
autism coming into the right age for the service. It therefore responded to a request from
the comune for specific services for these young adults. During my visits, there were 9
youth. Staff generally referred to the people with autism as “ragazzi,” kids, which is how
140
I will refer to them in this section.
My Typical Week
Staff introduced me to the ragazzi as a stagista, an intern. I gradually took on
more direct engagement over my three months at Day Center 3. At first I shadowed staff
and ragazzi in their activities, chatting with both, and asking questions. Chatting with me
was used as a reward for some of the ragazzi, but this was the extent of my initial
integration. I then began to shadow specific staff members through specific activities with
the ragazzi and engage in the staff role myself, with their supervision. The staff role
consisted in these cases largely of supervising ragazzi in their activities such as cooking
and setting the table, or leading games such as Memory, Tombola, Game of Goose, and
Sorry! By the end of my period of participant-observation, I was scheduled “in turn,”
directly the point of reference for one or two ragazzi at a time, always with my own point
of reference in the staff to help me.
I stayed at Day Center 3 all day twice a week. Every day, I arrived before most of
the ragazzi and watched the staff create the day's agendas. Most of them arrived with
their parents. Upon arrival, each ragazzo deposited his or her backpack in their
designated cubby and changed from street shoes to ciabatte (slip-ons/house slippers). For
external activities, they changed back into street shoes.
Each day had the same basic structure: Activities, Snack, Activities, Lunch,
Break, Activities, Snack, Wind-Down and Go Home. Many activities occurred each day,
for different subsets of ragazzi in different orders. Some activities were physical, such as
staffetta, an indoor obstacle course/relay race; and group stroll through a nearby park.
Others focused on various work activities. Ragazzi completed cognitive skills exercises
141
(classifying, matching pictures and words, stuffing envelopes, completing puzzles, etc.)
in a variety of formats: lavoro al tavolo in which a ragazzo completes a series of tasks
independently and in silence; lavoro in coppia in which two ragazzi complete the same
tasks in pairs, one person passing the puzzle piece or word-image pair or whatnot to the
other person for placement or classification; percorso in which the ragazzi in turn
complete similar tasks but on foot, walking between the collection of pieces and the box
or board on which they must be placed or sorted. A few ragazzi had computer workshops
with the staff in which they completed specific tasks such as searching for a specific topic
or typing letters to take home to their parents. Ragazzi also did domestic tasks in the
center, including a morning cucina kitchen workshop making fruit salad, nutella on toast,
or other desserts; setting the table; washing dishes; cleaning surfaces and floors; and
hanging up wet bathing suits and towels to dry. One person wrote what they had eaten
today and posted it on a bulletin board outside the door for the families to see as they
arrived to pick up their children. There were also dedicated leisure activities like games
such as Tombola, Memory, Sorry!, or Game of Goose; or individual free time activities
such as puzzles, reading magazines, drawing, or writing.
There were also day specific external activities. One day began with a visit to a
nearby gym for a subset of the ragazzi. Upon returning, it was time for the first snack of
the day. The day also ended with a visit from a gym coach for a short physical education
class. On the other day I visited, most of the ragazzi went to the community pool for
swimming or water aerobics led by the staff. They changed and showered in sexsegregated locker rooms.
Ragazzi divided into two groups for lunch and snacks. There were some tasks that
142
were assigned to one ragazzo, sometimes varying who by day: setting the table, serving a
second course, bringing out the oil and vinegar. Each person cleared his or her own place.
After lunch every day was a break in which one ragazzo chose a DVD for the
three or four who were interested in films to watch. These DVDs were often Disney,
Pixar, or Dreamworks animated films, but also included the occasional live action or even
music video. Everyone had a short break before continuing with more work-like activities
such as cleaning up after lunch or setting the table for snack. This second snack occurred
late enough in the day that generally the only remaining activity was to get ready to go
home and wait for the parents to arrive. This period also served as a social and
recreational time in which the ragazzi chatted, read magazines, or road an exercise bike.
Then parents arrived to take their children home. Parents chatted with staff, each other,
and each other’s children at this time.
Residential Center 1
History and Administration
Although this center offers many day services to individuals who do not live in its
residential structures, it is one of the few organizations to also offer residential services. I
have therefore classified it as a residential center. Residential Center 1 was founded in the
late 19th century under the auspices of the Catholic Church. It operated as a hospice,
caring for people with disabilities and chronic illnesses and impairments from the
surrounding countryside. In the 1970s, Residential Center 1 took up 'psychopedagogical'
methods in working with lifelong disability, and created a hospital for regular out-patient
visitors. In these years, the directors of the organization decided to focus their attention
on people most severely affected by disabilities and the impairments of old age. Moving
143
beyond the primacy of religious figures in its early years, the role of lay staff greatly
increased. Since the late 1990s, it has operated as an ONLUS (i.e., Non-Profit), accredited
by the Regional governments of the area.
Residential Center 1 offers a wide array of services to various populations,
including the elderly, people with psychiatric disabilities, and people with intellectual and
developmental disabilities. In the geographical area in which I worked, it offered 3 types
of services for people with autism: Centri Diurni Disabili (CDD), Comunità alloggio
socio sanitaria (CSS), and Residenza Sanitario Assistenziale per Disabilie (RSD). Two of
the CDD (divided based on strength of intellectual functioning) and two of the CSS
(which are in the same building but administratively two communities) are specifically
dedicated to autism. Historically, the services at Residential Center 1 have been localized
at its headquarters. In recent years, the organization has branched out to have satellite
locations in nearby communities, as well as collaborations with other cities and even
other countries wherein staff travel to provide training and supervision. Residential
Center 1 staff and documents generally referred to the people with autism who received
their services as ospiti, as will I in the following discussion.
My Typical Week
Residential Center 1 was admittedly the location in which it was most difficult for
me to mingle with people with autism, although I knew a few casually. My activities,
primarily shadowing highest level staff, took me into therapeutic spaces as well as équipe
meetings and supervisioni (described below). I also attended public lectures. In équipe,
supervisioni, and public lectures, video recordings were an important tool. I was able to
view, alongside professionals, parents, and community members, videos of specific
144
training sessions, problem behavior, outings, and typical days. The more therapeutic
spaces I toured included day centers, vocational workshops, and private meetings. I also
accompanied high level staff to residential spaces, both autism-specific and mixed
disability; both the small CSS and the large RSD. I was called a tinoncinante (intern) or
la dottoressa Americana (the American doctor), and generally treated as an emerging
expert in the field and an apprentice of sorts.
I entered into the field gradually. I started with individual meetings with the
higher up staff and attending lectures to the public. I was then able to attend staff
meetings: équipe, in which staff talk together to raise problems and create solutions; and
supervisioni in which staff bring videos of their work for critique by the supervisors and
their peers. I also attended a presentation to the association of parents of adults with
autism in residential services, where they viewed videos of their adult children's
activities. Later, I was invited into more individually focused meetings as well: Parent
Training in which staff guided individual parents on specific issues to work on with their
children; inserimento (admission) of a new ospite into one of the day services; requests
for new admissions, particularly for people with autism approaching their maggiorenne
(18th birthday); and a trip to a work placement site for an adult with Asperger's. Many
days, I shadowed a staff member as he or she moved through such activities. I also
received tours of various CDDs, CSSs, and an RSD. I attended three dinners at nonautism specific CSSs. In the end, I was only able to spend some short time at the autismspecific residences themselves. The house was generally limited to residents and staff to
minimize the type of unpredictability visitors bring, which is particularly bothersome for
people with autism who rely on the predictability of the residential setting. Nonetheless, I
145
was able to attend a Sunday lunch accompanied by one of the highest level staff
members, and also a Sunday morning mass with the regular daily staff.
The residences together provides a home for 12 residents in double rooms with a
shared living/dining room, kitchen, recreation room, and outdoor spaces. Throughout all
of its services, Residential Center 1 serves hundreds of individuals with developmental
disabilities. There were staff day and night, although during the day the residents left to
attend day centers, go to jobs in protected workshops, or other vocational and
(reh)abilitational activities. Often these centers, workshops, and activities are those also
run by Residential Center 1, but not always. In the evening they return to have dinner
together (sometimes cooking together), engage in leisure pursuits, and get ready for bed.
For the highest level staff, this rhythm of work-home represents the 'normal' life, and is
the ultimate goal for which they are striving.
Residential Center 2
History and Administration
Residential Center 2 is a “farm community,” an agricultural context designed to
be more stable, simple, and predictable for its residents than the bustling city life.
Residential Center 2 was founded in in the early 2000s and accredited shortly thereafter.
It was born from the efforts of a parents' organization concerned with the question, everpresent for my participants, of “dopo di noi” (after us). It is run by psychiatrists and
educatori, with a staff including ASA, OSS, nurses, and doctors. A doctor is always on
call 24/7 to attend to emergency medical needs of the residents. Residential Center 2
provides services to 24 adults with autism and related conditions who live at the site in
small residential buildings and engage in work, recreational, and educational activities
146
during the day in their own buildings, shared common spaces, and locations in the nearby
neighborhoods such as the swimming pool. Residential Center 2 only provides services to
its own residents; there are no day services for those who do not also access residential
services. Residential Center 2 documents refer to the people with autism who live there
as residents and ragazzi. I will use the term residents in this discussion.
My Typical Week
I attended Residential Center 2 as a researcher/intern. I spent most of my time in
the company and role of the tironcinanti, interns from a local university program. I was
inserted into the Activities like the interns, and like the interns I was not scheduled for
non-Activity events such as wake-up, breakfast, lunch, and snacks. These events of daily
living took place in the private and homey residences; I knew the residents primarily
through the shared common spaces in which the Activities took place. I stayed the night
at Residential Center 2, in one of the guest rooms set aside for interns, volunteers, and
staff who needed to stay overnight between shifts. Some evenings I went into nearby
towns with the other interns.
I attended Residential Center 2 for two straight days, staying overnight in
between. Every day followed the same clear structure. For me, this structure was
generally Activity A, break, Activity B, break, Lunch, Passaggio di Consegna, Break,
Activity C. My break periods were paralleled by similar relaxing moments for the
residents in their own homes, generally Snack. I participated in almost all of the
scheduled activities. As a farm community, caring for their alpacas was a central task.
Other outdoor activities including Trekking and Percorso Vita (fitness trail), in which the
residents walked the grounds around their home. There were several creative activities.
147
Closely linked to the care of the alpacas were the weaving and sewing activities. Several
residents had learned or were learning from a dedicated instructor how to use looms of
varying complexity. They wove fabrics that could become bags, rugs, scarves, or other
products. In the sewing activity, residents worked closely with a staff member who
operated the sewing machine to create these products. The residents also worked in a
ceramics laboratory, in which the dedicated instructor there instructed them in various
stages of the ceramic arts, from creating beads and pouring molds to using the furnace. In
another activity, Bricolage, residents worked with staff direction to create items from the
finished ceramics, such as scacciapensieri (wind chimes).13 Products from the ceramics
lab were also sometimes used in the woven products such as buttons for bags and purses.
Another creative activity was the weekly music laboratory, led by a group of psychiatrists
from a nearby university. There was also a cognitive skills activity in which residents
spent time together working on mostly individual tasks such as reading, writing, math
problems, or facilitated communication with staff on the computer. Residents also had
dedicated activity periods for chores in their own houses, such as sweeping, taking out
the trash, bringing in water and juices from the storage room, or completing special tasks
for the home. Finally, there was the relaxing indoor activity of cineforum, in which
residents picked a film to watch from their library of superhero films, animated films
(such as Disney, Pixar, the Simpsons, and those by Italian companies), Westerns, and
Italian classics. There were also a few activities in which I did not directly participate
including farming, harvesting, and swimming.
Social/Recreational Group 1
13 This word also refers to a small mouth harp, but in this context it was used to refer to wind chimes.
148
History and Administration
Social/Recreational Group 1 serves people with autism and rare diseases, offering
both sports (primarily swimming and running) and indoor games (Ludoteca). Several
groups work closely together in the administration of these services. The story begins
with a parents' association, founded in the mid-2000s, for parents of “differently abled”
children of varying ages. The parents devised projects for their children, sought help from
local municipalities, and created a cooperativa to realize these projects with the
appropriate personnel. The Ludoteca began in the mid-2000s and has expanded over
time. In 2013, it was divided into two groups: i piccoli/bambini e adolescenti (the
younger, 13 years old and under) and i grandi/giovani adulti (the older, 14 years old and
higher). The sports project began in the late 2000s, with participants ranging in age from
about 12-19. Since the early 2010s, both have been managed by the cooperativa with the
same set of staff and participants. I therefore write about them as one group, although
there are certainly a few participants and staff members who frequent only sports and not
games, or vice versa. There are also those who attend only sports practices in the gym
and not the pool, and vice versa. Social/Recreational Group 1 refer to the participants
with disabilities using various terms: utenti (users/clients), ragazzi (kids), giovani
(youth), atleti (athletes). In this discussion, I have chosen to use atleti when referring to
participants in the sports (swimming and running), and giovani when referring to the
participants in the Ludoteca.
Although the cooperativa ran both activities, they coordinated them separately in
separate équipe meetings. The Ludoteca staff met three times a year, those who worked
with i piccoli and i grandi together. They also held 3-day training sessions for new
149
volunteers. Generally, both volunteers and giovani first got to know the cooperativa
through the Ludoteca before joining the sports practices, although there were exceptions.
The sports staff met monthly, alternating between équipe and supervisioni. In the unique
set of months in which I was visiting, every month had an équipe. équipe meetings were
divided into two parts: communications and cases. In the first part, the coordinator
discussed general progress, upcoming events, and new athletes who would be joining the
group. In the second part, staff discussed any specific cases that needed attention, i.e.,
problems with any of the individual athletes. They worked together, led by the
coordinator, to devise an educational strategy to address problems and to put all staff on
the same page in doing so. All of the educatori and the coaches attended. équipe meetings
were a place to discuss the administrative and educational aspects of their work.
Supervisioni, in contrast, served as an opportunity for the staff to reflect on their work,
their rapport with the athletes and their colleagues, and any problems they may be facing.
These meetings focused on the staff as individuals and their personal relationships to
their work.
My Typical Week
I was welcomed into Social/Recreational Group 1 as a “free agent” - not a
volunteer, educatrice or psicologa, but also not an athlete. At times staff referred to me
also as an anthropologist, which is how I referred to myself when I introduced myself.
Social/Recreational Group 1 marketed itself for people with autism specifically
but not exclusively. There were 24 participants during my visits, a third of whom had
diagnoses on the autism spectrum. In my observations, I focus on the staff, the
volunteers, and the athletes and youth that the staff identified as having autism spectrum
150
conditions. I do not write about the other athletes and youth who had a range of different
disabilities, except as it relates to the staff's work.
I spent the least amount of time per week at Social/Recreational Group 1, simply
because Social/Recreational Group 1 met less frequently than the full-time Day Centers,
Residential Centers, or even the other Social/Recreational group. I attended swimming
practice, gym practice, and Ludoteca once weekly. I also attended monthly équipe
meetings (4 in total) and three weekend gare (races, running or walking).
Swimming practice met for two hours in the late afternoon at a public pool. We
met in the lobby where parents come to drop off their children in a passaggio educativo
(educational passage) from parent to the designated responsible educatore. Then we went
in sex-segregated groups to the locker rooms to change, and into the pool where the boys
and girls joined back together and we had our opening circle. The coach asked the
athletes to identify who was absent, who was returning, and (when relevant) who was
new. Then we ran under a quick shower and into the pools. We generally started together
in the medium-sized pool for “games,” then divided into different groups for the
exercises. At the end, we all went to the smallest pool to form the closing circle. The
coach acknowledged good works and spoke words of encouragement. We then said
goodbye, and split into boys' and girls' locker rooms again. Back in the lobby, parents
retrieved their children and the staff delivered summaries of their children's performances
that day, as well as any relevant health information.
Gym practice worked in much the same way, meeting for two hours in the early
evening. Parents dropped their children off outside the gym, and athletes and educatori
walked to the locker rooms in small groups. We then entered the gym and had a period of
151
free play while waiting for everyone to finish changing. Once all of the athletes were
present, the coach whistled to start the initial circle. She asked each athlete in turn “con
chi alleni?” (with whom are you practicing?) and focused on getting verbal responses
from those who spoke (almost everyone). The ratio was generally two athletes to one
educatori, but with several volunteers present each athlete usually had one dedicated
guida (guide), either educatore or volunteer. As a “free agent,” I switched between
different small groups or assisted the coach. After the circle, we split into a series of
exercises. Sometimes athlete and guide ran together, other times they ran in turn or one to
the other, and sometimes the whole group of guides ran together and then the whole
group of athletes, somewhat competitively. Always it was clear who was with whom. The
day ended with stretching and a closing circle, then back to the locker rooms to shower
and change, sometimes alongside athletes from local sports teams. Parents met their
children and heard summaries of their performance from the staff in the gym hallway as
the other sports teams started their practices.
One purpose of the sports practices was to prepare the athletes for gare, races in
the greater community where the athletes could participate as a team amidst other teams
and individuals. I attended three such events – a walk, a 3k run, and a 3k/8k/23k run in
which I ran with a group that completed 8k.
The Ludoteca met every Saturday afternoon for three hours and included an
opening circle, the days' structured activity, a snack, a period of free play, the awarding of
stars, and a closing circle. Parents picked up and dropped off their children in the lobby
or on the patio right outside. Sometimes the Ludoteca was divided into maschi who
worked on constructing a Gioco Dell'Oca (Game of Goose) board game and femmine
152
who worked on other activities. As all of my sample were male, I went with the maschi.
When we played the Game of Goose, we played all together. Many times the Ludoteca
group made outings into the surrounding neighborhoods including a bowling alley, an
educational farm trip, and an evening pizza outing.
Social/Recreational Group 2
History and Administration
Social/Recreational Group 2 was founded in the late 2000s, an initiative of a nonprofit association of parents and self-advocates affected by Asperger's, which itself was
founded only in the early 2000s. Although I participated in occasional events hosted by
the association (such as a lecture they co-sponsored at the local university and the general
body meeting), I participated almost exclusively in the activities of Social/Recreational
Group 2 itself. Because Social/Recreational Group 2 had hundreds of soci (members), the
number of participants varied widely day to day and event to event. The same building
housed also another initiative of this group: a cooperativa sociale (social cooperative)
which translates and publishes English-language books on Asperger's Syndrome into
Italian, as well as designing websites and graphics. Staff and volunteers referred to the
teenagers and young adults who attended the Social/Recreational Group as “i ragazzi”
(the kids), as will I in this section.
My Typical Week
At Social/Recreational Group 2, I was explicitly established as an anthropologist.
I spent by far more time at Social/Recreational Group 2 than any other site, because even
after the period in which I considered myself officially researching - dedicating as much
time as possible to being present during Social/Recreational Group 2 activities, as well as
153
arranging and conducting interviews - I stayed on for the singular yet still timeconsuming task of finishing a tabletop RPG game I had started with a certain group.
Participating in this game was done mostly for fun, and I did not take intensive fieldnotes
as I did for the first five months. I wrote down only information of particular interest such
as when the participants reflected on Social/Recreational Group 2, their relationships with
each other and others at the Site, or the autism spectrum directly or implicitly; I did not
detail our RPG campaign, as I imagine this would not interest the reader.
My typical weekend at Social/Recreational Group 2 consisted of 7 to 8 hours a
day both Saturday and Sunday. I began participant observation on a Sunday, and it is
therefore on a Sunday that I shall begin my descriptions.
There is only one activity on a typical Sunday: Ludoteca. The Ludoteca was run
by one organizer, with the help of several psychology students who volunteered, usually
two per Sunday. A typical day at the Ludoteca started shortly after 15:00. I typically
arrived at 15:00, give or take 15 minutes. After most people had arrived, generally around
15:30 or 16:00, the organizer and volunteers would set up games. The organizer ran a
tabletop RPG in which at least 4-6 ragazzi participated. The volunteers and I ran smaller
board games of 4-8 people: Cluedo (Clue), Labirinto (Labyrinth), King of Tokyo, Jenga,
and the like - a combination of US and European games. We played until about 17:30.
Not everyone played the board games. Some remained the Conversation Corner reading
comics or magazines in silence. Others played the video game console or watched videos
on the other computer.
Around 17:30, Social/Recreational Center 1 hosted a snack break after which
players started to leave. Some left alone, others with parents. A few left quietly, but most
154
said cheerful goodbyes to the staff and volunteers, each other, or the room as a whole. I
often chatted with the parents who arrived at this time. Those who stayed often played a
few more board games, until 18:30 or 19:00 when the volunteers started to leave and
those who remained started to make plans for dinner. Starting shortly after or shortly
before dinner, we began playing tabletop RPGs, often until 23:30.
On Saturdays, I participated to varying degrees in the Theatre Course, Movie
Nights, and the Self-Advocacy Group meetings.
The theatre course was run by a parent organizer and two volunteer assistants: an
educatore with acting experience, and an educatrice trained in therapeutic theatre. Two
parents served in more administrative roles. They did not participate in the lessons
directly, but remained in Social/Recreational Group 2 and helped coordinate the midlesson snack break.
The participants arrived by 15:15 every Saturday for practices. The activity began
promptly at 15:30. They started with riscaldamento (warm-ups). The days that I attended,
participants started with mediation and stretching, and then we moved into a guided
warmups followed by theatre games. At 16:15, there was a brief break for cookies and
soda, then back to the "stage" at 16:30. The second part of the practice involved fourperson guided improv scenes followed by commentary from the audience of other
participants and the organizers and volunteers. I participated in the improvisation
exercises as well. Theatre practice ended at 17:30 with a closing circle, in which the
organizer asked for participant feedback. The final performance was a series of comedic
scenes with eleven actors and a full house in the audience.
Movie Nights were a regular occurrence when I began observation, dwindling by
155
the end of my time in Italy, and restarted after my departure from what I have heard. The
films I joined them in watching included Willy Wonka and the Chocolate Factory, Adam,
Blade Runner, Amelie, Kung Fu Cult Master, and Animal Farm (at one point a contender
for the theatre group's final performance), and I heard they had also watched Forrest
Gump on a day I was absent. One staff member explained that they are not always films
about Asperger's, but always about relazioni sociali (social relationships). Staff wanted to
have a film viewing followed by a discussion, which they accomplished most
successfully following the film Adam, whose protagonist explicitly has Asperger's.
The Self-Advocacy Group was run using a round-table discussion modeled off of
the “World Cafe” strategy, and facilitated by a volunteer psychologist. Each table
received a question to discuss, and marks to write brainstorming notes on the paper
tablecloth. After a set period of time, each table was to select and “ambassador” who
would stay and explain the notes, whereas everyone else moved to different tables. Then
they would get a second topic to discuss, and move forward the brainstorming. On the
first day, the topics were “what would you like (this group) to accomplish? And: what
will you personally contribute to today’s discussion?” Later meetings delved into more
specifics of fundraising, entertainment, self-advocacy, and self-help. I did not participate
heavily in this activity because it was aimed at adults older than my target age group, and
because many of the participants traveled to arrive and did not know me nor my purpose,
sometimes confusing me for a participant on the spectrum despite my self-introduction
and explanation of my role. Despite its target age group, a few of the young adults I knew
from Ludoteca, Movie Nights, Theatre, and miscellaneous outings participated as well.
The information I discuss in this dissertation comes from their perspectives and
156
discussions.
Social/Recreational Group 2 also hosted a few outings in which I participated,
including a bowling trip, a visit to the theatre, and a fundraising concert. There were also
activities in which I never took part including a group Cooking Course, coordinated by a
professional teacher in the area; more individualized instructional activities such
computer classes and mini-fieldtrips to teach the skills involved in getting around town
and interacting with local businesses; and outings organized by the boys themselves to
local festivals or attractions.
Results Overview
Drawing from interviews and fieldnotes, the remainder of the dissertation will
unpack how autism is defined, managed, and experienced in Italy. Chapter 4 reports on
professionals’ experiences, and answers research question (1): How is autism
conceptualized in Italy? Chapters 5 and 6 report on the experiences of parents and youth
with autism respectively, answering this question as well as research question (2): How
do people defined as autistici and their families define, manage, and experience that
label? Building on these descriptive chapters, Chapter 7 will turn towards the theoretical,
addressing research question (3): How does new biopolitical theory apply, or not, to
social organization around autism in Italy?
157
Chapter 4: Professional Conceptualization of Autism
Chapter Overview
This chapter presents the results of data collection with professionals through
interviews, observations, and informal conversations. It provides the first major step in
answering Research Question 1, “How is autism conceptualized in Italy?” It also sets the
scene for the conceptualizations of parents and people with autism who interface with
professional services by detailing those services from the provider perspective. The
chapter is divided into two sections, “What is autism?” and “How should autism be
managed?”
Section 1, “What is Autism,” addresses the definition and etiology of autism.
Professionals define autism primarily in terms of the triad of impairments listed in the
DSM-IV (sometimes called the Wing-Gould Triad): social interaction, social
communication, and restrictive and repetitive interests. Additional characteristics most
commonly mentioned by professionals include behavior problems, sensory and
perception difficulties, rigidity, and particular strengths. Special consideration is given in
this section to the definition of Asperger's Syndrome, particularly in light of changes
between the DSM-IV-TR and the DSM-5, which was published during the fieldwork
period. The majority of respondents defined Asperger's as autism without intellectual
disability, lending credence to the idea that Asperger's may become what Ian Hacking
(2010) calls a “transient” and Nadesan (2005) a “niche” disorder. Respondents were less
homogenous in their thoughts on the causes of autism, except for a consistently firm
rejection of the “refrigerator mothers” theory that poor parenting causes autism. Many
suspected that autism was caused by a combination of genetic predisposition and
158
environmental triggers
Section 2, “How Should Autism Be Managed,” delves into professionals' work. It
outlines eight different philosophies of intervention, and explicates their interrelatedness.
Although I do not explicitly compare Italian approaches to those in other countries, I do
discuss Italian perspectives on approaches that were initially developed in the United
States. What all the models discussed share is that they all employ what I read as a social
model of disability, focusing on changing the environment to facilitate the learning
ability, autonomy, and well-being of people on the spectrum, rather than attempting to
make the people with autism change into neurotypical people. This approach clearly
connects to the definition of autism used by professionals by adapting the environment to
the characteristics of people with autism, following a somewhat neurodiverse model.
Professionals also connected treatment and management discussions to the characteristics
of people with autism when they criticize certain interventions for being too “rigid,”
particularly inappropriate for people with autism who professionals described as already
so rigid themselves. Section 2 ends with a consideration of treatment in the context of the
life cycle and the problem that professionals called “dopo di noi,” meaning the futures of
people with autism as their parents age.
Section 1: What is Autism?
It is difficult to begin talking about autism due to important semantic concerns. As
discussed above, Italy draws from the DSM and ICD in defining autism for administrative
purposes. In these documents, autism forms part of disturbi pervasivi dello sviluppo
(pervasive developmental disorders) or disturbi generalizzati dello sviluppo (generalized
developmental disorders). When I asked one psychologist, he told me the terms are
159
interchangeable. However, a teacher told me that they have different connotations for a
lay listener:
Pervasive is something that changes your life. It’s a kid that is incapable of having
social relationships, it’s not generalized, like saying “oh well, he’s a little bit of a
goofball because every so often he does this kind of thing but… he makes friends
and goes places,” for example. No, if it's pervasive, it comes to affect you, to
damage you in something. Therefore, pervasive is more serious.
– Interview, Teacher
When discussing the term, as 17 professionals did, many simply described autism as
falling under the umbrella of disturbi pervasive/generalizzati dello sviluppo. However,
three professionals explained to me that they found these non-autism terms somewhat of
a linguistic trickery, a sentiment expressed also in the parent sample (Chapter 5):
We talk often of pervasive developmental disorder but that is still very broad, no?
[…] What I’ve noted a lot is that in medicine they don’t easily give immediately a
precise diagnosis, they prefer to wait [….] seeing that we have so many children
that maybe we give a diagnosis of atypical autism or pervasive developmental
disorder not otherwise specified, therefore for the, let’s say, ‘borderline forms’ of
autism, that it often gives the parents the false idea that they are children that can
be cured rather than that aren’t so serious.
– Interview, Consultant
They are similar, generalized developmental disorder and autism, but for a parent
to say “autism” is truly a label for which maybe there isn’t even awareness
therefore they think that their child is isolated, incapable, like he’s not... like he’s
not capable of feeling emotions. Generalized developmental disorder still talks
about development, therefore gives the idea of change, of growth, and, and
therefore they see it somewhat less like a condemnation.
- Interview, Educatrice
The above quotes demonstrate that while the term disturbi pervasivi/generalizzati
dello sviluppo referred, for many, to a technical term for the category of diagnoses into
which autism falls, for others it indexed a lack of severity. Though the teacher quoted
above sees pervasive as more severe than generalized, professionals saw both terms as
indexing something less serious than autism. As we will see in Chapter 5, parents did not
160
always find this semantic choice helpful in the long run. Focusing now on professionals,
however, those that referenced disturbi pervasive/generalizzati dello sviluppo described it
as either synonymous with a diagnosis of autism, or a broader diagnosis that might later
be refined into the more precise diagnosis of autism.
As a nosographic entity, autism falls under the DSM-IV category of pervasive
developmental disorders and the ICD-10 category F84. Professionals specifically invoked
these classifications. Just as the DSM and ICD have globalized the definition of autism,
diagnostic instruments have been translated and disseminated worldwide. In Italy,
participants who had knowledge about the diagnostic process (through direct or indirect
experience) consistently invoked the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS)
(Lord et al. 2000) and Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R) (Lord, Rutter, and
Le Couteur 1994), and less frequently Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (CHAT) (BaronCohen et al. 2000), Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS) (Schopler, Reichler, and
Renner 1986), or the Autism Quotient (Baron-Cohen et al. 2001). Diagnosis and
treatment planning also involves evaluations of IQ via tests such as the Wechsler Adult
Intelligence Scale (WAIS) (Wechsler 2008), Raven (Raven 2003), or Leiter (Leiter and
Porteus 1936) and evaluations of adaptive functioning, frequently via the TEACCH
Transition Assessment Profile (TTAP) (Mesibov 2007) or Psychoeducational Profile
(PEP) (Schopler and Reichler 1976), occasionally the Vineland (Sparrow, Balla, and
Cicchetti 1984) or tests of Italian or French origin. It should be noted that these
diagnoses, often made before age 3, almost must be made before age 18 due to the
transition from neuropsychiatry to psychiatry discussed in Chapter 2. An Italian
epidemiological study (described in Moderato 2008:46) noted that after as young as age
161
14, the prevalence of autism seems to drop suddenly due to minors with autism being relabeled with other diagnoses in adulthood. In my research, professionals from within and
outside the field of psychiatry were critical of mainstream psychiatry’s lack of knowledge
about autism, especially Asperger’s Syndrome, and both advocates and psychiatrists
themselves worked to increase awareness.
In defining autism, the majority of professionals included some discussion of the
“Wing-Gould” triad of impairments that characterizes autism in the DSM-IV and
elsewhere: qualitative impairment in social interaction, qualitative impairment in
communication, and restricted repetitive and stereotyped patterns of behavior, interests
and activities.14 More specific characteristics that fall under these domains in the DSM-IV
include lack of eye contact,15 using people as objects,16 a total absence of language,17
specific stereotyped mannerisms such as sfarfallamento (hand-flapping) and dondolarsi
(rocking),18 and insistence on ritual and routine or difficulty with change.19
14/45 professionals referred directly to some version of “the triad” of Lorna Wing or the DSM-IV, or
“the three areas.” However, an additional 22/45 described all three characteristics, just not explicitly
bounded together as a “triad.” Together, therefore 80% of participants stressed the importance of all three
characteristics.
15
Referenced in 4 interviews.
16
Referenced in 1 interview.
17
Referenced in 6 interviews.
18
Referenced in 4 interviews.
19
Referenced in 9 interviews.
14
162
Table 5: Commonly Referenced Characteristics of Autism (Professionals)
Characteristic
Behavior Problems
Sensory and Perception Difficulties
Rigid
Particular Skills, Talents, Intelligences
Difficulty with Organization and Multitasking
Good Memory
Lack of Empathy
Mindblindness
Visual Thought and Spatial Skills
Hate Being Touched
Motor Difficulties (Fine and General)
Attention Problems
Lack of Central Coherence
Concrete Thought
Honest
# of
Interviews
13
13
9
8
6
5
5
5
5
3
3
2
2
2
2*
* Asperger’s Syndrome Only
The diagnoses of autism that was codified and globalized in nosographies and
instruments such as the DSM, ICD, and ADOS were originally described in Austria, the
United States, and the United Kingdom through the works of those such as Leo Kanner
(1943), Hans Asperger (1944), and Lorna Wing (1981). In fact, the early influence of
Austrian-born professionals such as Kanner, Asperger, and Bettelheim leads an Italian
anthropologist to refer to autism, and Asperger’s in particular, as “il male austriaco (the
Austrian disease)” (Cola 2012). Nosologies and diagnostic tools were designed to
increase diagnostic validity and reliability between sites (Spitzer 1997) intra-nationally
and internationally. However, such efforts do not negate the need for attention to local
clinical expertise, as shown in cross-cultural studies of diagnosis for entities such as
schizophrenia (Kendell et al. 1971) and lymphoma (Keating and Cambrosio 2000) in the
presence of such reliable tools, as well as sections in the DSM-III and following editions
dedicated to understanding local expressions of symptoms and syndromes. Although this
163
study does not address clinician judgment experimentally, it does start from the
supposition that local professional expertise cannot be taken for granted in the face of
standardized definitions. Table 5 therefore shows which additional characteristics were
salient to professionals participating in this study (excluding those that were mentioned
by only one person). Two of the top three associated characteristics – sensory/perception
difficulties and rigidity – are now discussed explicitly in the DSM-5 criteria (Association
2013:50). The characteristic of honesty was salient only for professionals who were
describing Asperger’s Syndrome in particular. Participants stressed that people with
autism are very different one from the other, and that these characteristics may also vary
qualitatively or in strength of their presence. A few participants discussed gender
differences. Autism is diagnosed far more frequently in men than women. Participated
noted that women with autism diagnoses tended to have greater levels of support needs
(i.e., tended to be ‘lower functioning’) and also were less likely to have clear special
interests compared to their male peers, a finding that has been reflected in
epidemiological studies in the United States (Frazier et al. 2014).
The characteristics of autism were not entirely uncontested. Professionals
occasionally challenged them as misunderstandings. Media stereotypes often focused on
the specific aspect of impairment of social interaction that describes people with autism
as isolated and closed. This description was also given in a fifth (9/45) of interviews.
Others, however, contested this association, explaining that isolation is a consequence of
a lack of strategies for interacting or for overcoming sensory difficulties, not an inherent
quality of a person with autism. Another professional similarly challenged the association
of autism with a lack of empathy:
164
Some of the scholars who work on Asperger’s say also that the people with
Asperger’s do not have empathy, but this is something that many people with
Asperger’s reject, saying simply that their way of empathizing is different than
that which [the instruments] used to analyze them wants to see. Therefore with
those who study Asperger’s maybe they aren’t capable of understanding their way
of empathizing.
- Interview, Teacher
One interesting thing you see is that even with a person with autism [it is possible
to create a relationships, regardless of the fact] that before working together you
know that they are people who have trouble entering into relationships. In reality
they use preferential channels, anyway they use different channels [than] those
that most people use, [but despite this you can] have relationships, even very
important ones. It’s not that they can’t enter into relationships, it’s that they use
different channels.
- Interview, Educatore
When these professionals challenged certain characteristics, they specifically raised the
idea that people with autism do not lack certain traits (empathy, social interaction) but
rather that they present them in different ways.
This perspective that people with autism simply do things differently can be seen
in holistic descriptions of autism as well. Professionals had several ways of talking about
people with autism as fundamentally different from, but not necessarily lesser than,
people without autism. Half of professionals (N=22) made comments to this effect,
defining autism as a “modo” (a way) of being or understanding the world, a “stato”
(state) of being or functioning, “una modalità” (a way) of being human or processing
information, “un’aspetto della condizione umana” (an aspect of the human condition),
“una percezione diversa del mondo intorno alla persona” (a different perception of the
world around the person), and even “un’altra cultura” (another culture). Autism can
therefore be understood as a mismatch between the way the person with autism exists,
and the surrounding world. In the excerpts below, professionals explain how this might
happen:
165
Anthropologist: Are there other characteristics that are important?
Respondent: Uhm… eh, I’d say this. Maybe not for everyone… the existence of a
parallel world, that is, as though they are inserted into a reality different that the
one that we perceive.
- Interview, Psychologist
Autism is an impairment in relationships, in communication, and in the area of
imagination. And this is the technical definition. If then we want to see what
autism is, a person with autism is a person that lives with parameters different
than ours.
- Interview, Coordinator
Defining autism as its own way of being can offer positive valuation, moving away from
deficit-focused language.
The view that people with autism have their own culture or a distinct, yet valid,
way of being in the world, is reflected also in metaphor. A few professionals alluded to
metaphorical descriptions of people with autism that take these notions into the land of
science fiction and fantasy. They described people with autism as bambini or figli delle
fate (fairy children, changelings who were swapped for human children at birth, resulting
in children who are both physically beautifully and socially strange),20 aliens or
specifically Martians,21 or even fallen angels. Occasionally, professionals would also
draw on the metaphor of person-with-autism-as-foreigner, referencing my status as a
speaker of Italian-as-a-second-language and the difficulties I might face as a way of
understanding the difficulties a person with autism might face.
As I show in the next section, this perspective of autism-as-difference has
20
For a discussion of changeling myths and autism, see Valtellina (2015).
References to Martians assumedly allude to Temple Grandin’s famous comment to Oliver Sacks, “much
of the time I feel like an anthropologist on Mars” (Sacks 1995:259). In that statement, however, it is
Grandin who positions everyone else as Martians, not herself as an alien visitor. An anthropologist on
Mars, not an anthropologist from Mars. Metaphors of person with autism as alien are, however, common.
For an Italian example, see Macrì’s (2010) autobiography, Mio Figlio, un Alieno dal Pianeta Autismo (My
Son, an Alien from Planet Autism), reviewed academically by Ravanelli (2013)
21
166
implications for treatment. Professionals strive to create environments that respect and
work with the characteristics they attribute to people with autism, in what I describe as a
version of a social model of neurodisability that focuses on changing the environment as
the first step in facilitating change for people with autism. The remainder of this section
addresses further points in the definition of autism: the relationship between autism,
Asperger’s syndrome, and intellectual disability; and the causes of autism.
Autism, Asperger’s Syndrome, and Intellectual Disability
Although it is not currently a distinct diagnosis in the DSM-5, the DSM-IV and
ICD-10 recognize a particular sub-category of autism spectrum disorder, Asperger’s
Syndrome. The ICD-9, still commonly used in some Italian health boards, did not
initially include the term Asperger’s. At least in written nosologies, Asperger’s syndrome
might therefore be “transient” (Hacking 2010) or “niche” (Nadesan 2005:29) in the sense
that philosopher Ian Hacking describes Multiple Personality Disorder and others – a
diagnoses that peaks in prevalence and professional popularity at a particular time, then
all but disappears.22 Preliminary reports indicate that people who would previously have
been diagnosed with Asperger’s syndrome might fall under the new category of “autism
spectrum disorder” or the new and different “social communication disorder” (Frazier et
al. 2012). Just as Hacking argues that Multiple Personality Disorder is “transient” despite
the new name and category of Dissociative Identity Disorder, so Asperger’s can
tentatively be read as “transient” in this sense despite these new names and categories.
Over time, further epidemiological research will illuminate trends in changing diagnoses.
22
I presented a preliminary version of this subsection and this argument at the Society for Medical
Anthropology meeting in 2013 (Cascio 2013).
167
For now, this chapter looks at Italian professionals’ responses to the question asking for
the difference between autism and Asperger’s and applies these findings to the ongoing
discussion of Asperger’s potential fates.
Asperger’s Syndrome only entered into the Italian discourse over the past decade.
Professionals frequently described it as less recognizable, often diagnosed late or
misdiagnosed earlier. Professionals and advocates lamented the lack of awareness about
Asperger’s among doctors. They identified this problem among a variety of specialists,
but especially among adult psychiatrists. Psychiatrists who did work with Asperger’s
syndrome lamented the lack of awareness among their colleagues. Many people in a
variety of professions worked to increase professional and general awareness of the
syndrome.
Professionals had several ways of describing Asperger’s syndrome and its
relationship to autism. Of the 35 professionals who discussed the relationship between
Asperger’s and autism, four defined Asperger’s as some version of a ‘less severe’ global
deficit, placing Asperger’s on the high functioning end of the spectrum of autistic
characteristics. Twenty professionals explained high functioning in a different way, as a
higher IQ but not necessarily a weaker prevalence of symptoms. This more common
perspective is highlighted in the following quotation:
The person with autism has an intellectual disability. While the person with
Asperger’s does not have an intellectual disability but an IQ in the norm. Anyway,
the person with Asperger’s has serious difficulties always in the triad [of
characteristics], therefore in communication, in relationships, and in interests that
clearly, having an IQ in norm, it’s much more evident, in a person with
Asperger’s, [how] to verify the restricted and stereotypical interests, something
that for a person with autism with a severe intellectual disability, it’s more
difficult perhaps to observe.
- Interview, Educatrice
168
In perspectives like this, Asperger’s is defined as autism without comorbid intellectual
disability, or autism with a higher IQ. Indeed, the discussion of Asperger’s syndrome
links tightly with discussions of ritardo mentale or disabilità intellettiva both of which I
will translate here as intellectual disability following English language conventions on
non-discriminatory language.23 While many reports suggested that around 75% of people
with autism have co-occurring intellectual disability (Schreibman 2005:96), some
researchers have questioned whether the instruments used to obtain these numbers are
appropriate for people on the autism spectrum (Barbeau et al. 2012; Dawson et al. 2007;
Soulières et al. 2011; Biklen and Attfield 2005). As of 2014, the Centers for Disease
Control and Prevention (CDC) reports a much lower rate of co-occurring intellectual
disability in the United States: 54% of eight-year-old children with autism also have
intellectual disability (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention [CDC] 2014).
However, differing diagnostic practices cross-culturally may also affect these numbers;
as seen above, many professionals suspect that autism without intellectual disability is
less recognized by diagnosing practitioners.
The distinction between autism and Asperger’s based on the absence of
intellectual disability in the latter demonstrates that intellectual disability is often
considered part and parcel of an autism diagnosis unless that diagnosis is Asperger’s.
Many professionals addressed this specifically when defining autism. As one
neuropsychiatrist explains, “[L’autismo] ha delle ricadute sullo sviluppo nella sua
globalità, quindi, lo sviluppo motorio, lo sviluppo del linguaggio, lo sviluppo
dell’affettività, lo sviluppo cognitivo, lo sviluppo del gioco, è proprio… pervasivo”
23
See http://www.medscape.com/viewarticle/782769
169
([Autism] has repercussions for global development, therefore, motor development,
language development, affective development, cognitive development, the development
of play, it’s truly… pervasive.)
However, there were also dissenting voices in interviews, who saw autism and
intellectual disability as completely separate entities. In the words of one coordinator,
“L'autismo può essere associato sia ad una disabilità intellettiva importante, sia
all'assenza totale di disabilità intellettiva. Per cui ci possono essere soggetti autistici
molto autistici e molto intelligenti, e soggetti autistici poco autistici ma con grave ritardo
intellettivo” (Autism can be associated both with significant intellectual disability and
also with the total absence of intellectual disability. Because of this, there can be people
with autism who are very autistic and very intelligent, and people with autism who are
not very autistic but have a severe intellectual disability).
Several professionals discussed instead or additionally linguistic skills as
differentiating autism and Asperger’s. Three professionals explained that people with
Asperger’s had better language skills than people with autism. Six professionals
explained that people with autism had experienced language delays as children, whereas
people with Asperger’s had acquired language at the developmentally appropriate age or
sooner. Four professionals discussed this difference specifically as a way to distinguish
between Asperger’s Syndrome and “High Functioning Autism.”
It comes more easily to me [the difference] between high functioning [autism] and
Asperger’s. [....] The real difference between the two diseases, that is, the two
terms more than the two diseases, is that in those with Asperger’s language starts
immediately, normal with the age […] whereas for those with high functioning
[autism, their language] can appear also a little late, maybe language can arrive
also around 4, 5 years.
- Interview, Teacher
170
Some think that Asperger’s is different than autism [....] In fact, in [my]
experience, the distinctive characteristics are the language delay in the sense that
if a child has in his history from very young a history of language delay therefore
– then maybe he starts talking also very very well and reaches a good cognitive
level, but he started from a situation of delay, well, we talk about high functioning
autism. If instead the child has a story of completely normal development, or even
early acquisition [of language] […] in that case [it would be] more Asperger’s
Syndrome. Some say also that in Asperger’s syndrome a characteristic aspect is
clumsiness […] but this is not such a distinctive element in the sense that also
some of the kids that have high functioning autism could also be [clumsy] [….]
The [intelligence] quotient can be high, anyway, in both cases. In general however
the [intelligence] quotient of the people with Asperger’s Syndrome provides for
the verbal quotient, therefore the verbal ability, being higher than what is called
the “performance” quotient. While in subjects with high functioning autism it’s
the reverse.
- Interview, Neuropsichiatra
Even in these descriptions, what Asperger’s Syndrome and “High Functioning Autism”
share is their lack of intellectual disability. In practice, this creates a strong association
between the two concepts.
When I collected my data in 2012-2013 and the DSM-5 was only starting to be
released in English, professionals spoke about the response of advocacy movements in
Italy to the removal of the Asperger’s label:
The problem for the people who founded the Asperger’s associations, [is] the fear
that they could then, in one pot, lose the distinctive aspect of high functioning
[autism] and of Asperger’s.
- Interview, Psychologist
I understand that now it’s been decided to eliminate [Asperger’s] from the new
diagnostic manual, even if it’s an enormous pain for the people that have a
diagnosis and that identify themselves as “Aspie.” And therefore, from the ethical
point of view, I find a truly notable difficulty.
- Interview, Consultant
As of this writing in 2015, the dust has settled in the sense that the DSM-5 has
been published and the diagnostic category of Asperger’s is well and truly removed.
Groups focused on Asperger’s Syndrome did and do actively include people with autism
171
in their mission statements, following this trend of linking the two diagnoses rather than
focusing on the uniqueness of Asperger’s itself. The website of national advocacy
organization Gruppo Asperger explains the difference between Asperger’s and High
Functioning Autism (or better, “autismo con bisogno di supporto non intensivo” – autism
with need of non-intensive support) as principally linked to the earlier development of
language in people with Asperger’s. The site uses the terms syndrome di Asperger and
HFA (high functioning autism – autism ad alto funzionamento) interchangeably and
explains furthermore:
A partire dal 2014, sarebbe possibile sostituire la definizione di “Sindrome di
Asperger” con quella di “Spettro autistico”, specificando che la persona
interessata non ha disabilità intellettiva, e che non ha necessità di un supporto
intensivo.
Starting in 2014, it could be possible to substitute the definition of “Asperger’s
Syndrome” with “autism spectrum,” specifying that the person of interest does not
have intellectual disability and does not have need of intensive support. (Gruppo
Asperger Onlus 2014)
Like professionals, parents and self-advocates use terms like high functioning
autism alongside Asperger’s. This practice suggests that the loss of the diagnostic label
may not be as devastating as some anticipated. What remains to be seen in the coming
years is if the term Asperger’s remains in popular discourse and self-identification. Will
Asperger’s become a diagnosis “you have to fight to get” (Dumit 2006)? Will it become
an ex-diagnosis, like homosexuality when it was famously removed from the DSM in
1973? Will it simply be the common name for a diagnosis that is long and clunky in its
clinical terminology? 24 In 2014, it is too soon to tell. However, none of these possibilities
24
The DSM-5 (APA 2013:51) requires the recording of severity levels for both social communication and
restricted, repetitive behaviors, as well as specifying the presence of absence of accompanying intellectual
impairment and language impairment, suggesting that a diagnosis that would formerly have been called
Asperger’s disorder may now be written as “autism spectrum disorder requiring support for deficits in
172
would prevent us from calling Asperger’s a “transient” or “niche” disorder, a theory
which applies primarily to the professional recognition of a diagnosis. This section has
demonstrated that professionals often equate Asperger’s with autism (generally high
functioning autism) and seem fairly accepting of the change.
What Causes Autism
There are many theories and uncertainties about the cause or causes of autism.
There is no blood test for autism, no definite gene or other type of biomarker. Contested
etiologies provide space for many different theories, research agendas, and interventions.
Etiology is often tightly linked to intervention. I therefore asked professionals what they
thought was the most likely cause of autism. Their responses are categorized in Table 6.
Professionals often stressed that the causes of autism are unknown and hedged their
responses, but generally agreed that autism had its roots in biological and likely genetic
causes. Although two professionals argued that relational or affective factors might
combine with a genetic predisposition to create autism, professionals were most certain
on what was not the cause of autism: the psychogenic theories of the past, most famously
the “refrigerator mother” theory often attributed to Bruno Bettelheim, which claims that
autism results from “cold” mothers who failed to create the appropriate affective bonds
with their children.
social communication and requiring support for restricted, repetitive behaviors, without accompanying
intellectual impairment, without accompanying language impairment.”
173
Table 6: Possible Causes of Autism (Professionals)
Cause
Genetics – General
Biological, Neurological, Organic
Genetic Predisposition
plus Environmental Trigger
Genetics – Hereditary
Vaccines
Environmental
Genetic Predisposition
plus Relational or Affective Trigger
Genetic Predisposition
plus Bioagent such as Virus
Biological plus Environmental
Society that pathologizes differences
Digestive Causes
Pollution
Psychogenic/Relational/”Refrigerator
Mother”
# of
Professionals
Supporting It
26
13
11
# of
Professionals
Rejecting It
1
0
0
9
4
3
2
1
6
1
0
1
0
1
1
1
0
0
0
0
2
2
19
Autism spectrum disorders are associated with a range of specific genetic
disorders. Professionals often considered these disorders – such as Tuberous Sclerosis
and Fragile X Syndrome – to be causes of autism or co-occurring with an appropriately
made autism diagnosis. A few professionals differentiated between these cases of autism
and others by referring to “primary” and “secondary” autism – secondary autism being
autism resulting from another known condition. Others talked about “autismo puro”
(pure autism) in contrast to autism associated with other conditions. As we will see in
Chapter 5, parents did not always agree with professionals’ attribution of autism to their
child; several gave primacy instead to the genetic disorder.
This section has answered the question “what is autism” for professionals who
work with this population. We have addressed the characteristics of autism, the
relationship between Asperger’s syndrome and autism, and the causes of autism. This
174
work sets the stage for an understanding of autism treatment and management, to which
we now turn.
Section 2: How Should Autism be Managed?
Section 1 presented Italian professionals’ definitions of autism. In addition to the
triad of characteristics listed in the DSM-IV, participants identified important features of
autism such as a tendency towards behavioral problems and difficulties in sensory and
perception domains. Taking these characteristics together holistically, they saw autism as
a different way of being in the world and often described the difficulties a person with
autism faces as resulting from a mismatch between their characteristics and a world that
is not structured for them. What are the implications of this perspective for treatment and
management of autism? This section unpacks several philosophies and specific
approaches. Table 7 lists these philosophies in the order in which they will be discussed
in this chapter, starting with those that participants considered less appropriate for people
with autism and moving towards those that were more commonly lauded. Table 7 also
lists specific approaches and tools associated with each philosophy (though many are
associated with more than one). These specific approaches will be discussed later in this
chapter.
Table 7: List of Philosophies and Approaches
Philosophy
Synonyms
Psicoanalitica
(psychoanalytic)
Psicodinamica
(psychodynamic)
Specific Approaches
and Tools
Psicoterapia
(psychotherapy)
Psicoanalisi
(psychoanalysis)
Associated
Names
Bruno
Bettleheim
Psicologico
(psychological)
Ufficiale
(official)
Tradizionale
(traditional)
(Neuro)psicomotricità Local Health
Logopedia
Board
175
Philosophy
Synonyms
Psicoanalitica
(psychoanalytic)
Psicodinamica
(psychodynamic)
Specific Approaches
and Tools
Psicoterapia
(psychotherapy)
Psicoanalisi
(psychoanalysis)
Associated
Names
Bruno
Bettleheim
Psicologico
(psychological)
Psicoeducativo
(psychoeducational)
CognitivoComportamentale
(cognitivebehavioral)
Relazionale
(relational)
Abilitativo
(Abilitative)
CognitivoComportamentale
(cognitive-behavioral)
Educativo
(educational)
Psicocognitivo
(psychocognitive)
Abilitativo (abilitative)
Behaviorismo
(behaviorism)
Cognitivo,
Comportamentale
(cognitive-behavioral)
Neo-Comportamentale
(neo-behavioral)
Umanista (humanist)
Dinamico-Relazionale
(dynamic- relational)
“Emotivo”
(emotional)
Evolutivo
(evolutionary)
“Pietista”
(pietist/pitying)
Psicoanalitica
(psychoanlaytic)
Psicoeducazionale
TEACCH
ABA
Verbal Behaviors
Pivotal Response
Training
DIR/FloorTime
Denver Model
SonRise
PECS
Alternative and
Augmentative
Communciation
(CAA)
Social Stories
Superability
ABA
Functional Analysis
(AntecedentBehaviorConsequence)
(Neuro)psicomotricità
DIR/FloorTime
Denver Model
Massimo
Borghese
Eric Schopler,
Gary Mesibov,
Theo Peeters,
Enrico Micheli
Stanley
Greenspan
Lucio Moderato
176
Philosophy
Synonyms
Specific Approaches
and Tools
Psicoterapia
(psychotherapy)
Psicoanalisi
(psychoanalysis)
Associated
Names
Bruno
Bettleheim
Psicoanalitica
(psychoanalytic)
Psicodinamica
(psychodynamic)
Ecologico
(ecological)
DIR/FloorTime
Meta-Cognitivo
(meta-cognitive)
Stanley
Greenspan
Andrew
Meltzhoff
Bob Shalock
Psicomotricità
(psychomotor)
Animal-Assisted
Therapy/“Pet
Therapy”
Psicologico
(psychological)
(psychoeducational)
Relazionale-Dinamico
(relational-dynamic)
Sistemico-Relazionale
(systemic-relational)
Auto-MutuoAiuto (self-help
groups)
Psicoanalitica
As in Brazil (Rios and Costa Andrade in press) and France (Chamak 2008), Italy
has experienced a conflict between a psychoanalytic camp and other professionals who
work with autism. One participant explained:
Regardless of the guidelines […] they tell you “It’s not right, because it isn’t
enough, because the people with autism have an emotional dimension, a symbolic
dimension,” I say: “ok, I work here too – even the cognitive-behavioral approach
does not mean that you do not work with emotions, of course! But they give it to
you only in terms of behavior that… - then everything that I was reading even
yesterday was: not everything can be scientifically validated, because the people
have an individual dimension, but sure! We know that too! But still there is really
this diatribe and many – they are rekindling it, it’s not sleeping at all, it’s reemerging, in my opinion, it’s worsening.
- Interview, Psychologist
Participants raised psychoanalytic theories (psicoanalitica, psicodinamica) and therapies
177
(psicoterapia, psicoanalisi) primarily to reject them, and also as relicts of the past. Some
professionals acknowledged that such approaches could maybe be useful to parents
strictly in terms of providing psychological support, but not as a way to confront and treat
autism instead. Some professionals rejected psychoanalysis because they rejected the
causal theory associated with it – refrigerator mothers and poor parenting. Others rejected
psychoanalysis – like logopedia – because it is a talking theory that makes little sense for
people with communication difficulties. Even for those with good language skills and
high verbal IQs, professionals lamented that psychoanalysis is based on metaphorical
thought which is characteristically difficult for people with autism. Many pointed to the
lack of scientific evidence and/or support in the national guidelines.
However, there were professionals who found a legacy of psychoanalytic theory
useful in working with people with autism:
Each type of intervention has its pros and its cons, such that we use aspects of
classic behaviorism, aspects of DIR, aspects from the psychodynamic traditions,
therefore we use behaviorist techniques, but in this context with continual
attention to the subjective aspects that are of a dynamic derivation.
- Interview, Psychiatrist
In as much as psychodynamic theory was associated with relational approaches,
participants still found it useful. In as much as it was associated with psychoanalysis,
psychotherapy, talking therapies, and treating the family in order to treat the child, they
did not.
Psicologico
While the term psicologico could be synonymous with psicoanalitico, participants
also referenced it in terms of visits with a non-psychoanalytically trained psychologist
who might provide support to people with autism or their parents. Participants referenced
178
psychological support mostly for people with Asperger’s, but as a means to walk them
through emotionally difficult times or to teach them coping mechanisms and social skills,
not to psychoanalyze them.
Ufficiale
I asked participants what someone with an autism diagnosis receives in terms of
rights and services. People with autism had the right to certain economic supports, to
attend inclusive public schools, and to be taken into care by neuropsichiatria. Participants
consistently explained that the local health boards directed families towards
neuropsicomotricità (neuropsychomotor therapy) and logopedia (speech language
therapy), though there may be long waitlists to access public services and many families
had to pay privately. Participants referred to the combination of neuropsichaitria and
logopedia as ufficiale or tradizionale and lamented that these treatments were often not
tailored specifically for autism. However, there were many therapists who had specific
training in autism and collaborated with other professionals in equipe (medical teams) or
worked privately with people with autism. Both logopediste themselves and staff
members at various locations lamented the recent absence of these therapists from public
services and the need for parents to pay for them privately. I address these interventions
together as they were frequently referenced together. They are used most by very young
children.
Most participants referred to neuropsicomotricità by its shorter name,
psicomotricità. However, as we will see below, psicomotricità also refers to an entirely
separate school of intervention. As a psicomotricista equated neuropsicomotricità with
fisioterapia (physical therapy) In short, “La fisioterapia, la neuro, deriva dal movimento
179
patologico, ecco, e quinid noi, per noi il movimento è il mezzo. Per la
neuropsicomotricista, il movimento è un fine” (Physical therapy, the “neuro,” derives
from pathological movement, look, and therefore we [psychomotor therapists], for us the
movement is a vehicle. For the neuropsychomotor therapists, the movement is an end).
Similarly, a neuropsicomotricitsta told me about her job that “nei paesi anglosassoni non
esiste, questa figura professionale, quella più vicina è il terapista occupazionale” (In
Anglo-Saxon countries this professional figure does not exist, the closest one is
occupational therapist). This professional placed her work under the relazionale
philosophy and similar to DIR/Floortime. She explained, “la psicomotricità ha il pregio,
nonché difetto, di non essere un approccio rigido e codificato. E questo da un lato
permette di modellare nel qui e ora l’intervento su ogni singolo bambino” (Psychomotor
therapy has the virtue, and also defect, of not being a rigid or codified approach. And this
on the one hand permits us to adjust in the here and now the intervention for each single
child), but on the other hand it is difficult to evaluate scientifically. In practice:
It’s an approach obviously of a relational type, that puts the child in relationship
before everything else... with – it seeks to make him relate with the parent, then
with the environment, with other people, with the strangers, which are… that is
the therapist and with the world around him… through a process of progressive
regulation, self-regulation, no? and a progressive awareness of what – of attention
about that – both of the environment, but of the situation, that is, a capacity to
read the environment and the predictability of the situation of the surroundings,
[starting from] the stimuli. But this means managing to give your attention to
everything that is around … in that moment. We see children that improve so very
much from the viewpoint of skills in contact, gaze, responding to one’s name,
holding, sharing attention on objects.
- Interview, Neuropsicmotricista
Logopedia can be translated as speech therapy, but does not exactly equate with it.
As a logopedista comments:
I think that yours [in the United States] is more “speech therapy” and “speech” is
180
spoken language, and instead our logopedia is concerned with communication and
with language, therefore already our work the communication is already provided
for, in very form, that is: visual, verbal, gestural, whatever. We are concerned with
communication.
- Interview, Logopedista
As with neuropsicomotricità, professionals objected to “classic” logopedia that is not
specifically focused on autism. Some took issue with the entire concept of speech therapy
for people with autism who do not speak, but others pointed out the broader reach of
logopedia.
If I test you with a standard logopedia test, in which I ask you to repeat and name
things and you don’t have language, clearly… my test won’t result… reliably. But
logopedia means communication, and the communication you can evaluation, you
can teach it also in other… in other ways.
- Interview, Psychologist
Logopediste who worked with autism therefore worked also with visual communication
and were involved in developing visual tools for people with autism to use.
There were many types and subschools of logopedia, some of which focused
specifically on autism. Two professionals named and rejected one specific autism-specific
logopedia strategy, that of Massimo Borghese. One described this method as terrorizing
patients through “modi bruschi” (brusque strategies). However, there were also local
therapists who worked with autism specifically with methods that were generally well
received.
The other official service provided by the State is integrazione scolastica,
described in more detail in Chapter 2. Although not always perfectly executed in practice,
several professionals praised integrazione scolastica in theory and principle, describe it
as “un fondamento della nostra, è un valore per noi” (one of our bedrocks, a virtue for
us) and “una delle migliori legge della inclusione scolastiche e sociali” (one of the best
181
laws of educational and social integration). The rarer detractors, however, stated the law
was all politics and that having students in inclusive classrooms was not always the best
choice for them.
The majority of participants with autism in this study had finished their scholastic
careers by the time of the study, but the experiences from school certainly remain
important in their adult lives. Professionals raised several concerns about schools, most
prominently a lack of financial resources and lack of training for professionals. They
frequently lamented the fact that a students’ school experience was so closely linked to
the “buona volontà” (good will) of his or her specific teachers, not the valor of the system
itself. Several professionals who worked with older adolescents and adults argued that
people with disabilities learned more in centers than at school. The occasional
professional – generally those who worked with students with Asperger’s and autism with
need of less intensive support (‘high functioning autism’) – argued that schools might be
good for students with other disabilities but not with autism due to the particular
characteristics of autism:
Well, from my own experience, I must say that with respect to the integration of
people with, for example, learning disability, rather than a sensory deficit, I’ve
seen good experiences, where the integration has in effect worked and where in
effect there were also some good results thanks to it. Instead, with respect to
autism spectrum disorder, I must say that meanwhile many people who arrived
here from quote-unquote “normal” experienced also moments of frustration and
real difficulties, but really because the school was not outfitted, probably still does
not have the knowledge or competencies to be able to address integration in the
schools. And then I saw – but here, in a group mostly of youth with autism –
really because there are the same ways of communicating, the same
characteristics, the same way of approaching certain situations, that tie youth have
experienced it in a calmer way, because they have found an environment more
adapted to them, where they have even made friends, because anyway they have
found youth with the same type of interests. [….] Because where instead I have
seen situations at high school, where there are aspects like staying in class,
listening to the lesson, doing tests, integration, where there are a lot of requests
182
that for our youth are not high requests – they are too high, but not from the
standpoint of competence or cognitive ability, but really high in terms of handling
anxiety, of handling stress, it’s very tiring for them. Whereas in a situation more
tailor-made surely it makes them feel more comfortable.
- Interview, Coordinator
As this professional describes, there were occasional educational programs tailor-made
specifically for people with autism. She describes a program for older and more
academically achieving students that focused on the use of visual aids. Another program
to which many professionals made reference offered a dedicated classroom for children
with autism in an inclusive, which allowed them to spend part of the day in a setting
entirely structured following the TEACCH protocol and part of the day in the main
classroom or in joint activities with certain other students. As one teacher explained, “La
cosa che mi piace molto di questo progetto è che è proprio una scuola pensata e
strutturata per I nostri bambini in cui loro hanno il momento in cui sono assolutamente
con i loro compagni di classe” (The thing that I like so much about this project is that it’s
really a school thought up and structured for our children in which they have moments in
which they are absolutely with their classmates).
In short, neuropsicomotricità, logopedia, and integrazione scolastica are those
services and rights supported by the local health boards that can be very useful for people
with autism, particularly when they are younger, but which professionals often criticized
for not necessarily being autism-specific. As one professional explains:
The problem is that currently we are using in Italy therapeutic programs in my
opinion traditionally old, therefore psychomotor therapy and logopedia that the
guidelines for autism and international research have said are not adequate for
autism because they need structured psychoeducational interventions, intensive
but not obsessive.
- Interview, Coordinator
The following subsections turn to these structured psychoeducational interventions and to
183
how professionals worked to create spaces adapted to the needs of people with autism.
Psicoeducativo
The vast majority of approaches fall under the umbrella that participants called
varyingly psicoeducativo, educativo, and abilitativo. Many participants also called this
approach cognitivo-comportamentale, but this term was also used to refer specifically to
cognitive-behavioral therapy. I will first unpack the psicoeducativo approach, then turn to
cognitivo-comportamentale.
Participants used the terms psicoeducativo (psychoeducational), educativo
(educational), and cognitivo-comportamentale (cognitive-behavioral) to refer to
approaches approved by the 2011 National Guidelines for the treatment of autism
spectrum disorders (Sistema Nazionale per le Linee Guida-Istituto Superiore di Sanità
2011). The lay summary of the guidelines refer to these approaches as “pedogogici ed
abilitativi” (pedagogical and abilitating25) and specifically recommend parent-mediated
interventions starting from a young age, naming in particular applied behavior analysis
(ABA) (Sistema Nazionale per le Linee Guida-Istituto Superiore di Sanità 2012). The
broader guidelines (Sistema Nazionale per le Linee Guida-Istituto Superiore di Sanità
2011) use the term educativo as well and recommend specifically TEACCH.
Participants explained the psychoeducational or educational approaches as
follows:
Cognitive-behavioral therefore psycoeducational therefore the idea is to
concentrate attention on teaching the person with autism all those skills that are
useful for daily life, for their life, to be able to have a life, let’s say, of success in
the sense that is realized, dignified, happy, and also to support the family and then
the people that work with them. I make reference a lot to the philosophy of the
A neologism from “riabilitativo,” rehabilitating, given that teaching skills to people with autism is not
exactly rehabilitating, but abilitating for the first time.
25
184
TEACCH division.
- Interview, Psychologist
The best way to help people with Asperger’s Syndrome is to help them from an
educational standpoint. That is, to correct some of their behaviors, help them to
learn new instruments that they do not have, to relate better to other people, to
organize better than own lives, to manage situations. [….] There are different
approaches. But all of them should be based on educational help and therefore
helping the person to understand reality, decode reality for himself, and giving
him the best tools for interacting and understanding what are his true gaps and
improving them.
- Interview, Advocacy Organizer
The psychoeducational philosophy encompasses all those approaches and
strategies that focus on teaching people with autism and helping them learn, throughout
their life. They may include instruments like visual agendas, social stories, and
alternative and augmentative communication. The specific programs of TEACCH and
ABA are discussed in more detail below. Before delving into this detail, I need to address
a synonym for psychoeducational that sometimes has other meanings – cognitivocomportamentale.
Cognitivo-Comportamentale
Cognitivo-comportamentale (cognitive-behavioral) was the most common of the
above terms, yet it is one of the most difficult to parse. While cognitivo-comportamentale
often serves as a synonym for psicoeducativo, the term can also describe what others call
simply comportamentalismo – behaviorism. Participants seemed to use the terms
interchangeably – for example, many participants classified ABA as one or the other – so
I began asking for the difference. One professional explained:
The cognitive-behavioral [approach] is a little bit less... well, at one time the
behavioral approach was training, was considered like training. Therefore you
learn by reflex that is, not because you have really learned, no, but because you
were conditioned to learn. And so there. But in other situations, you would not be
able to do this thing, no? The cognitive behavioral [approach], on the other hand,
185
you work more instead on… uh… more on making them learn what helps them
do this this this and this other thing, not just one thing. That is, what you are
learning is not because I am training you to do this thing like a reflex of a
behavior that you do because there was this thing, fine, I give you a candy and
you respond to me, if I do not give you a candy, you do not respond to me. This.
But that I give you, uh, that you must do it, you must respond not so much
because I give you the candy but because it helps you do so many other things.
- Interview, Coordinator
However, when we look at how participants described simply comportamentismo, the
parallels and overlaps are striking, and also comportamentalismo can be described as
psicoeducativo.
Always starting from the assumption that for me psychoeducational means using
all the strategies and knowledge that we have ehm… pursuant to the person to
develop their abilities. For which, like I told you the other day, ABA; TEACCH;
the Denver Model, ehm…. I don’t know, CAA, Alternative and Augmentative
Communication, FloorTime, Sonrise which is English… that is, there are a
thousand things. [….] Therefore, the approach is always that, behavioral, neobehaviorist, I don’t know, call it what you want, with those techniques there.
- Interview, Psychologist
The Linee Guida (SNLG-ISS 2011) make a diversion between educational
interventions and a group they term “Interventi comportamentali e psicologici
strutturati” (structured behavioral and psychological interventions) including ABA and
early intensive behavior interventions (EIBI). These structured approaches are most often
used with (and evaluated for) pre-school children. With older children, adolescents, and
adults, participants referred to the Superability Model, the consistent use of Functional
Analysis (Antecedent-Behavior-Consequence), and the continued use of ABA teaching
strategies similar to discrete trial training as examples of this approach. One professional
described the use of rewards for fulfilling certain individualized goals for good behavior.
She described this system both as a tool taking from the cognitive-behavioral approach
and a strategy that is “tipicamente comportamentale” (typically behaviorist). These
186
techniques strive to create a carefully structured physical and social environment that
facilitate the learning of people with autism by removing distractions and making things
more predictable.
Adding the prefix “cognitivo” stresses the evolution of behaviorism beyond strict
Skinnerian “addestramento” (training). One professional explains this evolution as a
combination of behavioral and relational approaches:
[We seek] to integrate the traditional behavioralist techniques and in their even
more recent evolutions with constant attention to the aspects of motivations, the
effects, and the person. Because we cannot do a good rehabilitative work and we
cannot guarantee a good quality of life the people with whom we work if we treat
them as just a sum of behaviors to correct without investigated what there is under
that behavior, what is, for example, the distress or suffering that a challenge
behavior or problem can express [….] The problem is a matter of integrated all
the behavioral techniques with a constant attention to the subjective aspects, to
feelings, and to the emotions of the person. We aren’t dog trainers, right? We are
dealing anyway with people.
- Interview, Psychiatrist
While, as seen above, many professionals were not shy about using the term
comportamentalismo to describe one instrument among many under this approach, the
term comportamentalista might also be used in a derogatory way when objecting to
cognitive-behavioral approaches. One professional recounting the tale of staff
complaining about new cognitive-behavioral strategies as “comportamentale,” but she
herself explained, “Che poi invece cognitivo-comportamentale non è proprio
comportamentale ma vabbé” (But then cognitive-behavioral is not really behavioral but
whatever).
As a synonym for psicoeducativo, the cognitivo-comportamentale approach
involves the strategies listed above. As cognitive-behavioral therapy specifically, it
involves specific strategies such as prompting, constructive sabotage, and antecedent-
187
behavior-consequence (ABC) analysis. It employs highly structured instruments of
evaluation. While it does not inherently involve a relational approach, the two are often
combined.
Relazionale
Participants also described their approach is relazionale, synonymous with
evolutivo (developmental) and umanista (humanist). This approach rarely stood alone but
was an important or even central characteristic of their work for some participants. The
following two examples from professionals explain in more detail:
Well the philosophy is... in part behaviorist, in part humanist. They are [like this],
because anyway we are Italian (laughing), and in my opinion very humanist. In
the sense that we like more than working on behavior, on skills, tasks, uh, we also
have opportunities to experiment, some experiences that seem to have little value
from the standpoint of learning but that have a great human value, like birthdays,
birthday parties, vacations [….]. We are, yes, in my opinion we are very
humanistic as a philosophy. In my opinion this is a very Italian characteristic, in
my opinion, yes.
- Interview, Educatore
The most important instrument is the relationship, to understand that […] it [is]
impossible that that the child does not have anything to say and if he has nothing
to tell us it is we who do not succeed to read him. This is why it’s the relationship.
Clearly in a welcome and attentive environment. This [is] fundamental.
- Interview, Coordinator
In short, this term served as a reminder that their work was individualized and personcentered, and indicated a preference for maintaining good relationships between the staff
and those that used the services over strict adherence to any particular strategy. Like in
the psychoeducational and cognitive-behavioral philosophies, in the relational approach it
is important to create environments that are welcoming to people with autism by
engaging them at the relational level, seeking to enter into relationships with them based
on their interests and presupposing that it is possible to do so.
188
Despite the importance of this style of approach for many participants there were
also criticisms. One professional raised the concern that this approach by itself is difficult
to execute professionally. Another referred to it as “pietista,” pitying. Professionals also
raised concerns with the implications of stressing the relational perspective. It could be a
point of contention for those who felt it implied other work was not relational:
It’s not like that, and if you did not enter into relationships, if you do not have a
relations with who you have in front of you, you never teach anything. While the
idea that we on the other hand do not have… do not enter into relationships with
them. We act as conductors or trainers without having (laughs) without being
aware of all the relational-affective part. It cannot be. Because it cannot be.
Because anyway the fact to sit here talking with you, anyway it’s something, no?
If I don’t enter into relationship with the kid, the kid will let you see, he won’t
even respond to you. The fact that they are people, that they are kids that with
some of us don’t work and with others do, and vice versa. This is something that
is difficult to make others understand.
- Interview, Coordinator
In fact I often find myself not very much in agreement with some of my
colleagues because it is obvious that you must have a good relationship with
patient. But this, you owe it and (claps) stop. But you must construct these things
at the level of reality, no? and therefore you absolutely must also do the exercises
and anyway have a method.
- Interview, Psicomotricista
Nonetheless, many participants described this as a particularly Italian approach or an
Italianization of other approaches, and it cannot be undervalued when considering the
introduction of specific protocols in a setting.
Ecologico
Another term that three professionals used to describe their approach was
“ecologico.” In their own words:
Our approach is more – how to say it – it seems to me more ecological, that is...
like… that the language must come in its natural context and not… for… Then
instead of the ABA treatment itself, as a complex treatment I find that it is very
effective for skills, for the autonomy of the child, this is efficiency. But for
language you need to pay attention, more attention.
189
- Interview, Logopedista
Well, [our philosophy is the] creation of a context that we have called ecological
but which means, ecological does not mean “in the countryside,” it means in
harmony, it means that the activities, interventions, attendance of people, all must
be harmonized and each intervention that you do with the people with autism
must be contextualized, eh? Nothing gets proposed that doesn’t enter into this
moment here and this environment. This is the premise. After that, each
intervention and activity that gets proposed must be meaningful and this meaning
must be explained, in ways adapted to that person, a way that can be understood.
Moreover, we maintain that it is very important the take for the subjectivity of the
person with autism and therefore seeking to amplify as much as possible the tools
of communication, each at his level, and understand what is their intrinsic
motivation because this is something that improves the quality of life, also for us.
[….] In the end… doing it together. Like Meltzoff said, the famous phrase, “from
shared actions to shared minds.” That is, therefore working together, one learns to
stay together and maybe also starts to share a thought.
- Interview, Psychiatrist
One professional described this work also as “meta-cognitivo” (meta-cognitive).
Professionals advocating these approaches referenced scholars such as Bob Shalock and
Stanley Greenspan. These approaches focused on creating an environment that was
adapted to people with autism, but based not so much upon the characteristics of autism
but on the characteristics of the individuals themselves and their intrinsic motivations.
Psicomotricità
As mentioned above, the term psicomotricità can refer to a therapy with or
without the prefix “neuro.” This chapter already discussed neuropsicomotricità, which is
close to physical and occupational therapy. How does psicomotricità differ? The term and
practice derive from the French. One psicomotricitsta explains:
Psychomotor therapy derives, culturally, from psychiatry and motor education for
dance, from rhythm, so from spontaneous movement. […] For us the movement is
the vehicle for expression, for the communication, for learning. It’s not an end.
- Interview, Psicomotricista
In psychomotor therapy, the therapist uses games and spontaneous movements as
190
instruments to address communication and cognition difficulties. In the case of autism,
communication is the first step. Psicomotricità may be used for an intervention between a
person with autism and the therapist, or mediated with animal-assisted therapy
(sometimes called Pet Therapy), especially with dogs. In either case, the therapist seeks
to understand the interests of the person with autism and engage them through those
activities.
Well, let’s that with a child with autism, me being a psychomotor therapist, I have
a psychomotor approach, with the child with autism. The fact that there’s an
animal, obviously, makes me change some aspects … but it does not radically
change my way of doing things. When a child enters into the room, I look at
what’s around him, at what he’s interested in, what pulls his attention. Because
especially with a child with autism, you must enter a bit into their world of
interests. Then you try to open them up… to our world.
- Interview, Psychologist
To accomplish this, the therapist might start by simply staying with the child while he or
she interacts with her environment (including animals) and pursues his or her own
interests. Only once the child realizes that the environment is created for him or her and
that the therapist is supportive will the therapist enter into closer contact with the child
and start working on developing specific communication and/or cognitive skills.
Auto-Mutuo-Aiuto
A final philosophy, particularly present in one social/recreational activity group,
was that of auto-mutuo-aiuto (self-help groups). In this model, people with autism
(particularly Asperger’s) can help each other and themselves by sharing their experiences.
This model is also intergenerational – older people with autism can share what they have
learned with younger people with autism, suggesting tools that may help the younger
through experiences the older have already had. One tool of this model is formal
conversation groups, but it can also be exercised in informal settings by introducing
191
people with autism to each other and providing the opportunity for an exchange of
information.
This perspective was not necessarily mutually exclusive with other philosophies,
although it often stands alone. In settings not organized around auto-mutuo-aiuto, I noted
practices that may fall under a similar umbrella, in which people with autism held roles as
tutors or volunteer aides for other people with disabilities (autism and otherwise). In one
setting, staff instructed one ospite in how to help another complete a task. In other, an
ospite requested to volunteer alongside a group of youth that were temporarily helping at
the service, and was able to do so. In these roles, people with autism not only received
assistance, but also gave it.
Specific Approaches
These eight categories represent the overarching philosophies taken towards
autism, which are often combined. Given these overarching categories, the following
paragraphs look at how participants discussed more specific approaches. Table 8 presents
all of the interventions participant named in order of frequency, demonstrating the
relative popularity of each in the world of autism treatment – whether they were
evaluated positively or negatively. Rather than address each approach in detail, this
section focuses on two main themes: (1) the need to remain flexible and avoid, as
practitioners, manifesting the same “rigidity” as the patients with autism;26 and (2) the
contested status of certain activities-based interventions that some considered not
necessarily “vere terapie” (true therapies) or “vero e proprio intervento” (real and true
26
I have made versions of this argument elsewhere, at the Society for Applied Anthropology Annual
Meeting (Cascio 2014b) and an upcoming article in Culture, Medicine & Psychiatry (Cascio 2015a).
192
interventions). More details on professionals’ evaluations of certain therapies will be
raised in the next chapter when they are compared to parents’ evaluations, but a quick
glance at their evaluations can be seen in Table 8.
Table 8: Specific Approaches References
Approach Name
TEACCH
ABA
Medication
Diet
PECS
Alternative and
Augmentative
Communication
Facilitated Communication
Horse Therapy
Doman-Delacato27
Pet Therapy/Animal-Assisted
Therapy
Floortime/DIR28
Abilità Sociale (Social Skills
Groups)
Supplements
Chelation
Art Therapy
Denver Model29
Defeat Autism Now!30
Water-based Therapies
Sports Therapy
Feuerstein31
Dance Therapy
Dolphin Therapy
Social Stories32
27
References in
Interviews and
Fieldnotes
43
43
17
15
13
13
Support Integrate
or
Modify
12
23
7
27
10
2
0
3
0
11
0
13
Reject
11
10
10
9
0
2
0
3
1
6
1
3
11
2
9
3
9
7
3
4
6
3
0
0
6
6
5
5
5
4
4
3
3
3
3
0
0
1
0
0
0
0
1
1
0
0
2
1
4
5
1
3
3
1
2
2
3
4
5
0
0
4
1
1
1
0
1
0
Institutes for the Achievement of Human Potential (2014)
Greenspan Floortime Approach (2014)
29
Rogers and Dawson (2009)
30
Autism Research Institute (2010a; 2010b) and Silverman (2012)
31
The Feuerstein Institute (2014)
32
Gray and Garand (1993)
28
7
9
5
12
2
0
193
Approach Name
Sonrise33
Mindfulness34
Theatre as Therapy
Borghese Logopedia35
Hyperbaric Chamber
Pivotal Response Training36
Verbal Behaviors37
Homeopathy
Immunostimulants
References in
Interviews and
Fieldnotes
3
2
2
2
2
1
1
1
1
Support Integrate
or
Modify
0
1
2
0
1
1
0
0
0
0
1
0
1
0
0
0
0
0
Reject
2
0
0
2
2
0
0
1
1
The most frequently referenced specific interventions were TEACCH and ABA.
Participants often discussed these approaches together.
TEACCH is an educational and service provision strategy for people with autism
developed by Eric Schopler, Gary Mesibov, and colleagues in North Carolina. The
acronym sometimes stands for “treatment and education of autism and communicationshandicapped children” but also encompasses the key philosophies of the program:
Teaching, Expanding, Appreciating, Collaborating and Cooperating, and Holistic. The
term TEACCH refers most specifically the University of North Carolina TEACCH
Autism Program, founded in 1972. This system of community regional centers is
supported by the State of North Carolina and coordinators activities of diagnosis,
training, education, and employment. The Program also provides training to autism
professionals around the world on the principles of structured TEACCHing. Several
books and manuals detail these strategies, but in short TEACCH supports understanding
33
Kaufman and Kaufman (1995)
Mitchell (2008)
35
Borghese (2007)
36
Koegel (2011)
37
Sundberg and Michael (2001)
34
194
what they call the 'culture of autism' – the specific characteristics (both strengths and
challenges) of people with autism, individualized person- and family-centered planning,
careful structuring of the physical environment to facilitate the learning of people with
autism, and most specifically visual supports such as pictographic icons identifying
components of the environment and in agendas for people with autism (University of
North Carolina at Chapel Hill School of Medicine 2014).
Applied Behavior Analysis (ABA) refers to approaches derived from the work of
Ivar Lovaas who used Discrete Trial Training to teach people with autism through one
and one interaction with a therapist. Many participants described this as behaviorist or
cognitive-behaviorist. Some complained that ABA made children robotic and did not
sufficiently generalize, but modified ABA to enhance generalization. Participants also
referenced specific therapies based on ABA such as Pivotal Response Training and Verbal
Behaviors. TEACCH was generally considered more cognitive-behavioral whereas ABA
was considered the more strictly behaviorist part, such as one-on-one work in which
people with autism worked on small tasks in a distraction free environment with a single
therapist to refine and maintain motor or cognitive skills. Most ABA practitioners suggest
intensive therapy of 20-40 hours a week, which many participants found unfeasible for
children in Italy, who by law should be going to inclusive schools most of the day. The
majority of participants either supported these methods or integrated them with others
under the umbrella of psychoeducational and cognitive-behavioral therapies supported by
the national guidelines and the relational approaches that were important in many
settings:
Yes, more or less I think these are the two, that is, the TEACCH approach and the
dynamic-relational approach, that for sure. All in a very flexible frame, in my
195
opinion. [By] flexible I mean that we don’t (stick) to the TEACCH manual in such
a rigid way but we try also to gather also hints from outside the TEACCH
philosophy, in short, because there’s not just TEACCH.
Interview, Educatore
The approach used [here] is the psychocognitive approach that is very broad and
anyway we chose, how to say it, to consider the classic approaches therefore
TEACCH, ABA, only for the part that seemed to work, therefore within the
psychoeducational approach, we chose only those things that seemed useful for
our type of intervention that is so particular because we don’t have a [physical]
structure. [We don’t have one.] And therefore how to say, our work is itinerant
labor. Therefore we definitely considered the structuring of the time, the
environment, the activities, therefore the cleanliness of the context which is very
important, the consequences of actions. Another thing very is important and... And
this then clearly helps us give them a predictable, known world.
- Interview, Pedagogue
Here in Italy, there is, people tend to wed themselves to an approach like “or you
do TEACCH, or you do ABA, or you do...” no? And everything is a little, I don’t
know what to say, categorized, can I say it like that? Instead I have an approach, a
very broad training for which I seek to take all the approaches that I know that
might serve for this particular child. Therefore some strategies that come from
ABA, some strategies that come from TEACCH, strategies that come from DIR;
etc.
- Interview, Psychiatrist
Particularly with ABA, participants would stress the need to use it carefully.
Some of the language used in these quotes about integrating the approaches
anticipates the criticisms of psychoeducational and cognitive-behavioral approaches as
well, which focus on rigidity. A teacher explains how she faces these concerns from
parents:
We wanted to give the message to the families because often they tell us
(mimicking angry parent versus calm teacher) “Oh, but you do TEACCH!” “Yes,
OK, that too.” “But in TEACCH there are just the little boxes.” “No, it’s not just
that.” “But TEACCH means that you are rigid.” “No, it’s not just this. Here we
use the structured education, the behaviors and behavior education, that is, there
are other things. Certainly it is all inserted into the TEACCH philosophy.” “But
no, it’s the TEACCH method.” “No, it’s not a method, it’s a philosophy, a
program, that’s a different thing.”
-Interview, Teacher
196
But even practitioners who use these approaches can associate with a risk of rigidity. A
psicomotricista explains her hesitation about FloorTime:
Well, I like, regardless of the fact that it is a method, I get a lot out of the
FloorTime method, but because the FloorTime method, like all the methods, has
some precise rules, that you must – you know? – you must stay 20 minutes with
your son at home, then 10 minutes no, then another 20 minutes – like 8 times in a
day giving 20 minutes of activity and interaction with the child. At the beginning,
on paper, it seems too rigid a thing. Then I saw the filmstrips of the children…
when there were adequate parents in effect it is much more useful. Well I may
wouldn’t do 20 minutes eight times a day, that is, it seems a little rigid like that.
But I can understand that it’s, how to say, an incentive to a parent to dedicate
some time to the child, for play and activities and interaction to open a little his
interests, his range of interests. Well, probably, it’s given like a medicine, a recipe,
because the families need to have such precise instructions. But, really, let’s say,
the instructions on how to approach it and what type of activities to do are very
useful. Well, this I like, so I tell you, I am really wedded to the principle of
starting from their interests which is… is written immediately into FloorTime,
really that, in my opinion, is the winning card.
- Interview, psicomotricista
In this narration, the therapist explains her initial hesitation in hearing about FloorTime,
reading about it “on paper.” She read the precise rules, down to the minute, as very rigid.
However, she saw the intervention as effective and the precise rules as perhaps necessary
for some parents to do it correctly. However, she explains that she might not use it so
rigidly herself. Other professionals explain their own perspectives on the risk of rigidity
and how they might integrate or modify TEACCH to better suit their needs:
If someone says “I do TEACCH, only TEACCH”, it’s like saying I read only this,
well this becomes – however, this, in my opinion, is not only for TEACCH. In our
life it goes a bit like this for everything. Well the risk at times is to talk always
with the same people while it is important in my opinion to be open to other types
of experiences, integrating them [....] In my opinion, the disadvantage [of
TEACCH which I use] can be to remain [...] little flexible, in short. Already our
kids are so little flexible! (laughing)
- Interview, Neuropsichiatra
In my opinion, for example, both ABA and TEACCH at their base have good
ideas. Even I use their techniques, but in a much less rigid way, that is, the risk of
this techniques is to create a sort of rigidity therefore teaching in sequence is fine
197
but I risk that the guy because more rigid, therefore if you do not do the sequence
right, it doesn’t go well. It’s the only thing that I don’t share much with this
method. That is, I share the thought, I’ll give you an example. Um, they [ABA
practitioners] to teach you to set the table, for example they put a green rectangle,
a yellow circle, and a red rectangle. The guy must place the placement over the
green rectangle, the napkin on the red rectangle, and the place over the yellow
circle. We, I, in order to teach my kids how to set the table, I use the same method
but in a more natural way. For which for example I put the placemat and napkin
down, the guy knows that he has to put the silverware over the napkin and the
plate over the placemat. Therefore the same technique but seen in a less rigid and
more natural way.
- Interview, Therapist
Accusations of rigidity were not limited to TEACCH and ABA, but extended also
to the use of special diets for autism. Special diets are most associated with the movement
Defeat Autism Now! (DAN) of the Autism Research Institute (ARI). In 2011, ARI
dropped the name DAN, but it is still in use colloquially and was mentioned by this name
in four professionals’ interviews. This approach is associated with a casein-free glutenfree diet, the use of various dietary supplements, and the removal of toxic metals via the
process of chelation. Many professionals objected to chelation as dangerous, as reflected
in the scientific literature. For the diets and supplements, professionals objected at times
due to lack of evidence of the intervention as a whole or lack of evidence that a specific
child had need of it. This latter group objected when people advocated special diets as a
treatment for autism, arguing instead that it should be a treatment for gastrointestinal
problems and that children needed to be tested for those problems, not assumed to have
them due to their autism. Other professionals objected to the diets and supplements only
in so far as they were considered a cure, given that they saw autism as a lifelong
condition but not a disease from which one could be cured. When articulating these
objections, two professionals drew still on the language of rigidity, for example:
I believe that a good diet effectively helps everyone, but a rigid diet, taking away
198
a series of ingredients, then risks provoking other problems due to a lack of some
principle ones, look, therefore these are effectively interventions a little far from
our thought.
- Interview, Educatore
A third similarly complained that the diet was too “extreme” and therefore “against the
nature of the person,” but did not use the language of rigidity.
Participants raised concerned of rigidity also for Doman-Delacato, a motor
therapy involving a series of specific exercises that parents are expected to do at home.
This could include “sensory bombardment” of the person with autism with lights and
noises, or exercises to move the body in a certain way. Many professionals rejected the
approach or spoke of it disparagingly in passing. They objected to it as outdated or
scientifically unsupported. One professional acknowledged that it may work with other
populations, but not people with autism. The criticism of rigidity appeared in criticisms
of Doman-Delacato as well. The psicomotricista who favored FloorTime above continues
on to say:
FloorTime, really that, in my opinion, is the winning card. The others, Delacato,
TEACCH, are too rigid. However, with some children... They work. I would start,
however, with very small children, from a more flexible model, and at a certain
age I would start with something more rigid, the moment in which the child needs
to be directed. But from very young [...] I am more in favor of being destructured, opening, making them flexible, because this is our reality. If I do not
make them more rigid, when they are already rigid. I want first to try instead to
mellow them out.
- Interview, Psicomotricista
Professionals criticized a range of specific approaches – both those they found
useful and those they rejected – for being too rigid. Just as participants sought to adapt
the environment to better suit people with autism through structured time and space and
the use of visual supports, they also rejected overly strict adherence to particular methods
based on the characteristics of autism. Given that many participants saw rigidity as an
199
important characteristic of autism, it is no coincidence that rigidity is also the term they
use to reject approaches to autism.
There is one group major of therapies that this chapter has not yet addressed, and
those are the therapies based on pursuit of the arts, sports, and activities elsewhere
classified as recreational. Professionals mentioned specifically art therapy, dance therapy,
music therapy, theatre as therapy, therapies involving various animals, and therapies
involving various sports. Some professionals advocated certain approaches. As seen
above, animal-assisted strategies can be used in practicing psicomotricitista. Animals
help facilitate the therapy by providing a motivation for the child, being easier to interact
with, or engaging their attention when sitting with the therapist alone would be boring.
Many participants also referenced Ippoterapia (Horse Therapy), which works similarly.
Horse therapy was initially used for people with neuromotor problems and brain injuries,
but was later used to facilitate therapies for people with autism similarly to the dogs. One
participant also expressed interest in the potential of music therapy to help with language
development late in life. While many services for adolescents and adults offered them,
the majority of professionals expressed hesitation about these activity-based therapies.
For example:
Then there are instead maybe pleasant experiences, helpful and nice, for the
children, but that get sold as therapies, no? I don’t know… pet therapy, music
therapy, horse therapy… that are good things! … But it isn’t yet known how much
they are therapy. Pleasant experiences, that the children do happily, but we don’t
yet know if they can be thought of as true and proper interventions… therapeutic.
- Interview, Neuropsichiatra
As with neuropsicomotricità and logopedia, professionals worried that these approaches
were not sufficiently tailored to people with autism, but might work in certain cases.
Then there can be other things, I don’t know, Horse Therapy or Dolphin Therapy,
200
it’s not that they aren’t useful, but they are not specific for autism. Well if this kid
likes the water, likes animals, and then is in an environment that is stimulating and
motivating for him, and that in this environment obviously he can gain some
social communication skills, welcome. But he can’t just do that. That is, this isn’t
an intervention specifically for autism. Then if the parents want to provide it, in
short, even this type of experience, why not?
- Interview, Neuropsichiatra
The services at which I conducted participant-observation – the three Day
Centers, two Residential Centers, and two Social/Recreational Groups – used a variety of
philosophies and approaches. Most used some combination of
psychoeducational/psychocognitive, (cognitive)-behavioral, and relational philosophies,
prioritizing each aspect in varying degrees. One residential service used an ecological
approach. One Social/Recreational Group followed more of an auto-mutuo-aiuto model.
Although these services were in many ways very different – not only in that some were
day services, others residential, and still others social or recreational but also in their size,
their specific goals, and the tone of the day – what they had in common despite their
different philosophies was their focus on created spaces well suited to the needs and
characteristics of people with autism. Staff referred to the people with autism who
attended the services using various terms: utenti (service users), ospiti (guests), residenti
(residents), atleti (athletes), soci (members), or simply ragazzi (guys).38 Throughout this
section, I will use the term ospiti. Although certainly reflecting its own set of ideologies
and assumptions, using the term ospite avoids some of the baggage of the term “service
38
The term ragazzi does refer specifically to younger people, and staff used it to refer also to ospiti who
were older than the staff, even elderly. Only about very rare occasion did staff state that the term ragazzo
might be inappropriate for ospiti past a certain age. However, I would argue that the practice of referring to
the ospiti as ragazzi is not inherently infantilizing or disrespectful, because the term is most certainly also
used among adults of varying ages as they talk to each other, calling out to a group of colleagues
“ragazzi/e!”, not unlike the English expression “hey, you guys!” I have seen it happen both with superiors
addressing subordinates and vice versa. Though an age-based term, it is perhaps the broadest and most
neutral.
201
user,” at least in UK English, as unpacked by McLaughlin (2009). The term was
commonly used by staff can include people with autism across the whole range of
services.
Several specific approaches, tools, and instruments were used in these settings.
Commonly referenced specific approaches were TEACCH, ABA, FloorTime, CAA, and
PECS. Tools under and in addition to these approaches include behavioral techniques,
diaries, daily agendas, task agendas, and other communication tools.
Some sites stressed cognitive-behavioral or behavioral techniques either as
general practice or for use in specific cases. Behavioral techniques include prompting,
fading, shaping, and reinforcements (but rarely if ever punishments). The notion of a
system of prompting, fading, and shaping derives perhaps most famously from applied
behavior analysis, and refer to specific techniques used in teaching new behaviors and
skills. Staff at one day center explained that there were many types of prompts. Staff
could “prompt” a ospite to accomplish a task verbally (saying instructions), physically
(manually guiding someone through a task), or gesturally (pointing, without words). One
day, I accompanied the ospiti to a ceramics workshop where I learned about these
prompts hands on. In a large open space lined with shelves of completed and in progress
projects, everyone sat around a table working under the instruction of an art teacher and
sometimes the guidance of staff. As my fieldnote documents:
[A staff member] told me to go help [this woman] use two hands. I
nodded and picked up the stool. I sat between [her and another ospite] and
asked [following the example of conversations I had heard between her
and the maestra during a previous workshop]: dove l’altra mano? Poi:
metti la. Poi: ancora. [Where is the other hand? Then I said: put it here.
Then I said: Again]. [The staff member] walked over and told me:
“prompt fisica” [physical prompt]. I talked while moving her hand
202
towards the creta. [The staff member] showed me to stand behind her and
scaccia la creta [kneed the clay] with my hands over hers.
The staff member told me to use only the physical prompt today, because there had
already been so much conversation that words fa confusion (create confusion). He
explained to me that it was best to use “il meno possibile” (as little as possible) amount
of contact during a physical prompt. Reinforcements are factors that occur during the
desired behavior, whereas punishments occur after the desired behavior. Reinforcements
can be positive in the sense of something added or negative in the sense of something
removed. As an example, one of the most common reinforcements were social. Staff
would give the ospite social recognition (such as saying bravo) as long as they continued
a positive behavior but remove the social interaction when the ospite engaged in behavior
the staff did not desire. For example, staff might intentionally not respond to requests that
were not made properly, even if they understood what the ospite wanted, because they
wanted to encourage the ospite to ask in a particular way (for example, for some ospite
they may want to encourage them to ask verbally, for others to ask with PECS). The
implicit underlying tenant here is that, contrary to popular beliefs that people with autism
are not social, all people including people with autism crave social interaction.
Other forms of reinforcement were systems of incentive and reward, such as
educational contracts. Educational contracts are agreements (dos and do nots) between
ospiti and staff regarding particular behaviors or activities, written for and often by ospiti
in a combination of words and images. The ospiti agree to certain behaviors (dos and
don’ts), and in exchange receive a desired item, activity, or privilege, sometimes
immediately but sometimes after collecting a series of tokens to exchange for a reward.
Staff often worked hard to make the rewards appropriate to the ospiti's interests and
203
motivations through initial meetings with prospective ospiti and their parents and
ongoing conversations with the ospiti themselves. For example, many people with autism
that I met had a strong interest in cars, so rewards might include seeing a staff member’s
new car and asking questions about it, or the center acquiring a new automobile magazine
for their library. At one Social/Recreational Group, staff stressed how contracts are
written in the first person, such that the ospite is stating that he or she promises to fulfill
it. The staff asked the ospite to sign the contract, making it a pact between ospite and
staff. While most people I met with contracts talked about them only when staff referred
them to the contract, two women showed a lot of interest in sharing their contracts with
me. One woman invited me to see hers during my second visit at her Residential Center –
a house in a residential area somewhat outside the central city proper, with color-coded
rooms, visual guides on cabinets and drawers, and visual agendas posted near the
entrance. I visited on a weekend, so I asked her ‘come andava tua settimana?’ (how was
your week?) She told me it was good, and that she had eaten a particularly favored food –
an allusion to the premio from her educational contract. She then asked me, ‘c’è l’hai il
contratto?’ (do you have a contract?). When I told her I didn’t have one like she did, she
told me to come with her and that she would show me hers. I followed her and she
showed me the paper, describing the gist of it. A similar interaction occurred my first day
at one of the Day Centers. Almost as soon as we had been introduced, one of the ospiti
left the main room where I was waiting and went to her desk in the other room, retrieving
and bringing to me a binder full of social stories and contracts, written in the first person
and using a combination of images and words. She walked me through them page by
page, not only reading them to me but asking me to explain the rules too.
204
Another (cognitive)-behavioral technique used is structured teaching. Day and
Residential Centers that drew from ABA and TEACCH used some version of structured
learning. Although the vocabulary may differ from place to place, the principle was the
same: ospiti learned tasks through rapporto uno-uno or lavoro in due in which a staff
member directly teachers the ospite a task and repeats a cognitive, manual, or other task
until he or she can do it not only alone, but alone without anxiety.39 Then the ospite does
the same activity by him or herself, called lavaro al tavolo, lavoro in autonomia or lavoro
indipendente. Drawing from TEACCH, this work is extremely structured not only in that
the task itself is structured and the same from working with staff to working alone, but
that in all tasks are lined up on a shelf to the ospite’s left, often but not always in little
boxes (the famous scatolette referenced above). Ospite work on uncompleted tasks from
the left, and place completed tasks on the right. This structure makes it very clear what
tasks need to be done and when the work session will be over. Each site that used this
method did so in a slightly different way, though the general structure was still the same,
working from left to right. Some centers had actual little boxes with the tasks inside,
whereas others had more of a variety of items. In one center, identically sized boxes lined
the bookshelves on the wall, each with a label in the form of something from the inside of
the box taped on the outside (for example, if it was sorting beads by color, a bead would
be taped to the outside). In other centers, tasks were mounted on cardboard instead of
inside boxes (for example, one cardboard base might have a box full of cubes and a
container with a cube-shaped hole for an insertion task). Many of these scatolette were
39
Similar to discrete trial training, but participants never called it by this term. One defined this very
specific process as “ABA,” which is associated with the technique of discrete trial training.
205
constructed using recycled or donated materials, highlighting the economic difficulty of
this work. Some centers had dedicated rooms for this type of work, for the ospiti to work
in a quieter, emptier space. Others had work stations lined up against the wall, created a
quiet and clean space within the larger room where ospiti spent non-work time in other
spaces, writing agendas, exercising, engaging in free time activities such as puzzles and
reading, and so on.
Several written tools were visibly important in all of the settings. Day centers
used diaries, agendas, and educational contracts. Diaries facilitated communication
between the center and the homes of the ospiti, allowing staff to write messages to
parents and caretakers and vice versa. Sometimes staff wrote them alone, other times in
direct consultation with the ospiti. For example, at one day center I often sat in one of the
leisure time rooms in the afternoon while staff filled out the diaries. The ospiti read
magazines, did puzzles, played games, or listened to music during this time. The staff
wrote in each diary, sometimes asking ‘che cosa hai mangiato’ (what did you eat?) or
‘cosa devo scrivere alla tua mamma’ (what should I write to your mother?). Some ospite
wrote their own diaries to take home. Parents wrote back, and staff checked them daily to
see if the parents had anything to note or share. Daily agendas outlined the activities of
the day using objects, images, text, or combinations thereof. They were constructed in
several different styles according to each person's needs. Some agendas were displayed as
timelines on the wall, others in books, and others through the exchange of singular
objects or images. There were images alone for those who did not know how to read, and
images accompanied by words for those who did. Some images were photos, some PECS
(or PECS modified after discussing with the ospiti what images made most sense for
206
them), and some the ospite’s own drawings. While PECS were extremely common, there
were also professionals who strongly objected to the use of iconic languages because, like
spoken language, they were symbolic systems that would be more difficult for someone
with autism to understand. Such professionals stressed the use of photographs instead,
often taken from the ospite’s point of view. In some cases, staff prepared the agendas for
the ospiti to use. At one Day Center, I arrived every morning before most of the ospiti and
stayed with the staff in the main room, set up somewhat like a living room with couches,
a small table, and a television. At the small table, staff and I created agendas by looking
at the day’s schedule, and then finding the appropriate PECS icons in a binder and
placing them on each person’s posted schedule. At others, ospiti prepared their own
agendas with supervision. For example, I regularly arrived at one day center while ospite
were completing this process and their general accoglienza. I walked into the dining
room where ospiti were seated around tables, finishing their biscotti and tea. For many,
staff made the agendas – images attached to a timeline with Velcro - showing them to the
ospite as they did. For others, however, the ospite did most of the construction of the
agenda. I documented the following fieldnote:
[Ospite] did his agenda with [Staff member]. [Staff member] said “facciamo la
nostra agenda” (let’s do our agenda) – using the plural. [Staff member] asked
[ospite] what day it was and about the weather. [Ospite] got the date wrong […]
and [staff member] corrected him [….] Then [staff member] said the activities. At
“cucina” (cooking) [ospite] asked “cosa cuciniamo?” (what are we cooking?)
[Staff member]: non lo so, una sorpresa. Cosa cuciniamo? Vediamo cosa abbiamo
comprato alle spese. Ti dico tra un po’. (I don’t know, it’s a surprise. What are we
cooking? Let’s see what we bought. I’ll tell you in a bit).
While not all ospiti were as active in the construction of their agendas, these examples
show that constructing agendas (and writing diaries) served not only as administrative
tools, but also teaching tools and social communication events in which ospiti were
207
actively engaged.
In addition to daily agendas, some Day Centers, Residential Centers, and
Social/Recreational Groups used step-by-step task agendas for specific tasks such as
physical exercises, cooking, setting the table, or changing clothes in the locker room.
Beyond individual agendas, several other communication tools were used across
Day Centers, Residential Centers, and Social/Recreational Groups. Some centers used
PECS for specific communication tasks, such as asking for items at lunch. One service
also used facilitated communication by using the computer to converse with some of the
ospiti. Written, visual, and tactile tools helped make clear and predictable environments.
Written tools included posted agendas of the entire day, week, or month at day and
Residential Centers, and lists of rules for participation at Social/Recreational Groups.
Visual tools included labels (PECS or photographic) on doors identifying what activities
were done therein, and on coat hooks and lockers identifying to whom they belonged.
Some of the residential structures additionally employed color coding of the rooms to
help the ospiti find their own room even more easily, including colored stripes on the
floor. Several services used visual tools for specific activities, such as laminated sheets
papers laid on the ground outlining an obstacle course in printouts of footprints, arrows,
and figures of various exercises; or a visual list of whose turn it was to serve themselves
for lunch. Tactile tools included toys or exercise equipment passed between participants
in turn-taking activities such as games and repetitions of obstacle courses that clarify
whose turn it is, or tokens to be deposited at the end of the course to indicate the number
of repetitions.
In additional to specific techniques for day to day work with ospiti, several
208
instruments helped staff collaborate amongst themselves and create individualized plans
for each ospite. These instruments included individualized educational plans (generally
called programmi educativi individualizzati or PEI), evaluation tools, various types of
team meetings, and tools to introduce ospite to volunteers and new staff.
Most centers organized individuals' annual activities and goals using programmi
educative individualizzati (PEI), sometimes called a progetto di vita (life project) to avoid
the scholastic associations of the former term. The exception was the one
social/recreational activity group whose participants came solely voluntarily without
(necessarily) the planning and oversight of staff. PEI revolved around certain skills areas
such as: cognitive prerequisites, socializing, communication, motor skills, personal care
skills, cognitive skills, deductive logic skills, mathematics, reading, and writing, spatialtemporal skills, autonomy, independent work, and structured free time, and provided
discrete goals within these domains. PEI are shared with families and, when applicable
and possible with schools, social services, and other staff who work with the ospite in
other contexts. At one site, staff also create a synthesis of the PEI, a very brief version,
for the volunteers.
PEI are always tightly linked to various types of evaluation forms (schede di
valutazione). Such forms allowed staff to monitor ospite progress over the course of the
year. Places that used behavioral techniques would track for each ospite each day if he or
she had accomplished or not, and if so if it was in autonomia (autonomously), con
imitazione (through imitation), prompt gestuale (with a gesturing prompt), prompt fisica
(with a physical prompt), or prompt verbale (with a verbal prompt). Others might
operationalize skills as being present, absent, or emergent. The specific type of form
209
varied from site to site – for example, the social/recreational group that held athletic
practices tracked athletic skills, whereas others did not. However, all such forms allowed
staff to track individuals’ progress over time and adjust goals accordingly.
Most services routinely held one or more of the following types of team meetings
to help staff work together. In equipe meetings, an interdisciplinary group of staff meet to
discuss how to run activities and address specific concerns. In supervisioni, staff present
cases to a supervisor who provides feedback and suggests new strategies. In passaggi di
consegno, the staff from one shift reports to the staff of the next shift how the day has
gone, ospite by ospite.
One social/recreational activity group used several tools and strategies to get to
know new youth and share information with staff and volunteers. First, there is a meeting
with the parents that is used to create an “identity card” – a one-page information sheet
for volunteers and staff about each ospite. It is written in first person with a photo in the
center and bubbles explaining “Who I am...” “How I communicate...” “What activities I
succeed at doing...” “What I like to do...” “What I do not like to do...” and a two-part
section on difficulties phrased as “At times it happens that...” and “what to do [about it].”
The Identity Card effectively summarizes each individual so that new volunteers
understand how to interact with each ospite respecting his or her unique set of abilities
and difficulties. Staff also wrote a summary that was distributed to educatori addressing
abilities and difficulties, and also allergies to be aware of when planning snacks.
Life Cycle
In my role as a participant-observer at Day Centers, I had particular difficulty at
first understanding the big picture in terms of the goal of the weekly activities. The
210
adolescents and adults who attended the Day Centers worked on specific tasks to improve
their cognitive and motor skills or laboratories to improve domestic or professional skills.
Staff talked about vocational training, but admitted that they were hesitant to think many
of the youth in their care would ever be able to take jobs, even part time, because they
needed so much support. This lack of optimism did not apply to all people on the
spectrum and is largely a function of the fact that the Day Centers dedicated to autism
were also administratively dedicated to people with higher levels of support needs as
determined by the Local Health Boards (see Chapter 2). Nonetheless, I found myself
wondering: what is the point? Why do these activities?
The answer came to me one day in an unrelated conversation. During a scheduled
stroll outside, staff were reflecting on their relationships with the parents of the people
who attended the day center. I casually asked if everyone still lived with their parents,
and the staff member told me yes, adding that they will ultimately live “in communità,”
meaning a structured residential community and not in their own apartments. The staff
member explained that there are two types of communities - residenziale ed assistenziale.
In residenziale they do activities and have a better quality of life. In assistenziale, they do
not. The staff worked to avoid that the adults with whom they worked would have to go
in assistenziale – tutto il giorno in letto con pantaloni (all day in bed with diapers). The
staff member explained: if we can teach them to eat by themselves, to dress themselves,
to wash themselves, they can go in residenziale and have a better quality of life.
The work staff were doing at Day Centers, Residential Centers, and
Social/Recreational Groups was consistently focused on a life cycle approach.
Particularly as this study focused on older adolescents and younger adults, issues related
211
to the transition to adulthood played a central role in all activities. Anthropologists
classically define adolescence as the “time between puberty and the assumption of full
adult roles” (Whiting, Burbank, and Ratner 1994). This definition has been extended
through 30 years of research, and in some contexts also includes discussion of another
life stage between adolescence and adulthood called “emerging adulthood” (Arnett 2000;
Arnett 2004). What anthropological definitions of adolescence and other life stages
contribute most to academic understanding of these life stages is their flexibility. Rather
than relying on age-based defintions (e.g, the age of legal majority - 18 in both the U.S.
and Italy), anthropological definitions rely on locally determined stage-based definitions
(e.g., activities such as puberty rites, marriage, childbearing, employment, or other locally
determined markers). While the anthropological definition allows for a broad range of
activities that might constitute a transition to adulthood, the presence of a developmental
disability challenges this definition because people with developmental disabilities are
often excluded from whatever locally determined activities mark this transition.
One interpretation of this challenge is that people with developmental disabilities
never become adults. Certainly there is a problem of the infantilization of people with
developmental disabilities – some parents and professionals complained about it – but it
did not dominate my field experience. The purpose of this section is to highlight and
explicate professionals’ self-aware attention to the life cycle of people with autism.
Staff easily recognized the difference between an infant, child, adolescent, and
adult with autism and discussed these life stages as points along a developing continuum
– at times the same and at times different from people without developmental disabilities.
For example, one participant said of small children, “anche i bambini autistici sono
212
bambini” (even the children with autism are children)” who have childlike wonder for the
world, even if that wonder is focused on different objects than a child without autism.
Another pointed out that children with autism who do not want to go to school are acting
just like children without autism, who generally do not want to go to school either.
During my participant-observation, staff repeatedly noted the particular needs of
the adolescence and youth with whom they worked. Staff explained that because they
were young, the youth needed to “scaricare energia,” work off their energy. Participants
made similar statements for children: that they need to get up and move frequently. Many
people also stressed that adolescence is a difficult time for everyone, but especially for
adolescents with disabilities. Sexuality becomes an especially prominent issue during
adolescence. Peer group relations also become more important, and professionals who
worked with social/recreational activity groups stressed the importance of a place where
people with developmental disabilities could spend leisure time with their peers just like
anyone else.
There were issues specific to adulthood as well. When forming a new self-help
group, the organizers stressed the goal to have it be for adults because adolescents are
competitive, and because they talk about topics like school and their parents which are
less interesting to adults. Adulthood is also associated with responsibility:
We have a whole series of workshops at times […] really to permit the kid to
grow, to become and adult, to take responsibility for some chores that are related
to his wellbeing because it is important that a kid, adolescent, then an adult, is
able to care for himself.
- Interview, Educatore
Professionals also criticized others for using childlike materials for adults and stressed
age appropriate activities, including age-appropriate books to read, television shows to
213
watch, outings to go on, and decorations for domestic and therapeutic spaces. Once at a
presentation to parents of videos from a residential center, a professional pointed out that
adults go on outings accompanied, but without hands held because “they aren’t children
and they aren’t spouses.” Furthermore, staff at times also motivated the adolescents and
adults with whom they worked by pointing out their age and telling them to act a certain
way accordingly based on age-related expectations. Once a professional even used me as
an example, pointing out that we are age-mates and so I could be a role model. While
telling someone to “act your age” can itself be a form of infantilization (it is children who
need to be told what to do), I read this admonition as holding youth with autism to life
cycle expectations and as evidence that people with autism were not seen as perpetual
children or perpetual adolescents, but as developing and transitioning through a full range
of life stages with distinct age-based characteristics and intervention needs.
An alternative interpretation to the problem developmental disabilities raises to
the anthropological definition of adolescence is therefore that of defining “full adult
roles” in the presence of developmental disability. While this goal was not necessarily
important to professionals who worked with younger children, those who were working
with residential options for people with autism reflected on it quite explicitly. As one
participant explains, “Quando il soggetto diventa adulta, secondo me, ha diritto di avere
quelle cose che [l’età] adulta prevede: una casa e un lavoro. (When the person becomes
an adult, in my opinion, he has the right to have these things that adulthood forecasts: a
house and a job).” Similarly, I attended a lecture in which a neuropsychiatrist in the
audience stood and stressed: ‘Gli adulti hanno diritti di vievere da adulti. Adulti a loro
modo ma comunque adulti. Uscire da casa. L’adulto vive fuori di casa, anche l’adulto
214
malato, a qualisasi livello di sviluppo. (Adults have the right to live as adults. Adults in
their own way but nonetheless adults. Going out from the house. An adult lives out of the
house [of his or her parents], even a sick adult, at whatever developmental level.)’
Though a discussion of the appropriate use of the world “malato” (sick) to describe
someone with autism followed, the point was well-received – people have a right to live
in their own space, outside the natal home, at whatever developmental level. Participants
saw it as inappropriate both that adults with autism would live with their parents (like
children) and that they would go into assisted living communities with elders several
decades older than them. These professionals strove to empower people with autism to
access full adult roles through a rights-based discourse.
A rights-based perspective – access to adult roles is a right – can inform the
anthropological definition of adolescence and adulthood. Adulthood can be seen as an
assemblage of roles and materials to which all individuals, as they age chronologically,
have a right. The timing of this chronological aging can certainly vary cross-culturally
and does not simply fall back on age-based definitions. As one participant was often
quick to point out, ‘18 is too young for independence’ even if ‘maybe in the United States
they leave home at 18.’ A rights-based approach sees adulthood as a basic set of
characteristics to which individuals with disabilities have a right within an age range
parallel to that of typically developing peers.
The life cycle approach is also related to intergenerational relations. Professionals
referred to the problems of later life for people with autism by a set phrase, “dopo di noi”
(after us) – referring to after parents had aged past the ability to care for their adult
children with autism, or had passed on. Parents, as we will see in the next chapter, used
215
this or the similar term “quando non ci saremo più” (when we won’t be here anymore). In
attempting to give people with autism opportunities for independent living as young and
middle-aged adults, professionals spoke about the need for “distacco” (detachment) from
the natal home. This process could even begin at adolescence, a life stage it which one
participant noted it was important to begin passing time in peer groups and distancing
oneself from the family. The goal of distacco was even more pronounced in residential
settings:
This is a place that has the goal of allowing a gradual detachment from the
families because, if we’re speaking of adulthood, our guys will not be able to
following other goals of those without disabilities to graduate, get married, and,
except for two or chases that maybe one day will be able to live in the area near
here, most of them will live here because they would be in an institution if [this
service] did not exist. Therefore we say that the main goal for the families is to
favor a detachment.
- Interview, Psychiatrist
This gradual detachment ideally created a greater sense of security for parents to see that
their children have established adult lives – independent form the natal home but with
support.
Conclusion
This chapter has addressed research question 1, “how is autism conceptualized in
Italy,” through two basic and interrelated questions: “What is autism?” and “How should
autism be managed?” In terms of how autism is conceptualized, it shows that
professionals saw autism as a constellation of symptoms in line with the DSM-IV, plus
behavioral problems, rigidity, sensory and perception problems, and special strengths.
Half saw autism as a qualitatively different way of being in the world. The way
professionals defined autism related to how they argued autism should be managed.
Professionals favored treatments and interventions tailored for people with autism based
216
on their characteristics, under various philosophical umbrellas but all favoring structuring
the environment to help people with autism learn rather than fitting people with autism
into a hostile world.
Three additional findings emerge from professionals’ narratives that further
anthropological theory and the anthropology of autism. Firstly, related to
conceptualizations of autism, the removal of Asperger’s Syndrome from the DSM-5 may
qualify it as a “transient” or “niche” diagnosis as it has been rolled into “autism with need
of less intensive support.” It remains to be seen if the term Asperger’s will remain salient
outside the medical community, but insights from this fieldwork (which took place during
the period in which the DSM-5 was published) suggest that Asperger’s may appropriately
be categorized as a “niche” diagnosis (Cascio 2013). Secondly, combing findings on what
autism is and how it should be managed, professionals used the language of “rigidity”
when talking about both a characteristic of autism and as a criticism of poorly suited
intervention programs. This rhetorical overlap may serve to bridge a perceived gap
between people with and without autism (Cascio 2015a). Thirdly, relating to how autism
should be managed, I argue that a rights-based approach to the life cycle of people with
autism can challenge and improve the anthropological definition of adolescence and its
conclusion of “full adult roles” by defining adulthood as an amalgamation of materials
and roles to which all individuals have a right based on the standard of their peers.
Professionals’ discussions of their work differentiate between children, adolescents, and
adults, and stress appropriate materials and rhythms for each life stage (Cascio 2015b).
As the next chapter shows, parents share many of these goals and ideas.
This chapter sets the stage for understanding the professional world with which
217
parents and people with autism interact, while contributing the anthropology of
biomedical systems and psychological anthropology with respect to the life cycle.
218
Chapter 5: Parents’ Perspectives
Chapter Overview
This chapter presents data on the perspectives and experiences of parents of
adolescents and young adults with autism. It draws primarily from parent interviews.
Although I also got to know some parents when they arrived to drop off and pick up their
children or at parent assemblies, interviews were my main source of contact with and
information from parents. Some parents also had professional or volunteer/advocacy
roles within these organizations, and I therefore interacted with these parents on a more
regular basis. In 10 of the 38 parent interviews I conducted, one or both parents also
served on a professional or volunteer basis in roles related to autism services (within the
sites where I conducted fieldwork and in other organizations). Similarly, six of the 31
professionals I interviewed had an immediate family member on the spectrum and two
professionals identified traits of autism in themselves. I interviewed each individually
primarily for the role (parent or professional) for which they were recruited and I keep
them divided in this presentation of the data. The significance of such overlap of roles
will be discussed in more detail in Chapter 7 as it relates to new biopolitical theory.
This chapter addresses research questions (1) and (2) – how is autism
conceptualized in Italy, and how do people defined as autistici and their families define,
manage, and experience ASD. It largely parallels the structure of Chapter 4, with two
sections entitle “What is autism?” and “How should autism by managed?”
Section 1, “What is autism?” describes parents’ definitions of autism and the
characteristics of their children that they saw as related to autism. It begins by identifying
parent reported diagnoses. It then discusses perceptions of the DPS/DGS label and the
219
concept of “pure autism.” It next presents the traits of autism that were important to
parents and discusses the related yet distinct ideas of autism as a “different way of being”
autism as being in one’s “own world.” This section looks not only at how participants
described autism to me, but also how they discussed it with their children with autism. It
also presents examples of parents rejecting or contesting the label of autism for their
children and discusses the implications of these narratives for how autism is defined and
conceptualized. This section finds that parents defined autism in a wide variety of ways,
often related to the characteristics of the DSM-IV. When talking to their children, parents
often described autism to their children within the framework that all people have a
balance of strengths and weaknesses.
Section 2, “How should autism be managed?” describes parents’ interaction with
a variety of treatments, interventions, and professionals. This section begins with a
discussion of early interventions and ongoing interaction with therapists, luminaries, and
in-home approaches. It then moves into the school setting and discusses parents’
interactions with this very important institution during their children’s school years. In
both therapy and school settings, parents seek competent, knowledgeable professionals
and criticize about those that seem ill-informed about autism. This section concludes by
describing parents’ use of a variety of services. It concludes with parents’ hopes for their
children’s futures and their search for “un po’ di tranquillità” (a little tranquility) for their
adult children and themselves.
Section 1: What is Autism?
Understanding how different actors in the Italian context define autism forms the
crux of research question (1) and remains important to the other questions as well.
220
Therefore, I made certain choices in my research design that would allow me to
understand what autism meant to research participants. One possibility would have been
to verify an autism diagnosis through a diagnostic interview administered through a
trained professional either at the time of the study or as verified through review of
medical records. Such a methodological move is very important in many medical and
psychological studies because it allows different researchers from different backgrounds
in different locations to ensure that they are talking about the same thing. However, such
a move would have been begging the research question in my case, asking “What is
autism?” while stipulating that autism is that which is measured by the chosen diagnostic
instrument. It would also verge on a “category fallacy” (Kleinman 1987). My goal was to
get an “on the ground” understanding of what autism means in daily life. I therefore
identified “people with autism” through staff. All sites were targeted specifically, though
not always exclusively, to people on the autism spectrum. Therefore, not all youth who
frequented these sites had such a diagnosis (see Chapter 3 for site-by-site details); staff
identified to me those who did. This strategy allowed me to learn about the definitions of
autism, diagnostic instruments, and daily life observations that were important to staff in
making these identifications; and to understand how autism is conceptualized without
imposing my own definition.
Parents of these youth were eligible for interviews. They then had to consent to
the interview based on their role as parent to a child with an autism spectrum disorder.
Nonetheless, not all parents were in absolute agreement with this diagnosis and some
took the interview as an opportunity to explain their disagreement with staff on this issue.
Table 9 lists the categories of diagnoses given by parents when asked in interviews (all
221
parents in this sample had only one child on the spectrum). Not all of the diagnoses on
Table 9 are autism spectrum disorders, for several reasons. Some parents rejected the
diagnosis of autism attributed by staff. See the section below on contesting the diagnosis.
In the majority of interviews (N = 24, 63%), participants also discussed previous
diagnoses. Some of these diagnostic histories were stories of moving towards a diagnosis
of autism (see DPS/DGS below). Others moved away from an autism diagnosis by
contesting it. Of the five parents who listed their child’s diagnosis as intellectual
disability or another diagnosis without the addition of autism-related qualifiers such as
traits of autism, three had a previous diagnosis on the spectrum that they were actively
contesting, and two noted that their children had traits that they considered to be autistic,
but not part of the specific diagnosis they identified with their child.
222
Table 9: Parent-Reported Diagnoses
Category of
Diagnosis
Autism
Other + Traits of
Autism
Autism + Intellectual
Disability
Pervasive
Developmental
Disorder
Asperger
Other Diagnosis
High Functioning
Autism
Intellectual Disability
Asperger/High
Functioning Autism
Traits of Autism
Atypical Autism
Autism + Other
Examples
N
“autismo”
“autistico”
“psicosi con tratti autistici”
“epilessia generalizzata e disturbi dello spettro autistico”
“sindrome autistica con ritardo mentale”
“spettro autistico con insufficienza mentale media”
“disturbo generalizzato dello sviluppo”
“disturbo pervasivo dello sviluppo”
11
“Sindrome di Asperger”
“Asperger”
“disordini delle comunicazione”
“cerebrolese”
Genetic mutation
“autismo ad alto funzionamento”
“spettro autistico ad alto funzionamento”
“ritardo mentale”
"un autismo ad alto funzionamento barra Asperger.
Borderline.”
“tratti di autismo. Borderline.”
“Autismo Atipico”
“spettro autismo più ipercinetico”
3
6
4*
4*
3
2
2
1
1
1
1
* One interview listed both of these two diagnoses.
As discussed in Chapter 4, the terms “disturbo pervasivo dello sviluppo” and
“disturbo generalizzato dello sviluppo” refer to the autism spectrum, but index a milder
disorder. Professionals acknowledged that some practitioners started with these terms
before moving into a more precise definition of autism. For many parents these terms
straightforwardly meant autism. As one parent explained about doctors, “facevano fatica
a scriverlo” (they have trouble writing it). She identified her son’s diagnosis as autism
based on a verbal communication from doctors, but told me she thought the written
diagnosis might say disturbo generalizzato dello sviluppo or spettro autistico. One
mother described in more detail how she came to make the connection between the terms:
223
There were three meetings a year with the neuropsychiatrist. Each time she told
me generalized developmental disorder, but each time intellectual disability. The
diagnosis arrived bit by bit. Listening to the mothers at the [parents’] association,
I learned that it meant autism. I asked the neuropsychiatrist and she said yes.
- Interview, Parent at Day Center, not audio recorded
Two other mothers discussed similar conversations that went a different direction. When
asked what her son’s diagnosis of DGS meant to her one mother explained that it meant
“everything and nothing,” elaborating that “there’s everything inside it. It doesn’t have a
meaning. Except to say that there is a disorder.” She explained:
In fact, I asked the neuropsychiatrist, “Pardon me, but is he autistic or not?” She
told me “no, he’s not autistic because people with autism have a very different
behavioral profile in which they do not improve.” That’s what she said. On the
other hand [my son] improves. This is what she told me.
- Interview, Parent at Social/Recreational Group
With doubts, she began reading about Asperger’s syndrome and identifies her son more
with that label than with DGS. Another mother of a child with Asperger’s similarly asked
the neuropsychiatrist if DPS meant autism and was told no. Other parents differentiated
DPS from other diagnoses. One mother explained that it meant mild autistic traits, but
was not itself a helpful label.
I think that the diagnosis is a little vague and covers so much […] that is to say,
it’s not like Down Syndrome that is more… how to say it? More characteristic.
You understand it. This on the other hand is a bit… a bit generic.
- Interview, Parent at Social/Recreational Group
In these examples, the relationship between DPS/DGS and autism becomes blurry. In
fact, the boundaries of autism itself could be blurry. Thirteen (34%) parents made
reference to a concept of autismo puro (pure autism) or the similar adjectives vero (true)
or classico (classic), using these terms to describe their child (in one case) or as a point of
contrast with their child (the other eight times). Some spoke of pure autism as a simply
more serious impairment. Others spoke of autism that was not pure as the presence of
224
some traits (such as stereotypical and repetitive behaviors) but distinct absence of others
(such as language delay and being closed or unaffectionate). In two of these interviews,
the discussion of pure autism arose while directly contesting the diagnosis. In three other
interviews parents talked about pure autism and also contested a diagnosis in other parts
of the text. In these examples, parents discussed varying forms of autism not only in a
spectrum but also qualitatively – not just more or less autistic but also more or less pure
autism.
Taken together, what are these traits and characteristics that parents associated
with autism? Table 10 lists in order of frequency those traits of autism that were
commonly referenced. It excludes those traits that were raised only by a single
participant. It lists characteristics of autism raised by parents in answer to direct questions
about the definition and characteristics of autism as well as statements on these topics
that emerged in responses to other questions – including discussions of such traits of
autism or “pure autism” that their children did not share. In this way we can understand
how the concept of autism is understood by parents of youth identified by staff as having
autism.
225
Table 10: Commonly Referenced Characteristics of Autism (Parents)
Characteristic
Behavior Problems
Sensory and Perception Difficulties
Particular Skills, Talents, Intelligences
Good Memory
Hate Being Touched
Attention and Hyperactivity Problems
Rigid
Imitate Others
Lack of Empathy
Difficulty with Self-Control
Egocentrism
# of
Interviews
13
10
9
6
5
5
3
2
3
2
2*
* = Only for Asperger’s
Unlike professionals, parents rarely spoke of the “triad” of diagnostic
characteristics by that term. Only one parent who was also a medical professional used
the world triad, and in only 16 interviews (42%) did parents list all three traits:
impairments in social interaction, impairments in communication, and presence of
restricted and repetitive behaviors or interests. Nonetheless, the most common
characteristics mentioned by parents did fit into these categories. Twenty-four (63%)
interviews discussed Stereotypical and Repetitive Behaviors, nine of these in the form of
Routine and Ritual. Thirty-three (87%) talk about social interaction, particularly being
‘isolated,’ ‘closed,’ and ‘in one’s own world’ (25 interviews, 66% of the total). Twentyfive (66%) talked about social communication. It is worth noting that this table only
includes traits parents associated with autism per se, not all the traits they associated with
their children. More children evidenced differences in social communication, social
interaction, and patterns of interest that parents discussed but did not list as traits of
autism. Severe behavioral problems, particularly violence and aggression, were also
important to some parents’ narratives, although they did not list this trait as a
226
characteristic of autism, just as a general characteristic of their child.
The “different way of being” discourse was also important to parents. Fifteen
interviews (39%) included some version of this discourse which described people with
autism as having “una visione dell’esterno differente” (a different view of the outside); a
“modo” (way), “modo suo” (own way), or “maniera … tutta sua” (manner all his own) of
seeing, relating, understanding, being, perceiving, communicating, functioning,
socializing; a “mondo molto particolare” (a very particular world); “un altro mondo
reale” (a different real world) “dove tu capisci gli altri, ma gli altri non capiscono te”
(where you [the person with autism] understand others, but the others do not understand
you). This invocation of “another world” occurred in these statements either to describe
autism as itself another world, or as a condition in which the world of others, built by
others, is not hospitable. One mother went so far as to tell me, “per me il fatto che abbia
l’autismo è come dire ormai che ha capelli biondi o occhi azzuri, uguale, sullo stesso
piano, proprio. È una caratteristica” (for me the fact that she has autism is like saying
that she has blond hair and blue eyes, it’s the same, on the same plane, really. It’s a
characteristic). This perspective lends itself to a diversity framework. As one parent
explains:
In my own words, autism is... it’s not a disease but a way of being […] we should
learn from the time we are in preschool that each of us is different from the other,
and therefore even people with autism are not to be seen as strange people, but
like people different from us. Very different than us, clearly, sure. [….] We need
to have respect, you see. Don’t take away the respect that each of us on this earth
deserves. In the same way they need [it], they have a right to the same respect.
- Interview, Parent at Day Center
However, parents notably invoked this discourse less than professionals, who more
commonly included the idea of a different way of being in their definitions.
227
Parents’ use of the “world” metaphor was more commonly used outside of the
“different way of being” discourse. They described “living in one’s own world” (being
isolated and closed, a part of difficulties with social interaction) as a characteristic of
autism. Many parents explained that autism was, for example, “il fatto di stare lontani
dalla realtà, praticamente. Crearsi un mondo proprio e che non è in contatto con
l’esterno” (the fact of being far from reality, practically. Creating one’s own world and
that is not in contact with the external); “un po’ chiudirsi in un mondo tutto suo” (a little
bit closing up into a world entirely yours); “un bambino che vive nel suo mondo che… il
mondo esterno non esiste per lui, per lui esiste il suo mondo, di lui” (It’s a child that lives
in his own world that... the external world does not exist for him, for him only his own
world exists); and having a “mondo chiuso” (closed world). This next section looks at
conversations parents have with their children, potentially bridging these worlds, when
explaining their autism to them.
Discussing Autism
I asked parents how they described their children’s difficulties or characteristics
to their children themselves, if they discussed it all. This subsection considers their
responses. An important theme pervades their answers, which is the central message that
all people have different strengths and weaknesses and that the characteristics of autism
are a part of that common human experience. One parent at a day center explained
“ognuno di noi ha le sue tempistiche” (each of us has their own timetable) to her son
when he was frustrated at school and called himself stupid. She encouraged him “no, tu
non sei stupido. Ci voule più tempo ma vedrai che riuscirai anche tu” (No, you are not
stupid. You need more time but you will see that you will succeed, too). Other parents
228
recounted their children calling themselves derogatory names for mental illness – pazzo
and matto, both of which can be glossed as crazy. Both parents resisted these labels. One
young woman at a residenti center had been bullied at school, where other children called
her “handicappata” (handicapped). Her mother told her, “Non sei handicappata. Fai
fatica a fare alcune cose” (You aren’t handicapped. You have trouble doing certain
things). Summarizing this theme, one mother explained very clearly:
One thing that I taught [my son], which fortunately he took up very well, I always
tell him, “each person has some difficulties. Each person is able to do some
things, but is not able to do others. Therefore you aren’t good, let’s say, in math,
like some other people who are very talented in it, but you for example have an
exceptional visual memory and therefore you will do something really great using
this skill of yours, for example.”
- Interview, Parent at Social/Recreational Group
In this passage, the parent explains very clearly that different people have different
talents. Another parents explains in different words:
We are all a circle, in the sense that in this circle we have all virtues and defects,
but we are all a circle. Therefore where there is more than one thing, there is more
of another. Therefore, I want to say, you will have some positive characteristics
that [your sister] does not have. Therefore each of us has their specificities and
their positive parts and each of us has their negativity.
- Interview, Parent at Social/Recreational Group
These examples and others show the language parents use to construct and present autism
as a balance of skills and difficulties, the same as the balance any individual has. While
these two longer quotations come from parents of children at Social/Recreational Groups
who have somewhat lower levels of support needs, note that the first two quotations
come from parent from a Day Center and a Residential Center, whose children have
relatively high levels of support needs. Despite these high levels of support needs, parents
still drew on very similar language to describe their balance of strengths and weaknesses.
Parents also talked to their children about specific differences which visibly set
229
them apart from others, such as the presence of support staff at school, services, or
absence of a girlfriend. One mother talks to her daughter, who notices the commotion of
the school next door to their home. Although her daughter does not talk, she stresses the
importance of talking to and with her daughter about these issues:
I tell her, “[daughter], do you see? You cannot go in those schools because you do
not speak,” I tell her. And she starts going, “ma-ma-ma-ma,” you know? Because
that’s how she is. And well, I tell her, “Would you like to go to that school?” And
she tells me, “No.” And… but I tell her, “You see, [my niece] now in September,
[my niece] will start elementary school." And I tell her, “She goes where there are
children, who do homework, draw, while you won’t manage to do it, but that
which you can manage you can do all the same with your mother and at the center
where you go.” But I talk, because she listens to you.
- Interview, Parent at Day Center
Another mother talked to her son about the athletes he liked to look up pictures of on the
internet. She explains how he asked if he can meet the women he sees and that she tells
him no, but makes sure to point out that she and his brother could not meet them either.
She explains that she could frame it as a case of “faresti fatica anche conoscere una
ragazza fuori giù in strada su marciapiede” (you have trouble even meeting girls down
on the street, on the sidewalk) which would be saying, “non hai la capacità di conoscere
di avere un rapporto con una ragazza” (you lack the capacity to meet girls, to have a
relationship with a girl). Rather, she explains, “Ok tu non poui conoscere però neanch’io
posso conoscere quindi non preoccuparti, che va tutto bene lo stesso” (Ok, you can’t
meet her but neither can I so don’t worry, it’s fine all the same).
Several parents talked to their children about the support staff they see at school.
One mother whose son also had vision problems explained that she first tried to explain it
away as related to his vision, but now when they talk about it she says, “ci sono ragazzi
che hanno un po’ più difficoltà e ragazzi che ne hanno meno. Tu hai qualche difficoltà in
230
più” (there are those kids who have a little more difficulty and kids who have less. You
have some extra difficulty) She tells him as much without, she explains, getting into the
specifics. Other parents explained “Abbiamo detto che è una figura speciale per lui che
aveva più bisogno di appronfondire certe cose e quindi l’aiuto era per quello” (We said
that it’s a special figure for him, who needs more support to understand certain things and
therefore the help was for that) and that “tu hai sostegno poi tu sei un po’ disabile. Hai
delle difficoltà rispetto le altre. Hai qualche problema” (you have a support so you have
a bit of a disability. You have some difficulties compared to the others. You have some
problem).
These examples show that when parents talk to their children about their
difficulties, they are able to normalize them as a part of a universalized human condition
– we all have difficulties, some more than others, some different than others.
Two parents discussed resolving these difficulties together. The occasional parent
re-framed it as a problem that they, as parents, have in understanding their children. One
parent explained in detail:
Well, there was a moment some years ago in which he realized it. I knew because
he often cried, he was distressed. He became aware, a bit, of his problems. And
well, that is, I told him about it in the same way. It’s not that I lingered on giving
an explanation to [my son]. I said, “[son], you are an autistic person, and therefore
you have a type of communication different that the one that your mother has, and
your mother wants to seek to understand you in order to be able to help you
because otherwise it is very difficult to help you.” And still today. He, we
lingered a moment at the explanation. I say, “you must help me to understand you
otherwise I can’t do anything because I am less intelligent than you are.”
- Interview, Parent at Day Center
Putting the onus of communication and comprehension on the parent moved beyond
identifying difficulty as a common experience. It framed the difficulty as one of
difference, even valorizing the child’s perspective over the parent’s. It therefore provides
231
further evidence of the central point of this section, that parents’ discussions of disability
with their children minimized a deficit narrative, even among those parenting young
people with higher levels of support needs who attended highly staffed day and
residential centers. Both reading autism as a way of being and explaining autism in a nondeficit-focused way allude to neurodiversity discourses and the idea of autism as an
identity. These discourses will be discussed more in Chapter 7, which discusses their
relevance in new biopolitical theory.
Contesting the Diagnosis
Parents in ten interviews (26%) in this sample expressed doubts about their
child’s diagnosis or outright contested it. These challenges took three distinct and
different forms: doubting the validity of the diagnosis of autism or Asperger’s at all,
pointing instead to another genre of problem entirely; challenging the specific diagnosis
(e.g. DPS/DGS) in favor of another (e.g., Asperger’s); and doubting the validity of an
autism diagnosis at first then changing their mind. In addition to revealing in more detail
how autism was conceptualized (research question 1), these rather common instances
illustrate points of conflict between professionals and parents on this very topic and also
demonstrate how parents come to know what they know about autism and mobilize that
knowledge with respect to their children.
The discussion above has touched upon the first challenge to the diagnosis, in the
theme of “pure autism.” Parents in five interviews challenged the appropriateness of an
autism diagnosis. One mother argued that the diagnosis was no longer appropriate for her
daughter because she had overcome it; one couple doubted the sticking power of the
diagnosis because their son’s diagnosis changed so frequently; and the other three argued
232
their respective children’s problems were not autism because they were due instead to an
identifiable physical problem.
These first two cases are similar in that they both doubt the validity of the autism
or Asperger’s diagnosis while acknowledging a difference and valuing the way the label
opened doors to Social/Recreational Groups that their children attend. They show how
unimportant the diagnosis can be for parents. In their two very different approaches –
actively seeking to change the diagnosis, and accepting that diagnoses will change – the
label was not meaningful. Nonetheless, for each youth the service was important.
The other three parents/couples in this category questioned the diagnosis because
they linked their children’s problems instead to discretely identifiable physical problems:
one due to brain damage, one to birth injury, and one to a genetic mutation. Another
mother similarly doubted the diagnosis of autism due to physical health considerations.
Her son, in his mid-teens, had recently undergone a genetic analysis that uncovered a rare
genetic mutation. Although he had had an autism spectrum diagnosis as a young child,
his mother now explained that the genetic mutation was the more correct diagnosis.
These examples show that physical health issues and other identifiable problems can be
more important to parents than the autism diagnosis. Indeed, some parents who were
approached to participate in this study declined because they did not see their children as
having autism, even though professionals identified them as having autism. However,
three parents pointed to both identifiable physical issues (brain damage, genetic illness)
and autism, seeing the autism as secondary to it but still very important (see Table 11,
below).
Three parents contested not the validity of autism per se, but the specific
233
diagnosis within the autism spectrum. Above we met the mother who explained that DGS
means “everything and nothing.” This interviewee was the mother of one of the youngest
children whose parents participated in this interview. Her son was 14 and had just started
attending a Social/Recreational Group at the suggestion of a school principal. She found
the youth with Asperger’s in the social/recreational activity group to be very similar to
her son, and she found him to be comfortable and happy in the environment. Yet his
diagnosis was disturbo generalizzato dello sviluppo. This mother explains:
She [the neuropsychiatrist] knows more than I do, so I defer. But I understood
because reading… here and there, I understood that autism isn’t just one situation,
that is it’s not, um, there are many types of autism. There are various gradations.
And [my son] does not have a severe autism. He has a medium autism. That I do
not know if you can call it Asperger’s or not. This I do not know.
- Interview, Parent at Social/Recreational Activity Group
She then described how she had been reading books about Asperger’s and recognized her
son in their descriptions. She also took a test in such a book which suggested he may well
have Asperger’s. She prefers to use the term Asperger’s to talk about her son, but
acknowledges that she has no professional confirmation. She spoke highly of one
professional who seems to agree with her. While DGS meant nothing to her, Asperger’s
resonated better. She nonetheless also defers to professional experience, making several
comments to that effect throughout the interview. In everyday life, Asperger’s is
personally meaningful, it is the term she prefers to use.
Another mother went the opposite direction, pushing away from the term
Asperger’s in favor of “high functioning autism” or “borderline.” She explains her
experiences with the online Asperger’s community in Italy:
Then I distanced myself a bit because I started to read “my son will go to a
scientific liceo, my son will go to a classical liceo, my son will go...” and I was
like “maybe [my son] is not so Asperger after all!” (Laughs). “I can’t send him to
234
liceo, no?” I felt a bit like a fish out of water reading these forums where they
talked about Asperger […] [my son] has Asperger’s in certain traits. But for
certain [traits] de doesn’t, in the sense that, how can I say it, a person with
Asperger’s Syndrome is a kid that can go to liceo, and mine cannot go to liceo.
- Interview, Parent at Social/Recreational Group
Another mother went back and forth between Asperger’s and spettro autistico.
Although some doctors had written or verbalized a diagnosis of Asperger’s syndrome,
she explained at the beginning of the interview that he had a diagnosis “di Asperger.
Spettro autistico più che Asperger, se voule, più spettro autistico ad alto funzionamento
che proprio Asperger perché [...] questi test che fanno per vedere se uno ha sindrome di
asperger o no, lui non era pieno” (of Asperger’s. The autism spectrum more then
Asperger’s, if you want, more high functioning autism spectrum than really Asperger’s
because [...] these tests that they do to see if someone has Asperger’s syndrome or not, he
wasn’t full [of those criteria].” She wondered, however, if he did the tests too quickly or
represented his own characteristics differently than she would have described them
herself.
Two parents contested the diagnosis at some points in the interview, but hedged at
others. Both of these parents contrast their child with another specific child with autism,
but then hedge by noting that not all people with autism are the same. For one of these
families, this story was one of gradual acceptance. One mother explained that she was
doubtful when she first heard the diagnosis of autism, “Perchè ripeto una pensa alla
persona autistica come quella che non parla in assoluto. E noi dicevamo “nostra figlia
parla.” Sempre abbiamo visti altri ragazzi [...] con movimenti sempre repetitivi, manuali,
gesti di chiusura” (Because like I said, you think of a person with autism as someone
who absolutely does not talk. And we said “our daughter talks.” We’d always seen other
235
kids […] with repetitive hand movements, gestures of closing off). She went on to
explain that her daughter, though repetitive in some things, also collaborates with others
when she wants. Mother and father discussed how perhaps more characteristics of autism
emerged with age. Their daughter had previously gone to a day center that was not autism
specific. This experience did not go well and they had to find another service. As the
mother explains, “diventava un’esigenza inquadrarla,” (it became necessary to
categorize her). Furthermore, “abbiamo capito anche noi che nello spettro ci sono gli
autistici non sono tutti nello stesso modo, che in 50% parlano in 50 no” (we also
understood that in the spectrum there are people with autism that are not all the same
way, that in 50% [of cases] they speak, in 50(%) no). This mother described an important
meeting with the person in charge of this autism-specific service:
We had some doubt then when [my daughter] came here, after the first while, we
even spoke with doctor [last name], we said “but listen, do you agree with the
diagnosis, could there be something else?” He told me, “no, no, I agree, fully.”
There you go. And therefore this… it’s not that it calmed us (but) in the sense that
the problem is like this but at least you say “go in this direction, go.”
-Interview, Parent at Residential Center
As was the case with some of the parents who rejected diagnosis above, having a
diagnosis still served a purpose in identifying services. The parents in this interview
expressed a lot of doubt about their daughter’s diagnosis, doubt that may not be fully
resolved. However, having the diagnosis gave them a direction to head when general
disability services were not a good fit for their daughter.
In situations in which parents contest the diagnosis provided by professionals, the
differences between professional and parent understandings of particular youth emerge
most clearly. These themes point to promising directions for future research, which could
fruitfully compare parent reports with practitioner reports with certificates of invalidity
236
with organizational coordinator reports and even with youth reports (discussed in Chapter
6). Such a study would provide insight into the popularity and desirability of an autism
diagnosis. Who is more likely to attribute autism, parent, physician, or other autism
professional? A systematic comparison is of course beyond the scope of this study, which
did not investigate medical files – but such as a study that did would be interesting
because it would reveal how different actors bring different understandings to the table. It
would also show how much of the medical terminology parents found useful.
Autism, Asperger’s Syndrome, and Intellectual Disability
As can be seen in Table 9, several parents included intellectual disability as part
of their child’s diagnosis when asked. Several others mentioned intellectual disability as
among their child’s challenges, implicitly including it under the umbrella of autism. This
equating was rarely challenged. However, there were exceptions:
I always say that they don’t have an intellectual disability, they have a different
learning style than what they are given, than what is around them, but they do not
have an intellectual disability because I take note of the fact that [my son] is able
to do, to think, and to put into action some strategies to reach his own objective
that we do not even manage to think of [….] He is much cleverer than us. And so
I cannot think that a person like this has an intellectual disability.
- Interview
They told me that he probably has an intellectual disability. But he managed to
cause so much trouble when he was with us. If autism carries with it such a series
intellectual disability, why is he so alert, so ready to escape, to take his brother’s
things? He’s clever [….] The strange thing that we did not manage to understand
when the psychologist said intellectual disability [is that] he’s much more alert
than my older son [….] We could not understand why he is so intelligent about
making trouble but did not want to talk, did not follow what the others were doing
[….] He didn’t talk. At school with the others he did not draw, did not
differentiate colors. They told for this reason that this is an intellectual disability.
- Interview, Parent at Day Center, not audio recorded
This discourse provided space for autism as a different way of thinking, learning, and/or
perceiving that was not necessarily deficient and might be superior. Note in both of the
237
above quotations the use of the adjective furbo (clever). The concept of furbezza has been
noted by anthropologist as very important in the Italian vocabulary, meaning clever but
“always with the connotation of craftiness” (Galt 1974:195). Despite its noted importance
in the older anthropological literature, the term was of central importance neither to
research participants nor to my daily life experiences in the field. One other parent
described her daughter as “furba,” but with an intellectual disability. However, it was
very relevant when contesting the association between autism and intellectual disability.
Staff occasionally interpreted the behavior of people with autism they worked with as
furbo as well. Two incidents in my fieldnotes draw on this term, both times describing
efforts of the people with autism to break the rules. It may be that the concept of furba
provided participates a lens to identify positive intellectual traits in people with autism,
even those who had low recorded IQs and high levels of support needs.
Asperger’s syndrome was often associated with intelligence. The mothers above
who contested diagnoses allude to this, doubting their children’s Asperger’s because of
their lack of higher IQs. Intelligence remains an important hallmark of Asperger’s
syndrome for parents, as for professionals. Particular skills or intelligences were a
commonly mentioned characteristic of autism (Table 10). However, in four of these
interviews, this characteristic is raised as part of contesting the diagnosis of Asperger’s
due to their child’s lack of above-average IQ, or contrasting their children with what they
have seen in film or read in books. However, some parents identified their child had
Asperger’s and intellectual disability, so the association is not absolute.
What Causes Autism
I also asked parents what they thought caused autism. Some parents answered
238
only for their specific child and others for both their own child and others with autism.
Table 11 breaks down these causes. Many of the categories are more specific than those
given by professionals, which is not to say they cannot be subsumed under them but
professionals often used the language of “organic,” “biological,” or “genetic” whereas
parents pointed to more specific causes within these categories such as birth/pregnancy
complications or specific genetic disorders. Moreover, parents were often extremely
tentative. Nine of those who stated that they did not know what causes autism also
provided speculative answers which are also included in the table. I have therefore
included a “Maybe” category. Please note that this table also excludes three parents who
contested the diagnosis of autism because of an identifiable physical cause that negated it.
Those parents, as discussed above, attributed their child’s problems not to autism, but to
another illness or identifiable disorder.
239
Table 11: Possible Causes of Autism (Parents)
Cause
I Don’t Know
Vaccines40
Neurological
Pregnancy/Birth Complications
Secondary to another Illness/Disorder
Genetic Predisposition + Environmental
(Physical or Social)
Fetal Development
Genetics – Hereditary
Genetics – General
Biological or Organic
Injury
Social Causes
Virus
Toxic Metals
Mother’s Age
Refrigerator Mother
Yes
15
7
6
4
3
3
Maybe
0
3
2
2
2
1
No
0
3
0
1
1
0
2
2
2
2
1
1
0
0
0
0
5
1
2
0
2
2
1
0
0
0
0
1
1
0
0
0
0
1
2
6
In short, the aggregated group had little consensus, with many parents responding
that they did not know. Many parents pointed to specific organic/biological events such
as vaccines, birth/pregnancy complications, injuries, or other specific disorders/illnesses.
Few parents invoked social causes as the origin of their child’s autism. However, three
parents did explain a social mechanism in which an original problem (e.g. epilepsy)
causes social isolation, perhaps keeping the child home from school a lot, which causes
or aggravates problems of autism in terms of social interaction and social communication
difficulties.
Section 2: How should autism be managed?
This section answers research question (2), specifically focusing on the treatment
and management of autism. It is organized vaguely chronologically in terms of the
40
A full discussion of vaccine causation theories is beyond the scope of this research. For more targeted
social science investigations into vaccine causation theories and autism in the United States, see S.R.
Kaufman (2010) and Silverman (2012).
240
services it discusses. As described in Chapter 2, public and private therapists such as
logopediste and psicomotriciste were most often visited during the pre-school and early
schooling phases. The first subsection therefore discusses these therapies and
interventions. From an early age, schools are a very important part of the lives of all
Italian children, including children with disabilities. The next subsection therefore
addresses parents’ perspectives on their children’s scholastic experiences. The third
subsection then moves into the day, residential, and social/recreational services parents
accessed, often after and sometimes during the scholastic career. The central points of
this section are two important goals that pervade parents’ narratives: the search for
competent experts, and the search for “un po’ di tranquillità” (a little peace), as it was
phrased by one interview participant, for the present and future of their children and
themselves.
Therapies and Interventions
Table 12: Approaches
Approach Name
Medication
Psicomotricità
Logopedia
Horse Therapy
Music Therapy
Psychotherapy
TEACCH
Diet
Water-Based Therapies
Doman-Delacato
Psychological Therapies/Visits
Physical Therapy
Applied Behavior Analysis
Facilitated Communication
Supplements
References in
Interviews and
Fieldnotes
34
24
15
14
14
14
11
10
7
6
6
6
6
5
5
Support Integrate Reject
or Modify
18
18
7
10
10
6
5
5
6
6
5
4
2
5
5
1
1
1
0
0
0
4
0
0
0
0
0
1
0
0
15
4
7
4
4
8
2
5
1
0
1
2
3
0
0
241
Approach Name
Comportamentale
Pet Therapy
Theater as Therapy
Feuerstein
Sport Therapy
Defeat Autism Now!
Dolphin Therapy
(Psico)educativo
Psychoanalysis
Cognitivo-Comportamentale
Cognitivo
Art Therapy
Alternative and Augmentative
Communication
Portage Method42
Vagal Stimulator
Emotion Recognition Software
Eurythmy
Desensibilizzazione Uditiva
Holding
Occupational Therapy
Hyperbaric Chamber
References in
Interviews and
Fieldnotes
5
4
3
341
3
3
3
3
3
2
2
2
2
Support Integrate Reject
or Modify
2
2
3
3
3
2
2
1
0
2
2
1
1
1
0
0
0
0
0
0
1
0
0
0
0
0
2
2
0
0
0
1
1
1
3
0
0
1
1
2
1
1
1
1
1
1
1
0
1
1
1
1
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
0
2
0
0
0
0
1
1
1
Table 12 presents parents’ evaluations of therapies and interventions they
encountered, including therapies used for epilepsy which parents may or may not
understand as part of autism. It also includes the evaluations of those parents who
contested the diagnosis of autism, because they still encountered the same or similar
suggestions from professionals and, as we will see below, professionals’
recommendations were extremely important. I list therapies as rejected if parents reported
that they did not try them, and specifically mentioned deciding not to do so or being
41
One parent also references positive experiences with a psychologist who used the method of
“Feuerbach.” Not finding anything related to developmental psychology by this name, I wonder if it is not
also Feuerstein.
42
Cameron (1997) and Shearer and Shearer (1972)
242
advised against doing so as the therapy was not appropriate for their specific child. Some
parents spoke highly of professionals that provided these services even if they ultimately
did not provide them to their children – in fact specifically praising professionals who
were honest with parents if their therapy would be a bad fit. I also mark as rejected
therapies that they had negative experiences with when they did try them. I marked as
supported interventions if parents tried them and had good or decent results, and also if
they wished they had been able to try them but the therapies were not available at the
time (i.e., ABA and TEACCH decades ago). Because parents often took an “all of the
above” approach to therapies and interventions, I classified as “integrate or modify” only
those comments that explicitly stated that the given therapy is good only in combination
or only when modified, for example, “Poi sicuramente farmaci contano ma contano
sicuramente tutto quello che è l‘ambiente, contesto, secondo noi” (Surely the
medications count, but surely all that which is the environment, the context, counts in our
opinion).
Recall from Chapter 2 that children with disabilities are served by the field of
neuropsychiatry, and it is often a neuropsychiatry office that initially directs parents to
services. For autism, these services are traditionally (neuro)psicomotricità and logopedia,
as discussed in Chapter 4. Indeed, these two therapies are towards the top of the list of
most commonly discussed therapies, in addition to psychotherapy and medication.
Neuropsychiatrists might also refer parents to specialized centers or other practitioners
who provided other therapies. There were also services that parents sought out
themselves, including private practitioners of psicomotricità and logopedia, private
psychologists or psychotherapists, privately hired educatori, or privately run services
243
such as residential services or activities such as horse therapy.
In all of these types of encounters, parents discussed their search for competent,
trained experts who would address their child’s needs. When complementing
professionals they had encountered, parents used terms in gamba (‘on the ball,’ lit. in
leg), valido (valid), and preparato (prepared) in addition to the generally positive bene
(well, good) and brava (expert, good). There were also complements not linked to
competence, including affettuosa (affectionate). When describing negative experiences,
parents used terms such as incapace (incompetent), inadeguato or non adeguato
(inadequate), non competente (incompetent), inesperto (not expert), and non informato
(uninformed). Parents also complained of certain professionals as only looking for
money, that vendono (sell) therapies, rubano soldi (steal money), sometimes to the extent
of calling them ladri (thieves). Parents did at times quit certain therapies or professionals
who they found to be incompetent, purely profit-motivated, or negligent. A small number
of parents also reported filing denuncie (formal complaints) against services and
professionals.
There were certain therapies that were especially hot topics for parents, including
medications, psychotherapy/psychoanalysis, logopedia, and dietary interventions. The
following several paragraphs address these interventions in turn.
Medication was by far the most commonly referenced treatment by parents.
Commonly prescribed medications included seizure medication; antipsychotics and
antidepressants; and benzodiazepines. While the majority of parents accepted these
medications, especially those for epilepsy, there was also a large minority (15 interviews)
who expressed significant discomfort with medication. Even some parents who
244
ultimately accepted medication explained that they were initially hesitant. For example,
one mother told me, “Guardi, io era contraria ai farmaci. Non avrei voluto dare nessun
farmaco a [filgio]. Poi però quando ho visto i risultati, quando ho visto per [figlio]
effettivamente era più tranquillo, era più controllato, ho cominciato ad apprezzare i
farmaci” (Look, I was against medication. I would never have wanted to give a
medication to [my son]. But then when I saw the results, when I saw for [my son] was
effectively calmer, was more controlled, I began to appreciate the medications). The
following discussion unpacks parents’ more negative experiences.
Some parents outright questioned the necessity of the drugs their children used.
Regarding a medication against aggression, one parent explained, “Adesso effettivamente
non sappiamo se serve ancora. Aspettiamo che ci dicono perché forse non c’è neanche
più bisogno, non so” (now we really do not know if he still needs it. We are waiting for
them to tell us because maybe there is not even any more need, I do not know). Another
couple explained that medication was only a poor substitute for “entrare nel suo mondo”
(entering into her world), that medication “non cura niente” (does not cure anything) and
that “più che essere gestita dai farmaci” (rather than having her managed by
medications) it is important to “entrare in comunicazione con lei, capire le sue difficoltà,
le sue esigenze […] suo modo di vivere, insomma” (enter into communication with her,
understand her troubles, her needs […] her mode of living, in short). Parents also stressed
the importance of not having been prescribed medication, as the mother who explained,
“Io spero di non aver mai bisogno di farmaci per [figlio] perché trovi anche dei
specialisti che ti dicono ‘sì suo figlio così, prenda farmaco’” (I hope I never need
medication for [my son] because you find even specialists that tell you ‘yes your son is
245
like this, take medication.’). However, for some this was a better safe than sorry situation.
One couple explained that their son started taking anti-seizure medications as a young
child, and the dose has been gradually lowered. They explained that they wonder if it is
still necessary, but are perfectly willing to let him keep taking it because they are so
scared of a repeat of the seizures, which caused him to faint and sometimes get injured.
While the parents quoted above continued with the prescribed medication or lack
therefore, others left services or specific professionals as a result of suggested
medication. One mother complained of a psychiatrist who, without even knowing her
child, prescribed a medication that this woman’s family practitioner later told her was
outdated and inappropriate. Another left a service after an incident in which the
neuropsychiatrist reacted by suggesting medication and the mother decided instead to
find another service. One couple talks about the decision to leave one Day Center:
One day the director called us in, and told that we must give medications to [our
son] because otherwise he is too agitated, et cetera. We told her, “no, we will not
give him medication, because his doctor does not consider it appropriate to give
him medication. You need to find a way to intervene in [our son’s] behaviors.”
- Interview, Parents at Day Center
The Day Center insisted on medication, and so the family left.
Along with this general discomfort with medication came an interest in reducing
or eliminating entirely the dosage. One father proudly explained, “Lei aveva anche crisi
epilettiche a quel periodo, prendeva anche dei farmaci. Poi piano piano anche con un
certo tipo di lavoro siamo riusciti anche a togliere i farmaci a [figlia] e da quanto era
ragazzina fino a una certa età non ha più avuto crisi” (She also had epileptic seizures
then, she was even taking medications. Then ever so slowly with a certain type of work
we managed to even eliminate [my daughter’s] medications and from the time she was a
246
young girl up until a certain age she did not have any more seizures). This decision was
made in conjunction with the physician. In other cases, this interest became a quest and a
real struggle against professionals. One couple described a struggle they were
undertaking to return their adult child’s dose to its previous level. Another parent did
decide to stop medication entirely without first consulting her doctor. She tried giving her
son a medication prescribed for hyperactivity, but she found it just made things worse.
One day shortly after starting the medication, she decided not to give it to him anymore.
This story demonstrates that parents did sometimes take matters into their own hands.
Most, however, did not. When I asked parents who opposed medication why they used it
anyway, they consistently explained that they were told to, e.g., “sono stata consigliata”
(I was advised). One parent went so far as to tell me, “Devi accetare quello che dicono
anche che sia sbagliato” (you must accept what they say even if it is wrong). This
statement demonstrates parents’ reliance on expert professionals and willingness to
follow their indications even when they would rather not.
While medication was controversial in the parent sample – some supporting its
use, others rejecting it, and many experiencing tension and discomfort – psychotherapy,
especially psychoanalysis, was more consistently rejected by parents. Although a
minority of parents found it useful (one of whom had a non-verbal son, the others verbal),
many spoke out against it. Their complaints included that their children were too young,
too nonverbal, or too affected by intellectual disability for this highly verbal, reflective
therapy.
General complaints held that it was just not applicable for someone with autism.
One mother described a Jungian analysis, telling me, “è andato che… un’esperienza
247
frustrante [....] Perché era una cosa… che non aveva nessun senso. Io non ne capisco
niente, non mi rendevo conto che non aveva nessun senso quello che stava facendo. Cioè,
portare in analisi una bambina di due anni… non ha alcun senso. (It was… a frustrating
experience [….] Because it was something… that didn’t make any sense. I do not
understand it at all. I did not realize that it did not make sense what she was doing. I
mean, taking a two year old into analysis… it does not make any sense). She further
explained, “E già si sapeva che l’analisi non serviva a niente, perché non c’entrava
niente la mamma-frigorifero, che i problemi erano di altro tipo” (And already it was
known that analysis doesn’t help anything, because the refrigerator mother does not have
anything to do with it, that these problems are of another kind). As another parent
explained, it would be better to have a psychopedagogical and pragmatic therapy: “quindi
non fare analisi, non fare ma… seguire dei criteri pedagogici per risolvere i suoi
disordini, chiamiamoli, sociali” (so do not do analysis, do not do that but… follow
pedagogical criteria to resolve his, let us call them, social deficits). Unlike with
medication, parents who did not find psychotherapy useful were often able to quit it. Like
with medication, several parents said they used psychotherapy because they were
advised, e.g., “Perché mi l’hanno detta, per me quella che mi l’ha consigliata era un
autorità” (because they told me to, for me it was that an authority advised me).
Experiences with logopedia were particularly split among parents, for somewhat
similar reasons. Parents occasionally found it not useful for children who did not speak,
and much more commonly not applicable for children on the other end of the spectrum,
who had a strong command of verbal communication. For those in the middle, the
therapy was often very useful. Some parents whose children did not need help with
248
language skills per se reported that they saw other benefits from logopedia, including
additional help in school subjects and strong relationships with the therapist.
Finally, dietary interventions were particularly contested. Some parents
considered it and found it not applicable for their children. One couple did perform tests
for intolerances and follow the diet, but saw it only as helpful for physical health. Others,
however, reported great success with dietary interventions related to removing
ingredients to which children were intolerant (most commonly lactose/casein and gluten).
Four parents described their experiences consulting with specialists and developing diet
plans. One parent who followed the DAN! Protocol even considered her child to no
longer be diagnosable with autism, and was seeking to have the diagnosis removed (as
discussed above).
Dietary interventions often required a lot of parent-directed intervention in the
home, guided by professional experts so important they bordered on luminaries. While
certainly there were local luminaries parents turned to who were involved in the services
included in this study, there were also traveling luminaries who pioneered specific
approaches. Parents consulted local luminaries to learn educational strategies to use at
home. One father detailed in his interview the close collaboration he had with a doctor as
they gathered and supervised a large group of volunteer therapists to work collectively
with his child. Traveling luminaries headed programs like DAN!, the Doman-Delacato
method, and the Feuerstein method. Parents would see these luminaries when they were
near the city, or would travel in order to visit them and get guidance. The next few
paragraphs look at these interventions in turn.
As discussed in Chapter 4, the most prominent interventions advocated by
249
professionals in the area were TEACCH and ABA. Many autism-specific services for
individuals of all ages used these strategies within their centers. Some parents consulted
with professionals on a one-on-one basis to learn educational strategies to use at home,
derived from TEACCH, ABA, Behavioral and Cognitive-Behavioral Therapies, and
more. For example, one mother at a day center explained, “loro sono sempre stati tutti
disponibili per... aiutarci, o per suggerci dei metodi uh... per esempio ecco quando
abbiamo fatto l’analisi del sangue quindi loro hanno fatto tutta una preparazione con
[figlio]” (They were always available to... help us, or to suggestion some methods to us,
uh... for example when we had to do a blood test so they did a whole preparation with
[my son],” getting him ready for this new experience. Two parents (both of whom are
describing the same local expert) explain these strategies in detail:
Here they also give us so many practical suggestions, at this place. For example,
they showed us even at the day center. I know that, let’s say it like this, having
used the TEACCH method therefore everything made with very schematic plans,
plans against the wall. Uh or all the visual imagines that for [my daughter] have
worked well because even if you say the things 10 times, you try to reassure her
about what will come next, about what will come after that. But verbally it does
not work. When we saw both in the day center and outside of the [residential]
community her panel with her photo, with all the visual steps, with the removal of
this thing. We said to ourselves, “but wow, one thing like this is enough maybe to
help [our daughter] at school, to help us at home, to give her reference for the
passing of time, for the steps, for the things she does, visual, and not, let’s say,
always verbalizing them.”
- Interview, Parent at Residential Center
It was also a help for us because the psychologist came to our house, saw come
we behave with [our son], therefore made a whole diagnosis on the base of our
behavior, gave us also indications on how to intervene with those problem
behaviors that emerged [....] for example, sometimes he suggested that we ignore,
i.e., not continue to say “let it be, let it be” but to ignore the behavior in such a
way that sooner or later this behavior at any rate is eliminated.
- Interview, Parent at Residential Center
Another couple talks about learning to use visual communication strategies at home, but
250
also learning that they really cannot organize their home as well as at the residence their
son attends, in which he is more comfortable. Although by no means all parents used
these “mainstream” staff and professionals to advise them how to work with their
children at home, there were those that did and that found it extremely helpful.
Three relatively common methods parents used at home in consultation with
traveling or international experts were DAN!, Feuerstein, and Doman-Delacato. The
DAN! or Defeat Autism Now! Protocol originated in the United States and has been
much studied by social scientists in that context (Eyal et al. 2010; Silverman 2012). In the
United States, the term DAN! is no longer used, but in Italy it is still in circulation. DAN!
supporters view autism as a biochemical problem originated in gut enzymes, allergies,
intolerances, and gastrointestinal problems (McCandless 2009). The appropriate
treatment for autism is therefore a diet that eschews foods that aggravate these
intolerances as well as dietary supplements and perhaps chelation. The one parent
interviewed who supported the DAN! protocol explained how she was looking online and
heard about DAN! and got in contact with a doctor, a former neuropsychiatrist who, upon
having a daughter with autism, went to US to learn about DAN and now travels Italy
seeing patients with autism. Although only one parent used the term DAN!, four other
parents followed similar diets and had to similarly consult with specialists who perform
allergy tests and the like. The Feuerstein method refers to an educational strategy
pioneered by Dr. Reuven Feuerstein in Israel for children with learning difficulties (The
Feuerstein Institute 2014). One mother interviewed used the strategies at home to teach
her child cursive, mathematics, and English. Another mother took her son to Israel to
meet with Dr. Feuerstein for “full immersion” to work on fine motor skills and the like.
251
The Doman-Delacato method, also known individually as Doman and Delacato Methods,
similarly refer to therapies pioneered by professionals of that name – U.S. physical
therapist Glenn Doman and psychologist Carl Delacato. Their program, headquartered at
the Institutes for the Achievement of Human Potential in Philadelphia, was originally
developed to help children with brain injuries and their website explains that autism is
one of the many common labels for “brain-injured children” (Institutes for the
Achievement of Human Potential 2014) Their interventions involve a series of exercises
that parents perform at home. Parents consulted with trained physicians and even
Delacato himself for guidance in these exercises, then performed them at home by
themselves or with the help of others such as a teacher.
These at-home experiences are further evidence of parents’ ongoing search for
expertise. In these cases, parents not only follow professional experts but become trained
as paraprofessional therapists themselves. All of the above three therapies are often
contested by professional associations, and are not recommended by the Italian autism
treatment guidelines (Sistema Nazionale per le Linee Guida-Istituto Superiore di Sanità
2011). Nonetheless, a minority of parents spoke favorably of them (see Table 12) and a
subset of those found them very useful. A few acknowledged criticism from other
doctors. One mother frequently contrasted her experiences with specialists who dealt with
the dietary, Doman, and Delacato methods with her experiences with “medici normali”
(normal doctors).Whether following “normal” doctors or these specialists, parents were
actively seeking expertise to guide them through their child’s development. Parents
learned about interventions – both recommended and not recommended by the guidelines
– through doctors, friends, conferences, listservs, websites, and the like.
252
Parents who changed their home environment or became paraprofessionals and
therapists themselves went against the slogan (Zapella 2010:15) “i genitori devono fare i
genitori” (parents must be parents/act only as parents).43 This sentiment was indeed
expressed by some parents. One mother told me about a conference she had attended in
which a man who had lived in New Zealand explained their (in her opinion) exceptional
system. She recounted, “ha detto che prendono in carico dal momento in cui nasce il
bambino, ‘io facevo solo il papà’” (he said that they take the child into care from the
moment he is born, ‘I act only as the father’). Some parents talked about the importance
of having an external figure, sometimes in order to maintain family relationships (going
to a therapist can be unpleasant; being with family should be pleasant) and sometimes
because of family relationships, as in the case of one family who explained that children
have an “arma psicologica” (psychological weapon) with which to “blackmail” parents
into giving them what they want. Even a mother who was also a medical professional
explained her desire to keep these roles separate when working with the schools:
Even if in the end I always prepared the tools myself, I wanted to keep separate
the role of doctor and the role of mother. Therefore I was preparing all the
materials and I was passing them to the neuropsychiatrist and I wanted it to be she
who interfaced with the teachers.
- Interview, Parent at Social/Recreational Group
She told me that the teachers of course knew that she was the source of the materials, but
that she wanted to respect the order of things. “Io sono convinta che è stato molto
importante anche il rispetto degli ruoli in cui una mamma comunque anche se medico
faccia la mamma ci sia neuropsichiatra competente” (I am convinced that it is also very
The phrase does not translate smoothly into English, as the verb “fare” indicates not “to be” but “to
make” or “to do.” The phrase therefore indicates more accurately “parents must do parents” in the sense of
Rocko’s hit song, “Umma do me” which states “you just do you, and umma [I’m going to] do me.”
43
253
important to respect roles, in which a mother, even one who is also a doctor, acts as
mother when there is a competent neuropsychiatrist), she explained.
Parents also chose to pursue various therapeutic programs based around
“recreations” such as music, art, sports, and animals. Some parents explained that they
pursued these therapies because, as one parent told me for Art Therapy, “ci ha
consigliato” (they advised us to). Others made clear a distinction between these therapies
and the interventions such as psicomotricità and logopedia. One mother explained, “la
musicaterapia è stata sempre diciamo un hobby, che attraverso un hobby, una cosa che
gli piace [....] poteva essere un modo di sbloccarlo” (Music therapy was always, let’s
say, a “hobby,” that through a “hobby,” something that he likes [....] it could be a way to
unblock him). Another mother explained, “La musicaterapia, anche lì, non la prendiamo
come una terapia, come la psicomotricità o come la cosa me come un po’ l’ippoterapia
nel senso che abbiamo scelto noi da farla con uno psicologo che conosco, ed è un... ogni
15 giorni, quindi non è una terapia. È un momento piacevole per [figlio]” (The music
therapy, also there, I did not take it as therapy, like the psicomotricità or like that but a
little like Horse Therapy in the sense that we chose ourselves to do it with a psychologist
that I know, and it was a… every 15 days, so it was not a therapy. It was a pleasant
moment for [my son]). This mother further explains the difference between music and
horse therapy and the interventions of psicomotricità, logopedia, and physical therapy,
saying “sono terapie non che scelgo io ma che mi fanno fare quindi lì mi aspetto dei
risultati” (these are therapies that I did not choose myself but that the made me do
therefore in that case I expect results). In this mother’s explanation, it almost seems that
parents’ choice negates the therapeutic nature of the intervention. Therapies are things
254
that are assigned by professionals. Things that are chosen by parents are not so
therapeutic. This is by no means true of everyone, but as a thread in the discourse it
further demonstrates parents search for competent, trained experts to make those choices.
Furthermore, although some parents did use music, art, sports, and animal therapies for
concrete therapeutic outcomes related to autonomy, self-expression, and the like, there
was also a parent version of professionals’ “pleasant experiences” that are not “true and
proper therapies” (as described in Chapter 4). For parents this category came in the form
of “hobby” or “doing something they like.”
School
This subsection considers the scholastic context, adding an experience-near
perspective to the schooling policies described in Chapter 2 and further demonstrated
parents search for competent professionals. Schooling was a particular problem for many
parents. It depended a lot on what kind of teachers and school principals these families
encountered. Some had good experiences, others bad. As with therapists, parents’
complements and criticisms demonstrated the importance of competence and preparation
for professionals. Complementary terms included esperta (expert), professionale
(professional), attenta (attentive), in gamba (on the ball, lit. “in leg”), informata
(informed), sensibile (sensitive). Critical terms included ignorante (ignorant), non
preparata (unprepared), chiusa (closed), and incompetente (incompetent). A lot depended
on the person with whom people with autism worked. Parents sought competent persons,
as in the therapists above. For some, competence simply was not found in the schools but
was found in the services their children attended later, the next section.
Relationships with classroom teachers, support teachers, and educatori were
255
extremely important to students with autism, as will be discussed in more detail in
Chapter 6. As we will see in the below discussion, the quality of the teacher made an
overwhelming difference. However, when it comes to support teachers and educatori,
another problem arose – these figures changed frequently, sometimes multiple times a
year (Maggiolini and Molteni 2013). Parents often felt these changes strongly.
Integrazione was an important goal for many parents. One explained that it was
her primary goal at the school, saying, “L’obiettivo è l’inclusione sociale non la
scolarizzazione, OK?” (The goal is social inclusion not scholarly learning, OK?). Parents
praised educational professionals who created environments that favored integration,
describing for example, “degli ottimi insegnanti che hanno inserito [figlia] secondo me in
modo eccezionale all’interno della classe e nelle attività che hanno svolto” (some great
teachers that inserted [my daughter], in my opinion, in an exception way inside the class
and in the activities they undertook). Another described a teacher as a “maestra
eccezione, e riusciva a contenerlo, insomma, a farlo stare in classe. Certo, faceva ben
poco. Una scrittura molto particolare (an exceptional teacher, and she succeeded at
containing him, in short, at keeping him in the class. Certainly, he did not do very much.
A very particular writing assignment, very blunt).
In turn, parents complained about lack of integrazione or times when the law was
not applied correctly. A father at a party once pulled me aside to ask me about schooling
laws in the U.S. and tell me about his son’s experiences. He told me that given his
experiences, the law is good but the practice can be quite varied. Other parents spoke of
this tension in interviews, such as the parent who explained, “Tutti i problemi con i
famosissimi insegnanti di sostegno – che sono belle le leggi italiane, però poi dopo se
256
non si reisce ad applicare, rimangono solamente con una bella cornice” (All the
problems with the famous support teachers – the Italian laws are great, but then
afterwards if they are not applied, they remain simply a pretty frame). In such statements,
parents unequivocally state their support for the law, but identify shortcomings related to
its execution.
Most parents who talked about separate classrooms did so negatively. Parents
complained that they were a waste of time where students did not learn, where parents
did not know what students were doing, and where the teachers ignored the students. The
following parent links this complaint to the law and explains a recurring concern that
support classrooms can function as “ghettos” for children with disabilities:
What they were really doing during the support hours, that is what I was saying
earlier: now he says that the support teacher sleeps. He had many support
teachers, but the refrain is always the same. Well, the support hour is a ghetto.
That is, how does the support work? The kids are taken and placed in the support
classroom, and there they do... no one knows what. I maintain that the support is –
must be carried out in the classroom, together with the others, because the support
serves to allow the person with a disability to be integrated into the classroom,
and instead it works like this.
- Interview
Parents often echoed the complaints of this interview: that they do not know what goes
on in the support classroom, that the teachers are inattentive, and that their children are
separated from the other students.
There were, however, parents who had positive experiences with support
classrooms. One parent explained that she would have preferred a separate room:
There are not always… principals or directors that are sensitive to these problems
therefore even there I had a great struggle because in the first year of elementary
school they only gave him 6 hours of support. They did not have a class for him to
go, he had to stay in class with others and therefore in practice it was a disaster.
- Interview
257
For this parent, the support classrooms were not the problem – the regular classrooms
were. More support hours would have been preferable.
Parents praise specific programs. Sometimes professionals went to the school to
train teachers. Another preferred a special school, in part because of problems like those
discussed above:
Mother: Then he went to elementary school. I wanted him to stay with the
neurotypical children to learn to stay with them. In the fourth year of elementary
school, I was angry.
Anthropologist: Why?
M: He had very great difficulty. I took him out and sent him to a special school.
(A school for children with eye problems).
A: How did it go?
M: [….] Very well. Only two years. Then he went to middle school. I did the first
and second years at public school. A disaster. Enough. The teachers did not
succeed in managing the situation. The other mothers did not accept him. [He
was] always alone, isolated in a room with two support teachers.
- Interview, Parent at Day Center, Not Audio Recorded
Another mother who took her daughter out of school and straight into a day center
recounted a conversation she had with another mother just before hand:
And she told me this: “[Signora], do not make (for yourself) the biggest mistake
of my life, what I did, sending her to middle school, because it is nightmare.”
Because then it all depends on the teachers, then, that you find, eh?
- Interview, Parent at Day Center
Another parent expressed ambivalence about the special room:
Well, elementary school was a fairytale, Ariel, a fairytale. Because there were
these really available teachers, the children were all friends, I had children in my
home every day, he went to the parties. That it, it was all a fairytale, I tell you.
Teachers, educatori, all good. A fairytale. In middle school it went well enough in
the sense that uh in class he stayed a bit less but he had a support class where
there was a good group of kids with various types of problems, not only
intellectual disability but also the suffering of a foreign student, or an adopted kid,
or a kid with other problems. Therefore they were a good group. And I would say
that it went well enough. From the, how to say it, didactic point of view. From the
relational point of view I tell you that he lost all his friends, and therefore his
group was these 4, 5, kids here.
- Interview, Parent at Social/Recreational Activity Group
258
A few parents even filed denuncie (lawsuits or formal complaints) against schools.
Parents who had good experiences at schools often framed this situation as being
fortunate. They explained, for example, “Per fortuna gli utlimi anni abbiamo trovato
degli bravi insegnanti per cui io gli ho fatto ripetere un anno. Lui è stato inserito in una
classe in cui ha avuto davvero un grande aiuto da parte degli insegnanti” (Luckily the
last years we found some great teachers for which I made him repeat a year. He was
inserted into a class in which he truly had a great help from the teachers). With the
frequently changing staff, much varied person to person. Parents also expressed this
variation in terms of fortune, for example, “Però siamo stati fortunati con l’educatrice
perché dopo la prima questa (nome) che è stata con lui per 5 anni” (But we were
fortunate with the educatrice because after the first, this (name) that was with him for
five years), and “Terzo anno siamo stati fortunati, bravissima ragazza” (Third year we
were very fortunate, a very great girl).
Several parents work closely with the schools. One mother talked about creating
“la scuola informata” (an informed school), explaining “quindi avere dei genitori
consapevoli che a questo punto rendono consapevoli anche la scuola è stata
fondamentale” (therefore having some aware parents that at this point make the school
aware too, it was fundamental). Two parents fought to have their children graduate
normally rather than get held back or receive only a leaving certificate. Another fought to
have her daughter complete the exit exams for obligatory schooling so that she could
leave school and enter a day center after middle school, rather than completing a leaving
certificate at 18. The parent organizers at one Social/Recreational Group worked hard
with the professionals at that group and the local schools to find a secondary school that
259
would accommodate three of their children as they finished middle school this year. Their
goal was primarily social inclusion of the students. There was one dissenting voice,
parents whose child finished school after the terza media who spoke out against a friend’s
push for such inclusion:
We know people who have children with severe disabilities. And one of these
graduated from college. How much this degree was earned… for me it was gifted.
However, it did damage, in my mind. Because this youth has a degree, and
therefore has expectations, those expectations that the fact that he has that degree
gives him right to have. But unfortunately he will never have, they will never be
satisfied, those expectations. Because he lives in a context in which… mmm…
it’s not – there’s no work for the – for regular applicants. Besides then, he also has
problems that make it that his degree will never be applied in something practical.
So this youth is really – he went into crisis, he went into crisis – he’s really bad
off! If his family, instead of aiming so high, would have aimed much lower and
inserted him in maybe a work context, and by now he would may have had a job,
been satisfied.
- Interview, Parent at Day Center
Indeed, the children of many parents in this sample left school before finishing
secondary school. Eighteen parents (47%) specifically mention their children leaving the
educational before secondary schooling and going straight into day centers, residential
centers, or other similar services. Seven parents (18%) specifically reference going to
school part time and centers part time. The transition out of schooling could be very
difficult. As one parent explained, “C’è un buco nero per cui fino a 14 anni, la scuola
dell’obbligo” (there is a black hole after 14 years, after obligatory schooling).44 Parents
discuss their decisions at the end of this period. One parent explained that while in the
early years her son had substantial support, this changed as he progressed in schooling. In
See also Molteni (2011), which presents the story of Donata Pagetti Vivanti, who writes, “Eppure, spente
le candeline dei diciotto, senza una ragione plausibile perché nulla nella realtà cambia, la burocrazia
sanitaria li trasferisce in una specie di buco nero, una terra di nessuno dove letteralmente non sono più
riconosciuti” (and then, as soon as the 18th birthday candles are out, without any plausible reason because
nothing changed in the real world, the healthcare bureaucracy transfers them into a type of black hole, a noman’s land where they are literally no longer recognized).
44
260
middle school, the number of hours were reduced, and he would have only been able to
attend school for those fewer hours in which he had support. Others could progress
through middle school, but not secondary school:
For youth like [my son] there was no possibility of entering into secondary school
because in the secondary schools they demand autonomy at 80% and, that is, well,
I go and do the secondary school, that is, my son cannot go. And therefore after
four years of middle school, he was supported by [this service], by day centers.
- Interview, Parent at Day Center
There was the problem of finding somewhere that would insert him into other
types of activities. I did not want to insert him in a secondary school because
given that I work with the schools, as my job, I knew that they were not equipped
for these problems.
- Interview, Parent at Residential Center
Some parents explicitly stated that they found the Day Centers to be better equipped than
the schools. One parent told me, “preferisco questo servizio della scuola. Avrei iniziato
anche prima” (I prefer this service to the school. I would have started even sooner),
citing the clear daily communication from the center via written diaries that the school
did not have, and the focus on sports for her son to let off tension. Another parent
explained in no uncertain terms:
And then nothing, I found this place because she would have needed to start
middle school, and well... what do I send her to do? It’s worse there, in the middle
schools, because already she was 13, 14 years old, it was worse. Because the little
kids are already bigger. And well… I decided to send her in a school like…. I
found this place, which was my salvation, see.
- Interview, Parent at Day Center
This chapter therefore now transitions, as did these youth, into the context of services.
Services
Parents discussed their experiences at the types of centers at which I conducted
participant-observation (Day, Residential, and Social/Recreational) as well as temporary
residential services providing weekends away, and a professional school. As in all
261
interactions with professionals, parents used complementary terms that revealed the
characteristics they found important, including disponibile or disponibilissima
(available), attenta (attentive), preparata (prepared), ingambissima (on the ball, lit “very
in leg”), and benformate (well trained).
Parents heard about services in a variety of ways, as expressed in Table 13. While
the narrative below addresses parents’ experiences in the services from which they were
recruited and other services they encountered, this table refers only to the services from
which they were recruited.
Table 13: How Parents Heard About Services
Heard About
Comune/Assistente Sociale
Physician or Therapist
Parents’ Association
School
Parent of Another Attendee
Friend or Family Member
Involved in Founding it
Previous Service
Internet
Random Encounter
Day
Center
(N=16*)
8
3
1
3
1
1
0
1
0
0
Residential
Center
(N=11)
0
2
4
0
2
3
1
1
0
0
Social/Recreational
Activity Center
(N=12)
0
2
1
2
1
0
3
0
2
2
Total
(N=38)
8
7
6
5
4
4
4
2
2
2
* Column totals exceed participant totals because some participants heard about the service from more than
one source
I wanted to understand why parents chose these specific places. Parents contrasted
them with other places (both schools and other services). One father stresses the
importance of respect for parents. He recounted an argument he had with a psychologist
who wanted to send his daughter to a place he had visited and determined would not be
ideal for his daughter. He explained that he was looking for a center where there were
some other youth who had slightly superior skills to his daughter, because she imitates a
lot, and he did not want her to regress by imitating only those who had fewer skills. In the
262
following passage he explains his disagreement with the psychologist and contrasts it
with his experience at his daughter’s current day center.
And in that structure I maintained that the situation for [my daughter] – not
because they didn’t do their jobs well but because that structure, in my mind, was
not compatible with [my daughter]’s pathology. And well, I refused it. And I had
a conflict with this [psychologist] because she believed that she was the best, the
person that knew what to say, […] she allowed herself to judge a girl that she had
seen a half hour but I knew her for her whole life. Allow that a parent could better
know the problems of his daughter. Then here I can listen and the doctor the
people that are here at the center, they have already known her for several years.
If they tell me something, I listen because I know that they live it daily. Since I
have faith in them I say, “OK.” [….] But if you believe that you understand a
person because of one single meeting that you had, and to send her where you
believe is best because there they have a place, there they have a situation that you
find comfortable, no, this, no.
- Interview, Parent at Day Center
Many parents talk about finding a center dedicated to autism and contrast it with
previous centers that were not autism specific. These centers could be problematic both
for lack of staff trained in autism specifically, and for the presence of other youth whose
behaviors could be especially difficult for youth with autism to handle. One couple whose
child attended a residential center similarly explained that at a previous, mixed-disability
day center, despite having great staff was a place “Dove si mettavano tutte le sitauzioni
grave ma anche poi uno disturbava qual’altro in sostanza” (where they put all the
serious situations together but also then one disturbed another, basically). Another couple
at a day center described a similar experience. The mother explained of a previous day
center, “Non è andata molto bene perché c’erano gli spazi che non erano adeguati,
c’erano operatori che non erano adeguati… ehm… c’erano patologie fianco a fianco che
facevano scintille, per cui assolutamente non è andata bene. Da lì è arrivato qua” (It did
not go well because there were inadequate spaces, there were inadequate staff members,
there were people with other pathologies side by side that made sparks, so it absolutely
263
did not go well. From there he came here). This couple explained that things went much
better here, and that their son regained some skills he had lost in other centers after
middle school. I asked how this center was different, and the father explained, “qui ci
siano le competenze per trattare l’autismo” (here they are the skills for treating autism)
whereas in the other services, “hanno competenze per i Down, per… cioè, per quelle
patologie che in realtà non sono molto problematiche. Cioè, il Down è così e… non crea
problemi” (they had the skills for Down Syndrome, for... that is, for those pathologies
that in reality are not so problematic. That is, Down Syndrome is like this and… it
doesn’t create problems). Parents often invoked a comparison between autism and Down
Syndrome, and often argued that autism is much more problematic.45 One mother
describes her evaluations after her son was rejected from a mixed-disability residence for
his behavior. She explained: “Secondo me è opportuno inserire ragazzi autistici in
strutture specializzate. Sopratutto perché gi educatori sono preparati specificatamente
per lavorare con ragazzi autistici. Le attività sono adeguate ai ragazzi autistici. Gli altri
genitori hanno tutti ragazzi autistici per cui nessuno si lamenta di eventuali crisi, di
eventuali aggressioni, ecc” (In my opinion it is opportune to insert youth with autism in
specialized structures. Above all because the educatori are specifically prepared to work
with youth with autism. The activities are adequate for youth with autism. The other
parents all have children with autism so no one complains about any crises, any
aggression, etc.).
The issue of autism specificity is a complicated one, not appealing to all parents.
45
As Seltzer and colleagues (2004) note, families in which a member has Down Syndrome are commonly
used as a comparison group in research on families in which a member has autism.
264
A mother at an autism-specific day center told me, “Non è giusto che siano soli autistici.
Meglio che ci possano essere chi che parla, ecc. Sopratutto Down, che sono più svegli”
(It’s not right that they are only people with autism. It would be better if there could be
someone who talks, etc. Those with Down Syndrome, above all, who are more alert).
The isolation of people with autism, I don’t find it right, because in my opinion if
my son must hang out with the others, maybe one who has a little bit of a learning
disability, another who has a physical disability, etc., he integrates much better
than when it is just between them. Because each of them is isolated enough [….]
but here [at the day center], let’s say, for the instruction like this, fine, but for the
vacations, or also if there were to be a residence, I think it would be better if they
were mixed, in my opinion.
- Interview, Parent at Day Center
Some parents, especially at Social/Recreational Groups, talk about tension upon
first considering an autism-focused place. Both of the parents below had previously
attempted to give their children opportunities to socialize in contexts that had nothing to
do with disability, such as the oratorio – Catholic youth centers especially active during
summer break as day camps and central to Italian youth experience.
In the beginning for [my husband] let’s say, the idea of putting [our daughter] in
an environment of people with Asperger’s Syndrome was a way of ghettoizing
her. He did not manage to see the possibility on the other hand, finally, of an
interaction for her. Ok? When finally then he came and noted that finally his
daughter interacted with someone.
- Interview, Parent at Social/Recreational Group
But I looked at it, I said no, because my impression that I have always had was,
let’s make him go both to places where there are people like him but also places
where there are people that are different from him. After that, (look), I understood
that it did not work that way, that with the “normal” people, so to speak, he does
not manage. And it stresses me out, this [service], as the first idea that I saw, or I
talked with someone, because I had the experience of my friend who started with
an association for children with Down Syndrome from which she escaped
because, she told me, and this (they experienced little), all the parents talk about
the same problem, in my opinion it does not relieve the spirit. Look. It stresses me
out more. And so at first I said no.
- Interview, parent at Social/Recreational Group
265
Parents found other positives in the services, praising them for good communication, for
making the parents feel supported and in good company, and for the careful structuring of
the environment. For example:
I was astonished because I found a lot of silence. Something that wasn’t at [a
previous service], where there was a confusion (inaudible). I entered and I said:
this seems to be almost a paradise here, for these youth, I promise! Because there
it was an undefinable chaos. And I was saying, “How can it be that these youth
with autism are busily working here?” Because there I saw them at their table,
where they were doing things, I do not know what, however… very contained,
very busy. Look, this was my first impression: The silence, of these youth,
occupied at the tables and all silent. The educatori that floated between the
tables… this was my impression. Because the confusion really bothers [my son],
the noise… and I said, “ah, maybe this is the right place for him.”
- Interview, Parent at Day Center
One residential center was run by parents of people with autism, which parents in my
sample found particularly reassuring to have it “gestita dai genitori o dai fratelli [….]in
mano di noi dirattamente interessate” (managed by parents and by siblings [….] in the
hand of we who are directly invested in it). At a Day Center, one parent specifically
praised the fact that the staff were parents, though not of children with disabilities.
Some parents praised the staff for their appropriate handling of difficult patients.
For example, the following parent contrasts her daughter’s day center with her earlier
experiences at school.
[At school it went] poorly. She was promoted. In reality she arrived here not
knowing how to read, the write, nothing [….] It was lost time. Always arguments
with professors, fights, hyperactive, not staying at her desk. She was convinced
that the only thing to do was to throw herself to the ground. The school called me,
I picked her up. I explained to the educatori, and they told me no, the girl will be
re-taught 100%.
- Interview, Parent at Day Center, Not Audio Recorded
A parent at a residential center framed this situation in terms of the daughter’s anxieties.
When she first entered the residence, her daughter had a very hard time, expressing her
266
discomfort by destroying everything she could. Her parents explained:
However she certainly found a prepared staff that at any rate in the face of these
things were never, in quotation marks, “scared” or… because then her worry was
always saying “they don’t want me anymore, they are sending me away.” Because
her experience was that she acts like this and then gets sent away. Sent away from
the school, from the center where she was, from the day center, from us, therefore
sent away.
- Interview, Parent at Residential Center
Unlike these previous settings, this residential center, however, did not send her away.
Rather, according to these parents, they said, “siamo in difficoltà ma vediamo ritrovare la
strada per uscirne” (we are having trouble but let’s look to finding the route to follow).
This perpective was important to her parents, who explained, “Quindi anche nei momenti
difficili abbiamo avuto questa percezione, e poi abbiamo visto che non era sola una
percezione ma che era così, che era personale preparato che sanno cosa stanno facendo.
Perché non si sono arrese. Ecco questo, e questo ci ha detto la convinzione”(Therefore
even in difficult moments we had this perception, and then we saw that it was not just a
perception but that it was like this, that the staff was prepared, that they know what they
are doing. Because they did not stop. See, this, and this gave us the conviction).
True criticisms were rare, and parents rarely even volunteered things they would
like to go differently. There is, of course, a notable potential for bias since I recruited
interview participants through services. Parents’ complaints related to staff changes,
resources, and once just the other youth at the center. Parents almost always followed up
with statements explaining that they understood why staff did what they did, just as staff
often followed up their rare complaints about parents with similar comments of
understanding. Parents expressed their desire for more activities, for specific other
activities, or for more direct involvement of their specific children in activities. They
267
spoke about regretting the loss of staff who had gone elsewhere. One parent wondered if
maybe the other opsiti were too loud and created a bothersome environment, but noted it
was not their fault but the fault of their “pathologies.” Another parent similar wondered,
though not as a criticism, about the range of the spectrum in one service, asking herself,
“è giusto che condivide la sua vita con così tanti ragazzi che hanno capacità magari
inferiori alle sue?” (Is it right that he shares his live with so many kids that have skills
that might be below his own?) but then explains, “in un minuto, mi do la risposta, nel
senso che [figlio] si trova benissimo, perché in realtà anche lui ha un problema di
autismo grosso. Però per certi aspetti sembra più evoluto. Invece in realtà ci sono altri
ragazzi che magari sembrano meno evoluti di [figlio] che hanno delle capacità cognitive
molto più alte di [figlio] e che sanno magari sviluppare un pensiero molto meglio di lui”
(in a minute, I answer myself, in the sense that [my son] does really well there, because in
reality even he has a serious problem of autism. In some ways he seems more advanced.
But in reality there are other kids that maybe seem less advanced than [my son] who have
much higher cognitive skills than (my son) and maybe know how to develop their
thoughts much better than he does).
Other wishes related to specific goals for participation, to which this chapter now
turns. Table 14 outlines parents’ goals for participating in the services from which they
are recruited, although the narrative draws more broadly also from other services and
what we can learn about parent goals from comparisons and contrasts with the services
from which they were recruited.
268
Table 14: Parent Goals for Participation
Goals for Participation
1. Autonomy
1. Close to Home
1. Learn and Grow
1. Something to Do
2. Child is Content
2. Il Dopo di Noi/Distacco
2. Too Difficult to Manage at
Home
2. Service Becomes Home
2. Child is Respected
3. Child Feels Understood
3. Interact with Others
3. Learn Social Skills
3. Make Friends
3. Physical Activity
3. Social Inclusion
3. Support Group
Day
Center
(N=16*)
7
2
4
3
3
2
0
Residential
Center
(N=11)
3
0
3
1
7
3
2
Social/Recreational
Activity Center
(N=12)
1
0
0
2
1
1
0
Total
(N=38)
0
0
0
1
0
0
1
0
0
1
1
0
3
0
1
0
0
0
0
0
1
5
1
6
2
1
1
1
1
1
9
1
7
3
1
1
11
2
7
6
11
6
2
* Column totals exceed participant totals because some participants heard about the service from more than
one source
This table breaks down parents’ responses to questions about their goals for the
service and how they started using it by type of service. This is not to say that parents did
not achieve, for example, greater autonomy in other services – just that they did not list it
as an initial goal. This table shows that goals varied by type of site. I have loosely
clustered these goals into three categories which were associated with different types of
sites, in line with their purpose. Common goals for attending day centers including
learning and growing, increasing autonomy, and having “un impegno” (an
obligation/commitment). Common goals at Residential Centers included having their
child be content and addressing the issue of “dopo di noi” (after us). Common goals at
Social/Recreational Groups were indeed more social: making friends, interacting with
others, being understood, and having a support group. This subsection further unpacks
269
these goals, looking at parents’ narratives about services more holistically, including their
stated goals, the outcomes they have observed, and their wishes – things they would like
services to do that they do not currently do.
Some parents listed autonomy as their goal, and others talk about the autonomy
their children learned at the service beyond their expectations. Learning and growing are
a part of this, too, although may related not as explicitly to autonomy but to behaviors.
Another parent lists some specific skills, including hanging laundry and setting the table.
Sometimes Social/Recreational Groups provided the opportunity for people with
autism to practice autonomy in a more natural setting. Two parents of children at
Social/Recreational Groups specifically mention the importance of their children having
somewhere to go autonomously, a space that is all their own.
Several aspects of Day Centers aligned with these goals. As described in Chapter
3, youth with autism generally arrived at Day Centers with their parents or via shuttle
busses. Although each Day Centers had different physical structures and different ways
that physical space was divided, they also had several features in common. All of the Day
Centers had copious amounts of visual cues. Signs on doors explained visually what was
on the other side. Signs on drawers and cabinets showed their contents. Agendas lined the
walls, one for each person explaining the order of events for the day. Pictures of each
ospite identified their locker or coathook. Each had two or more rooms where the ospiti
spent their free time or waiting time during the day. Often each ospite had a designated
desk, sometimes with their name or favored free time items. Some free time spaces were
less structured, with couches and lounge chairs and shelves of games, books, and
activities. For some ospite, these spaces wer more private, with separate rooms or
270
cubicles that were open to the main space but provided some personal space from the
others for those who were bothered by the group setting. Each Day Center also had
separate work stations for lavoro indipendente, as described in Chapter 4. In either a
separate room or a dedicated table in the larger room, each Day Center had dining tables
for lunch as well as other group activities (such as cooking workshop).
People with autism did not necessarily spend all day in the physical Day Center
itself, but might travel around the community by foot or by public transit. What was it
like traveling on the public transit system with people with developmental disabilities?
People with disabilities are entitled to a reduced fare bus pass, which our group used. We
also used the wheelchair accessible entrance, which allowed us to enter as a group and
avoided any problems anyone might have using the turnstyles in the other entry lanes.
Most of the people in our group, like many people with autism, did not have any visible
markers of disability. Although some of the ospiti engaged in slightly “off” behavior such
as rocking or shaking their heads, or even just wore sweat pants in a city that did often
consider sweats and sneakers inappropriate for riding public transit,46 only a few people
ever stared and people seemed to sit next to the ospiti without problems. Once we met
another day center group, and one of the Day Center's staff asked one of their staff where
they were coming from. Another time, a woman on a bus struck up a conversation with
Day Center staff about where we had been as a group. We encountered some transit
annoyances such as musicians and panhandlers, but with minimal disruption to the ospiti.
The staff asked specific ospiti to announce when our stop was arriving one the metro, to
46
Some former roommates of mine – young professionals like myself – once told me how surprised
(bordering on scandalized) they were when the visited the United States and saw people wear
sneakers on the subway. They told me sneakers were only acceptable for the gym.
271
read the time remaining listed on the electronic signs that tracked the buses, or to push
the stop requested button on the bus. In the latter case, someone explained that he could
do it because of the routine more than because he knew how to operate the bus. However,
it was still a sort of travel training.
Another goal at Day Centers was giving young adults something to do or provide
the rhythm of a normal life. This concept of the normal life also ties into a life cycle
perspective. For adolescents, some parents who talked about this wanted them to have a
peer group and a gradual distacco from their parents, much like professionals. For adults
some parents talked about having something to do during the day and, perhaps,
somewhere to go at night:
I hope that in this way my son is occupied. Doing something that he likes. And
being with others. And doing sports and… and… and becoming in this way
always more autonomous.
- Interview, Parent at Day Center
This theme appeared regarding Residential Centers as well, and at Social/Recreational
Groups parents talked about looking for their children to have somewhere to go and not
be stuck at home all day especially on weekends:
He likes it. Initially he sometimes did not want to go there, up when… yeah.
Then… bit by bit because first indeed he had trouble even at really… at needing
to go out at that specific moment. Instead then it became even a routine,
something habitual, therefore… he is a bit used to it, no?, to having this
commitment.
- Interview, Parent at Social/Recreational Group
Though one mother wished the Social/Recreational Group could provide something a
little more of an obligation:
I like it all. Look. Just that he does something, sees his interests [….] Then for the
rest I would like indeed that he could do something, not for pay, for goodness
sake, but to do something that isn’t just for fun [….] If there could be something,
not just for fun, maybe… he could do something of a commitment like more if, a
272
real commitment, maybe it could be useful, like I do not know if something like
this has ever been thought up.
- Interview, Parent at Social/Recreational Group
This theme resonates with Mehrotra and Vaidya’s (2008) report that day centers in Italia
give men with autism “something to do.”
Attendance at Day and Residential Centers could be normalized through analogy
to school or work, the type of commitments age mates usually had. Parents reported that
their children referred to both residential and day centers as “scuola” (school).
Although parents often removed youth from services if the youth expressed strong
and consistent resistance to going, some amount of resistance could be normalized as
well. One parent at a Day Center told me, ‘Ci sono giornate che non vuole venire, come
uno non vuole andare a scuola, non vuole andare al lavoro’ (there are days that he does
not want to come, like one does not want to go to school, does not want to go to work),
and another, “come magari tutte le persone che o vanno a scuola o vanno a lavorare
rientrano con un umore particolare un giorno (laughing) però in genere sempre
tranquilla, serena” (like maybe everyone who goes either to school or to work, they
come home with a particular mood one day (laughing) but in generally she is tranquil,
serene).
Parents might also normalize residential centers as homes, explicitly linking them
to life cycle goals of independent living in adulthood. One parent at a residential center
explained, “Ha la sua vita, pratticamente, come se fosse sposata, no? (laughing) è uscita
dalla [casa all’età], vive nella sua casa, insomma” (She has her life, practically, as
though she were married, no? (Laughing) She left [home at the age], living in her home,
in short). Another mother told me with pride, “per lui la sua casa è questa qua. Lui dice
273
che ha quattro case” (for him his home is this one here. He says that he has four homes),
refering to his natal home, vacation homes, and current residence. She continued “Però
lui si chiama “casa” adesso questo posto qua” (but now he calls ‘home’ this place here).
Similarly, a mother expressed her hope for achieving this sense of home:
I hope that it becomes a bit like his home, for him. Now it’s a little early [….]
[Service] represents his workplace, where he goes to work Monday through
Friday. [….] For me it means that [my son] uses it, uses [service] really like his
workplace, his activity. But then there is a house where he has his possessions. In
the future it may be that he refurnishes his room [at the service]. In my opinion,
the moment he starts to refurnish his room could maybe be the moment where his
home becomes [service] but it’ll still be a while. In my opinion when he really
becomes a grown man. Bit by bit he will start this process.
- Interview, Parent at Residential Center
Temporary residences for weekends away also provided important age-based
opportunities, such as in the case of a parent who explained, “a [filgio] piace questa cosa
perché è con gli amici, è fuori casa con gli amici, no? ormai ha l’età per essere, per
vivere da solo, voglio dire. Avrebbe l’età per vivere fuori di casa quindi lo fa molto
volontiere” ([My son] likes this [temporary residence] because he is with friends, he is
away from with his friends, no?, already he is of an age to be, to live alone, I want to say.
He would be of an age to live away from home therefore he does it very willingly).
The permanent residences that I visited did a lot to accomplish these goals.
Chapter 4 discusses the ways professionals sought to integrate entry into Residential
Centers with life cycle expectations: youth with autism should not move to Residential
Centers as soon as they turn 18 because youth without autism in Italy do not move out of
their parents’ homes as soon as they turn 18. However, the autism specific Residential
Centers I visited were designed to be for adults – young adults and older – not for the
elderly. Parents and professionals in fact expressed fear of people with autism entering
274
into elder care residential facilities where they might be far younger than everyone else.
A few parents specifically mentioned how glad they were that their young adult children
could be with people – both other residents and staff – who were their own age.
Both of the residential centers I visited were far from city centers – one in a small
residential neighborhood at the edge of the city, and another in the country completely on
its own though near several towns. Both took a while for me to get to. When they had to
leave home, the residents generally took shuttle busses or traveled with staff to go to Day
Centers or activities; and generally left with their parents if they were visiting their
parents’ home for the weekend or holidays. Parents sometimes remarked on the distance,
although it could be both a positive and a negative. One parent repeatedly lamented that
there were no autism-specific residences closer. Yet others considered city life too
overwhelming for their children, and were glad they could have the respite. Staff and
parents from both Residential Centers stressed how these were the residents’ homes.
These homes were very private, in the sense that they were generally occupied solely by
the residents, staff, and tironcinante but not other family members, friends, or visitors.
Residents had single or double rooms which some furnished more than others. While a
few residents were very eager to show me their rooms, for the most part they were kept
private. In terms of common spaces, Residential Center 1 had two floors, a balcony from
which residents sometimes greeted guests, a kitchen, a living room, a patio, and a second
floor recreational room. Residential Center 2 had three separate apartment buildings, with
a dining room/kitchen, living room, and backyard. These apartment buildings were the
true homes of the residents where only those individuals who lived there, and staff, had
breakfast, dinner, snacks, and evening leisure time. Residents from all three buildings
275
came together in common spaces such as the large dining room for lunch, the large
common room for various activities, laboratories for various workshops, and outdoor
spaces for trekking and agricultural activities. In these ways, the physical space of the
Residential Centers was intentionally structured to reflect homely ideals and resist the
institutional feel that professionals and parents so often lamented and feared. Like the
Day Centers, Residential Center 1 had several visual cues – photographs on cabinets of
what was inside, photographs on doors of what is on the other side, color coded
bedrooms, agendas on the wall. Residential Center 2, which followed an ecological
approach, did not. Rather, a schedule was posted on a bulletin board that residents (at
least those who could read) consulted as well as staff. Residents who did not read asked
staff about the schedule or followed staff instructions.
Though residential placements could be normalized, this is not to say that they
were not tricky. Sometimes, though rarely, it was that children were too difficult to
manage at home. Parents expressed a lot of anguish over this decision. For example:
And therefore since I was left pretty alone and I was... I was no longer able to
manage the situation, I knew it. I was doing poorly, he was doing poorly, we were
all doing poorly. And nothing, I took him to [a residential community]. And in
effect after a week there, things were decidedly better. He was decidedly better.
Therefore I said, “Fine, you did a week of monitoring him, now let’s try again at
home.” We had not even arrived at the house, it was all like before. Everything
like before. In fact I remember that I held out a month, I held firm […] in the end
I gave up, I said that “let’s send him to the community” even if I was not in any
way in agreement, because I did not want to separate myself from a 16 year old
kid. But I did not see any alternative, because since they told that medication
wouldn’t do anything, one couldn’t calm him, and it was true.
- Interview, Parent at Day Center
While describing this difficulty decision, many parents linked their child’s move into a
residence to the life cycle:
Then you know folk, relatives, some [say] “Ah! You put her in an institution, eh!,
276
how come!” I mean, an institution that then, fine, let’s say that they community is
something else. Fine, but I … these things are not important to me, what is
important is [my daughter’s] choice, and for her it was the right thing, look [….]
If I must be sincere, when we went to take [my daughter] here as she is, the next
step was going to be being having her able to come home. That [my daughter]
could return home. With us. That she could stay with us. However since we had
already made this step, and maybe as a mother I also understand that always
staying with mother and father isn’t the right thing. Maybe it seems like it to us,
but not for her.
- Interview, Parent at Residential Center
The need for an adult life ties into other goals of addressing the “dopo di noi” and to
ensuring their adult children have better lives.
The term “dopo di noi” translates as “after us” and refers to the problem of
ensuring the care and quality of life of people with autism, especially those with high
levels of support needs, after their parents age beyond the ability to care for them.
Professional and advocacy circles and documents tended to phrase the problem in this
way. Parents phrased this problem in a number of ways, most prominently “quando non
ci saremo più noi” (when we are no longer here). When I asked parents about their fears
and worries, 27 of 3647 (75%) offered some version of “the future,” “a later tomorrow,”
“after us,” “when we are no longer here,” or “when we die.”
Both permanent and temporary residential experiences allowed parents to feel
their child was more prepared for this eventuality. One parent at a residential center
explained that many families find themselves in trouble when the parents die and the
adult child, in his 50s, must live without them for the first time. This parent explained,
“Per cui la mia idea era fare qualcosa ma intanto che ci siamo noi. Non aspettare che
non siamo più in grado noi da seguire” (and so my idea was to do something but while
47
In two interviews, these direct questions were not asked.
277
we are still here. Not to wait until we are no longer able to care for him). Another parent
hoping to get her son involved in a residence explained, “Così quando succederà un
qualcosa, non è uno shock. Giusto? Diventa una cosa che lui ha già provato. Poi gli
mancherà la mamma, gli mancherà il papà però lui ha già provato. Non deve essere una
cosa che si trova lì da un momento all’altro e finisce in un istituto di quelli... che vedi in
televisione” (This way when something happens, it isn’t a shock. Right? It becomes
something that he already tried. Then he will miss his mother, his father, but he will have
already tried it. It doesn’t have to be something that one finds there in the moment and he
ends up in an institution… that you see in television).
In both this planned for future and the present, parents spoke particularly of being
sure their child was content – contento, sereno, tranquillo. For example:
Father: The goals are that she becomes always more autonomous and carries out
her life, and has also a good future. These are the goals.
Mother: Serene, and that she is always happy to come here. Because other places
where… it’s not possible. Even their rhythms, their daily life.
- Interview, Parents at Residential Center
The words sereno and tranquillo and their derivatives appeared time and time again in
parent narratives. The theme of serenità was especially strong also when I asked parents
about their expectations and hopes for the future. One parent explained it simply, “un po’
di tranquillità” (a little tranquility). The tranquility or serenity of youth with autism
generated tranquility or serenity for their parents, who knew they were going to be OK:
I would like something either like this or an apartment, protected, however, in the
sense that he cannot stay alone, obviously. Something… I would like… to be
tranquil, to say OK, my son can stay by himself.
- Interview, Parent at Social/Recreational Activity Group
One couple explained that the Italian word serenità could have a double meaning,
because it could also be used to refer to sedation (although I personally never heard it
278
used that way). They explained further:
We are talking of psychological serenity, that is, the capacity to feel safe and
therefore to be able to preview that happens and therefore be able to choose. And
it is not just learning but I believe really being able to confront choices and realize
things, new behaviors, habits [….] Everyone, I believe, we are very convinced in
this family that it is worth a lot even for so-called normal people. They are
normal, maybe very intelligent, paralyzed by anxiety, nervousness, incapacity to
decide. In someone with autism, oh, you so it so much more!
- Interview, Parents at Residential Center
If day centers were primarily associated with learning skills and autonomy, and
residential centers were primarily associated with having a good quality of life “dopo di
noi,” Social/Recreational Centers were most associated with building a social life.
Parents talked about wanting their children to make friends:
The main goal is – [my son] is alone, he needs a friend group therefore a nice
friend group. That is, he waits for Friday to see his friends and his team sport,
staying together. And going out alone without parents. It makes him as
autonomous as possible. Therefore the reaching of a distance, of an autonomy, but
above all feeling good, that is, my friend group, ok? Even I have my friend group.
- Interview, Parent at Social/Recreational Group
Parents also talked of the general need for a peer group:
Fortunately there is [this service] which is a very positive thing for him and on
Sunday at least he has a moment to hangout with other people, with other kids.
And I see him very content [….] Really immediately I understood that he
understood. Because when I said, “look, let’s go to a place where there are kids
that have a little trouble making friends, like you.” It’s enough. He was at his…
maybe he felt equal to the others.
- Interview, Parent at Social/Recreational Group
Social/Recreational Groups did a lot to accomplish this goal. They met largely in
evenings and weekends, times when youth were free from their schedules at work,
school, or Day Centers. The average age of participants at Social/Recreational Groups
was younger than at Day or Residential Centers, and many were still attending school.
Some parents dropped their children off, and talked to the staff at drop off and pick up.
279
Other youth came to the Social/Recreational Groups alone, by car, public transit,
walking, or biking. Getting to the Group may also be an opportunity to autonomy in
practice. Some parents were involved in the Groups as staff or volunteers, but
distinguished this from their role as parents of an individual youth in order to serve the
whole group during this time.
Both Social/Recreational Groups had a lot of visual cues as well, generally in the
forms of rules for participation. These rules may be posted on the wall or carried by staff
in large binders. Unlike Day and Residential Centers, there were very few, if any,
assigned desks or spaces at Social/Recreational Groups. All spaces were common spaces.
At Social/Recreational Group 1, the commonality of spaces was even more true in that
sports practices were held in reserved pools or courts in public recreational centers, so
that youth shared the space not only with each other but also with others who used the
locker rooms and practiced before or after them. At Social/Recreational Group 2, most
activities were held within the headquarters which was divided into several spaces with
dedicated purposes: a conversation corner with comfortable chairs and couches, a comic
book library, a board game library, a computer area, a kitchenette, and larger areas for
theatre practice and so on. At Social/Recreational Group 1, youth followed the directions
of a couch or staff member. At Social/Recreational Group 2, some youth participated in
guided activities and some times were entirely dedicated to such activities, but at other
times some or all youth could choose activities freely and spend time wherever they
would like. Both groups also took many group exercusions to outside activities near and
far from the headquarters.
Psychologists and anthropologists alike have long argued that adolescence is
280
associated with an increased reliance on and importance of a peer group (Erikson 1994;
Amit-Talai and Liechty 1995). Staff and parents participating in this study also linked
peer group participation to the adolescent life stage, noting for example, “Ha proprio
bisogno, non solo lui, tutti come loro a quet’età, basta mamma papà. Andare con gli
amici” (He really needs it, not just him, everyone like them at that age, enough with mom
and dad! Go out with friends). Chapter 6 describes youth relationships in more detail, but
parents also reflected on their perceptions of their children’s relationships. One parent
describes her son’s friendship with other youth, noting, “Se hanno le stesse difficoltà, se
hanno le stesse caratteristiche, so che con [nome di ragazzo] si trova benissimo perché
hanno gli stessi interessi, sono usciti qualche volta anche insieme così” (If they have the
same troubles, the same characteristics, I know that with [boy’s name] he gets on really
well because they have the same interests, they have even gone out together a few times).
Another describes her son’s relationship with a supportive adult, explaining, “So che
tornava a casa anche contento di frequentare [nome di uomo], che comuqnue [nome] lo
capiva, e parlava molto con [nome] e poi con tutti suoi diciamo amici. Io ritengo che
questo centro sia indispensabile in questo caso per mio figlio, presumo anche per gli altri
ragazzi” (I know he comes home also happy to have seen [man’s name], that at any rate
[this man] understands him, and he talks a lot with [this man], and then with all of his,
let’s say, friends. I maintain that this center is indispensable in this case for my son, I
presume also for the other guys). Although these relationships were more central at
Social/Recreational Groups, parents even talked about friendships at Residential Centers.
One parent explains, “Per lei fa un riferimento unico perché anche [...] tutti questi
ragazzi della commmunità frequentono il CDD per ciò vengono insieme, tornano
281
insieme, si conoscono, pratticamente hanno fatto, fanno, amicizie” (For her it’s a singular
point of reference because also […] all these guys from the [residential] community go to
the CDD so they go together, come back together, know each other, they practically have
made, are making, friends).
Some parents noted that their children’s relationships were more with staff than
peers. One mother faced a lot of frustration with this situation. She explained that her son
went a support classroom at school, “il rapporto era con l’insegnante” (the relationship
was with the teacher) because the other kids had similar problems to her son when it
came to social interaction. She then participated in a social/recreational activity group
with her son, noting “questi ragazzi, almeno la maggior parte di loro, cercano anche
durante le gite, cioè noi appunto facevamo queste gite per fargli socializzare. Però nella
maggior parte dei casi, specialmente ragazzi più gravi, si rivolgevano sempre a noi
accompagnatori. Quindi non creavano un rapporto di amicizia fra di loro” (these kids, at
least most of them, sought even during the outings, that is, we indeed did these outings to
make them socialize. But most of the time, especially with the more several affected kids,
they interacted always with us chaperones. Therefore they did not create friendly
relationships among themselves). Despite her efforts, her son had not made friends. At
the current social/recreational activity center, he did go out a lot with a particular friend.
All tolled, however, she explained, “credo che gli faccia piacere per lo meno il rapporto
con gli animatori. Perché effettivamente a noi sembra che principalmente il dialogo è
con loro più che con i ragazzi. Quindi è sempre lo solito discorso che loro alla fine si
appoggiano all’educatori e non creano gruppo, un gruppo vero proprio” (I think he at
least enjoys the relationship with the activity leader. Because effectively it seems to us
282
that the dialogue is with them more than with the kids. Therefore it is always the same
discourse that they in the end lean on the educatori and do not make a great, a real and
true group).
For other parents, however, this pattern was not a problem. It was still nice to
know their child was interacting with others, especially peers. Parents occasionally
referred to staff and therapists, especially educatori, as amici. The relationship could be
read as analogous. One parent at a Residential Center explains, “Non lo so come l’ha
preso. Io penso bene. Perché proprio un po’ si è trovato a vivere con dei ragazzi della
sua età. Cone delle ragazze giovani, perché le educatrici comunque avevano pochi anni
più di lui” (I don’t know how he has taken it. I think well. Because really a bit he has
found himself living with youth of his own age. With young women, because the
educatrici at any rate are not many years older than him).
In addition to friendship, parents at Social/Recreational Groups also talked about
other benefits related to feeling supported and understood, “non presso in giro” (not
teased), and that “alla fine non sei solo” (in the end you are not alone). Certain activities
provided explicit opportunities to discuss their lives. As one parent explains, “Perché il
fatto di poter discutere… fino all’anno scorso c’erano film, temi corretti, adatti a loro,
nel senso che parlavano delle loro difficoltà, delle loro fragilità, delle loro… quindi poi
quando aprivano una discussione” (Because the fact of being able to discuss… until last
year there were films, correct themes, adapted for them, in the sense that they talked
about their troubles, their fragility, their… therefore then when they opened a discussion).
One mother of an adult at a Residential Center expressed a similar sentiment. She told me
about a previous service that her son attended, telling me that it was “unfortunately”
283
mixed disability. I asked her why, and she said, “perché magari potevano legare, aiutarsi
l’uno l’altro. Scambiare qualcosa, in modo loro ma magari scambiare” (because maybe
they can join together, help one another. Exchange something, in a way that maybe they
can exchange). The next chapter turns to the experiences of youth themselves.
Conclusion
This chapter has addressed research questions (1) and (2) as they relate to a
second respondent group: parents of adolescent and young adult children with autism
spectrum conditions. With respect to the conceptualization of autism, it finds that parents
defined autism in a wider variety of ways than professionals, though still often related to
the characteristics of the DSM-IV. That idea that people with autism were “in their own
world” was especially salient for parents. When talking to their children, parents often
described autism within the framework that we all have strengths and weaknesses. With
respect to the way autism should be managed, this chapter shows that parents searched
for competent experts to care for their children, but did not necessarily become experts
themselves. This finding will become relevant in Chapter 7 as well, as it relates to new
biopolitical theories about patient/parent versus professional expertise.
Parents, like professionals, spoke a lot about their expectations for their children
based on a life cycle. Many parents talked about their goals for services related to a time
“dopo di noi” (after us, i.e., after we can no longer care for him or her). Parents searched
for “un po’ di tranquilità” (a little peace) for themselves and their children in adulthood.
Parent naratives further reinforce my argument that these narratives about the life cycle
of people with autism can challenge and improve the anthropological definition of
adolescence by defining adulthood as an amalgamation of materials and roles to which all
284
individuals have a right based on the standard of their peers and society (Cascio 2015b).
These insights provide an in-depth look at parents’ experiences and evaluations of
autism-specific services. This chapter has outlined parents’ goals for their children’s
participation. In the next chapter, we look at youth’s experiences and shed some light on
whether or not these goals were reached.
285
Chapter 6: Youth with Autism
Chapter Overview
This chapter addresses the experiences of people with autism themselves. It
presents data collected through participant-observation, interviews with parents, and
individual and group interviews with people with autism themselves. Like Chapters 4 and
5, it addresses research questions (1) and (2) and is divided into two main sections.
Section 1, “What is Autism,” attempts to unpack the meaning of autism and
Asperger’s for people given this label. It begins by noting that for many participants on
the spectrum, the diagnosis of autism was in fact not meaningful, for various reasons. It
then delves into the way people on the spectrum defined autism or Asperger’s and what
characteristics or traits they identified with the diagnosis. Many parents and professionals
doubted that people with autism who had higher levels of support needs were even aware
of the idea of “having autism.” This possibility is also discussed, along with its
methodological implications.
Section 2, “Interfacing with Services,” discusses youth’s experiences in services.
It recaps parents’ goals for their children’s participation from Chapter 5 and adds to it the
youth’s individual goals. Social goals such as friendship and social interaction were most
common, and this section considers whether these goals were accomplished within
service settings. It describes participants’ relationships with staff and each other,
demonstrating that participant-staff relations often seemed to be the most important. This
chapter also discusses youth’s evaluations of professionals and services as raised in
individual and group interviews.
Taken together with Chapters 4 and 5, this chapter provides rich description of the
286
lives of people with autism and creates a strong basis on which to explore new
biopolitical theory and answer research question (3) in Chapter 7.
Section 1: What Is Autism?
As detailed in Chapter 3, this study’s research design was created with the idea of
investigating identity and subjectivity around autism. I decided to conduct participantobservation and recruit participants from autism-specific services because these would be
sites where autism was actively the topic under consideration. I could have recruited
participants and their families though hospitals, schools, the health board, or parents’
associations. However, I wanted to respect that identity is a function of the situation, not
the individual (Bucholtz and Hall 2006) by looking at situations in which autism
discourses were already mobilized. I chose to research in situations where autism was
already central to the discourse, rather than entering into people’s homes and reifying that
identity when it may not have been important to them. Furthermore, I anticipated that if I
sampled in autism-specific places I would more likely encounter people who knew they
had autism. Signs about autism were all over the place – at the entrances to many
buildings, on bulletin boards, on documentation on the wall, and on flyers – and many
people attending these centers could read.48 Many of the places I visited had the word
autism in their name, which was often prominently displayed on a welcome sign or
smaller materials. Moreover, at least three of the places I visited (including the two
Social/Recreational Groups) had books on the shelves that directly related to autism
specifically or disability generally. While many of these books were scholarly at more for
staff and parents, others were for people with autism or other disbailities themselves.
48
All of the interview participants did read.
287
Staff sometimes even spoke about autism in front of or even with the youth who attended,
although how much this practice was done or even considered acceptable varied from site
to site. Nonetheless, I naively assumed given the ubiquity of the term autism in these
settings that at least those participants who would be able to participate in semi-structured
interviews would know that autism was a “something” that they had and a reason to
attend these particular services. As we will see below, this was not always the case.
Parents and staff often doubted that the youth with autism at these sites were
really aware of or understood their diagnosis. The following discussion with two parents
presents a range of typical concerns:
Father: But I do not know if she is aware of what she does not have, of what she
lacks.
Mother: She doesn’t manage to understand.
F: For her [it is] her life, for her [it is] always a good life. A life –
M: Yes, she has her things.
F: But I do not know if she is aware that she lacks what a normal person has, see.
M: Even if in reality when we go to visit her sister who has children… but not –
F: - not so much.
M: I do not think that she manages to understand in the sense of saying “my sister
has children, has a husband, and I do not.” See, I do not think that [she has] this.
Maybe with intellectual disability…
F: I do not know what in her mind –
M: - what she thinks F: - what she thinks.
M: Because she listens.
F: She listens to everything.
M: Nothing escapes her.
- Interview, Parents at Residential Center
In this passage, not only are these parents not sure how much their daughter understands,
they are not sure what she thinks at all, although they note that she seems happy with her
life. They note also that their daughter, although she does not actively communicate
much, seems to listen and understand a lot. These parents, like others, almost doubt that
their children could be totally ignorant.
288
Other parents more strongly suspected their children did know, even if they did
not talk about it very openly. In one interview, parents reflected on earlier interactions
their daughter had had with another child with autism. Even before the parents knew the
diagnosis, the child identified herself as like this other child. The mother recounted that
her daughter “vedeva che girondolava, più di una volta, e lei mi ha detto ‘ma io sono
come la [altra ragazzina].’ Più di una volta, più di una volta. E io dicevo ‘no, la [altra
ragazzina] non parla, tu parli’ ma più di una volta” (saw that she spun, more than once,
and she told me, ‘but I am like [the other girl].’ More than once, more than once. And I
said, ‘no, the [other girl] does not speak, you speak’ but more than once). Even when they
did not suspect autism (in large part because of this notable difference in verbal ability),
“lei la vedeva che somigliava e lei diceva, ‘mamma, ma io sono come la [altra
ragazzina]?” più di una volta [...] non so perché lei si identificasse, se lei sentiva
qualcosa” (she saw her, that she was similar to her, and said ‘mamma, am I like [the
other girl]?” more than once [...] I don’t know if it was because she identified with her, if
she felt something). Another mother explained that she knew when her son become aware
of his “diversity,” his “autism” because he wrote about it, and because he went into a
period in which he became nervous, aggressive, closed to others, didn’t sleep or eat well.
Another parent linked awareness with “an unhappy life”:
I am sure that he perceives his limits in part, and in part no. Well, the more
fortunate part is that which he does not perceive, because for example I always
thought that [someone else I know with a different disability] had, would have
had such an unhappy life a little bit because he had a bad character and a little bit
because he was very very aware of having limits. And this must have really …
infuriated him.
- Interview, Parent at Residential Center
One parent reported having heard this perspective from professionals who had explained
289
“se fosse stata meno consapevole della sua difficoltà forse [si] avrebbe potuto tirarla
fuori di più, [si] avrebbe potuto magari gestirla meglio. Ma lei si rendeva conto” (if she
had been less aware of her difficulties maybe we would have been able to bring more out
in her, maybe we would have been able to manage her better. But she is aware). Some
studies have indeed linked awareness of disability and difference with stress and
psychological difficulty, though no systematic epidemiological study has confirmed as
much on a large-scale level (reviewed in Ghaziuddin, Ghaziuddin, and Greden 2002; see
also Shtayermman 2009).
Though for many this lack of knowledge was framed as a lack of awareness or
understand, a few parents mentioned actively wanting to avoid direct discussion of a
diagnosis with their children, often for fear of causing such psychological distress. For
example:
The world in which we live now, I think for someone with autism, perhaps the
fundamental thing is that they do not realize their diversity. Because the moment
in which, in which [my son], were he to become aware that he is different from
the others, it would be a lot of grief, for him, eh? And therefore it would be grief
for me too. If I must think of [my son] as unhappy with autism, I would really say
no. I would say, on the other hand, that in his naivety, he is really happy. The
problem that I have in facing the world with my son’s autism is that I am afraid
that the world does not understand him.
- Interview, Parent at Residential Center
Others were happy to talk to their children about their difficulties, but not happy with
how professionals talked about it. For example:
At one of the meetings with the commune (municipality) they asked ‘davanti da
lui’ (in front of him) ‘ma, è autistico?’ (But, is he autistic?) Then her son said
afterwards, ‘mamma, sono un mostro’ (Mamma, I’m a monster). She explained
that he uses always the most powerful words, it’s his way of saying c’è ‘qualcosa
che non va’ (there is something wrong). She asked him: gli altri bambini che hai
visto, sono mostri? (The other children that you have seen, are they monsters?) He
said no. She said then you aren’t either. But the mother was very annoyed that
they spoke this way in front of her son.
290
- Fieldnote Excerpt
This same mother explained that her son is aware of his problems, particularly that he
speaks more slowly than his peers, but was depressed as a result of this awareness.
Another mother described a similar conundrum she and her son encountered in the
subway, when he was using his transit pass for people with disabilities to exit the
turnstiles. “Non so [mio figlio] che percezione abbia della sua disabilità, non ho ancora
capito” (I don’t know what perception [my son] has of his disability, I haven’t yet
understood), she began. They went to the station and she told her son to take out his card,
but the guard told him “no no, puoi andare” (no, no, you can go ahead). When he asked
why, his mother told him, “‘ti conosce, sei già venuto con la [nome], l’educatrice.’ E
pensavo questo, no? poi dico ‘sai, magari si vede, perché vedi questo ragazzo, a 17 anni,
a bracetto con la mamma, che cammina sperso, capisci che è disabile no?’
obiettivamente lo capisci” (‘they know you, you came with [name], the educatrice.’ And
I thought as much, no? Then I say, ‘you know, maybe you can tell, because you see this
kid, 17 years old, arm in arm with his mother, who walks lost, you understand that he has
a disability, no?’ Objectively you understand). When they encountered the same situation
at an unfamiliar station this first excuse could not work, and the mother explained “lì mi
sono trovata in difficoltà. Cosa gli dicevo? Che ha visto che sei disabile?” (I found
myself in trouble. What did I tell him? That he saw that you have a disability?” Instead
she told her son that they probably just trusted him, since there are so many people, they
let him go through. She suspected, however, that her son did know.
Other youth, as discussed in Chapter 5, Section 1, Subsection “Discussing
Autism” were very aware of their autism. “È consapevole” (he knows), one couple said,
291
explaining that their son already noted certain characteristics in himself by the time they
discussed it:
We confirmed what he already noted, that he has these... these interests. That we
made him note [that] some of his peers have other interests, that he doesn’t listen
much, that he isolates himself. And then the fact that sometimes he talks to
himself. Um. Let’s say I made him note his behaviors, and we reasons about those
[…] Uh, he says “autistic traits” because once we went to an exhibit on autism on
there were these, like, these examples of behaviors of kids with autism, and he
recognized himself, he said, like, he read these things, he said, “ah, but I am also
like this. Ah, like how I do.”
- Interview, Parent at Social/Recreational Activity Group
Another parent discussed the advantage of having a diagnosis for her son:
But yes, in a sense: I have something, I know what I have, I can call it like this, it
doesn’t make me happy but at least I know what I have. I’m not deficient, because
everyone always said that people with Asperger’s Syndrome are so intelligent,
and he read some things, and I made him read also some books that we bought
[inaudible]. So it was not very simple, maybe, but it was also not so traumatic. I
think that [my son] needed to feel understood in something, that is, saying… I
have a fever, I know that I have the flue and I don’t have pneumonia, so to speak.
- Parent at Social/Recreational Activity Group
One youth pursued his own diagnosis as a teenager:
Honestly, when I was 14 years old I self-diagnosed myself as having Asperger’s
syndrome. But I kept going to this psychologist, I was thinking I had something
else. In fact, I went to the psychologist more times to guarantee that I had
Asperger’s syndrome. I had written on a piece of paper my possible, let’s say,
syndromes. I had written that I could have either obsessive-compulsive disorder
or Asperger’s Syndrome. So I went to the psychologist about a year ago and I
asked him, “but in your opinion, do have obsessive-compulsive disorder?” and he
told me, “no, absolutely not.” And like this I understood, like this I excluded one
of the two possibilities that I said, so, I have Asperger’s Syndrome because when
I was 14 I had already started… I had already made the hypothesis that I had
obsessive-compulsive disorder or Asperger’s Syndrome. And then when I was 17
I had the confirmation of Asperger’s Syndrome [….] In fact as soon as the
psychologist told me, “you have Asperger’s Syndrome,” I smiled. Yes, because I
was happy that I managed to diagnosis this thing by myself.
- Interview, Youth at Social/Recreational Group
The phenomenon of self-diagnosis was rare among my participants (this youth being the
only one), but the practice is not unheard of in Italy (Cola 2012; Valtellina 2011).
292
As discussed in Chapter 3, I identified potential participates through the staff.49
Staff identified not only which participants had autism, but which might be good
candidates for an interview based on their communication skills and styles. Only at three
sites (the two Social/Recreational Groups and one Day Center) were such interviews
possible. The staff at three of the remaining sites explained that the majority if not
entirety of their participants would not have the skills to complete such an interview,
which was narrative and open-ended, paralleling interviews with professionals and
parents. This is not surprising given that the autism-specific day centers in my fieldsite
were classified as centri diurni disabili, not centri socio-educativi, and were therefore
attended by people with autism with higher levels of support need (see Chapter 2 for an
overview of different service types). Staff stressed that even those who were quite open
and verbal may not be able to participate in an interview. Indeed, I was often struck at
how easily conversation flowed with certain youth when that conversation was on the
topic of the youth’s choice, yet abruptly stopped when it turned to another topic (or when
I, being a second language speaker, asked a question that made no grammatical or
contextual sense). One interview went this way, in which the participant declined to
respond to most questions (as participants were informed they were allowed to do at any
time) yet struck up an engaging conversation with me about the mechanics of my audiorecorder partway through.
At the final site, the coordinator I met with thought that at least a few participants
might be able to do an interview, but was concerned that the questions about autism
49
Once a parent disagreed with the staff evaluation that the child could not participate, but I followed the
staff’s determination per my research protocol and did not conduct an interview.
293
would be too indelicate to ask. Certainly it would be possible to do another type of study
including people with autism as active research participants – a customer service type
survey, for example (e.g., Ruef and Turnbull 2002). However, the central topic of this
dissertation is the nosological entity of “autism” – how it is defined, managed, and
experienced – and I therefore limited my sampling to interviews with people with autism
about autism.
As with parents (Chapter 5), not everyone I interviewed ultimately identified
themselves as having autism despite agreeing to be interviewed about it. Two youth
interviewed told me about autism as the name of the physical location of the service they
attended. This finding tie into a recent study in the United States by Jones (2012), who
asked youth with a variety of intellectual and developmental disabilities, including
autism, to talk about themselves, terms related to intellectual disability, how they know
whether someone has a disability, and if they or any of their friends had disabilities. She
found that youth with intellectual and developmental disabilities but no physical
limitations often identified disabilities are related to physical capacity.50 Only 55% of
participants with intellectual and developmental disabilities in this study identified
themselves as having a disability. The presence of youth in my study who did not identify
as having a disability is not surprising in light of these findings. Although my sample is
far too small to be comparable with Jones’s sample of 51, replicating this study in the
Italian context could answer some interesting questions about the perspectives of people
with disabilities working and living in disability-focused contexts on the concept of
50
Similarly, one youth at a day center, according to the parent, reportedly complained at times that she did
not want to go to the day center because “there are sick children and I am not sick.”
294
disability. How might these youth’s experiences and perceptions differ from Jones’s U.S.
participants, and between different contexts (inclusive schools, mixed-disability settings,
autism-specific settings)?
Seven of the ten youth who participated in either group or individual interviews
did talk at some length about the concept of autism. Like parents, there were those who
contested the diagnosis.
Well first I was diagnosed as a… as, uh, depressed. Then even before as
Asperger. Before I had attention deficit. Before that they thought I was downright
autistic. Before they thought I was a psycho, and before they thought I was
anxious, and before they thought I was neurotic [….] I wonder what they will
think I am next!
- Interview, Youth at Social/Recreational Group
This participant identified himself as a having a different diagnosis, though he was
identified by staff and parents as having an autism spectrum condition (autism or
Asperger’s), and participated in services specifically dedicated to those groups. Another
wavered a lot on his diagnosis, linking his difficulties most to physical health problems.
He told me, “Io siccome non ho un autismo grave non ho difficoltà” (Since I don’t have
serious autism, I don’t have difficulty) like others he knew and described. “Dialogo
tranquillamente con te” (I talk easily with you) and with others at the services he
attended. Indeed, the Asperger’s label importantly gave some youth a reason to attend
services, even if they remained ambivalent about their identification with it. This youth
explained, “io in questa fascia qua non rienterei nella fascia degli Asperger. Abbiamo
detto che ero Asperger però era un modo per farmi ammettere al [servizio]” (In this
category here, I am not part of the Asperger’s category. We said that I had Asperger’s but
it was a way to let me into the [service].” Two participants mentioned that the term
“Asperger’s” struck them as particularly useless. The suspected doctors gave people this
295
diagnosis when they did not know what else to do. They noted a wide range of symptoms
and the fact that people with the diagnosis were very different from one another, noting
“non esiste neanche una sindrome di Asperger. Funziona ognuno in un modo diverso”
(there doesn’t exist one Asperger’s Syndrome. Every person functions in a different
way). The diagnosis of Asperger’s became “la cosa più generale di tutte” (the most
general thing of all). The following excerpt from a focus group demonstrates differing
perspectives. Two of the youth bounce off of one another, explaining in detail these
doubts about Asperger’s, then a third jumps in to say that he does, in fact, have
something to say about what he sees as his characteristics as a result of Asperger’s.
R2: In my opinion, Ariel [...] Asperger’s Syndrome does not exist. That is,
someone that is defined as different from the others or in practice they do not
even know what they are talking about any more, they say he has Asperger’s
Syndrome. I sincerely do not believe that I have Asperger’s Syndrome51 also
because if (you go to read) the symptoms of Asperger’s Syndrome, practically the
whole world has Asperger’s Syndrome. Given that there are (so many) symptoms,
50 symptoms of Asperger’s Syndrome.
R3: [….] Everything that is not a symptom of something else and even some
which are symptoms are something else are symptoms of Asperger.
R2: If you are too smart you have Asperger’s Syndrome [….] If you do poorly in
physical education, you can have Asperger’s syndrome. If you look people in the
eyes when you speak, you can have Asperger’s syndrome, if you do not look at
them, you can have Asperger’s Syndrome. If you are a geek you can have
Asperger’s Syndrome. You can have Asperger’s Syndrome for anything.
R3: If you are disorganized you have Asperger’s Syndrome, if you are too
organiz--- If you are methodical, you have Asperger’s Syndrome
R2: If you write poorly, you have Asperger’s Syndrome. Anything. I really
believe that I… do not have Asperger’s Syndrome. I –
R3: If you have interests, if you have some interests, if you have interests that are
very part-, if you have particular interests that you like more than other people do,
you have Asperger’s Syndrome
R4: Look –
R2: Yes, if you don’t like to bond with other people because you simply consider
them inferior to your own intellectual level and so very stupid as to not
understand anything that you do –
At other times, this participant does state that he has Asperger’s and identifies quite strongly with the
term, further demonstrating the performative, flexible, and interpersonal nature of identity.
51
296
R3: […] You aren’t Asperger, in that case if (inaudible) because you consider
others inferior to you, you are simply a sociopath.
R2: But I am considered Asperger. And just because, (someone) gets taken to a
psychologist from childhood, but just because, like, you don’t like going out with
the other children or you don’t… you don’t want to do things, you don’t follow
the rules very well, etc.
R3: If you have an opinion that your parents don’t like, you get taken to a
psychologist and (they won’t rest until they classify you) as something. Given that
there is no pathology for classifying just a different opinion, you end up classified
as Asperger because the psychologists, being at any rate of the doctors, have
professional information that is pathological enough that they must classify
everything like (a common series, they put) labels on everyone. Already with
everyone (inaudible) they stop considering patients as people, just as piles of
symptoms.
A: R4, what do you think?
R4: What?
R2: In fact, like when R3 said it, I also feel like a new human species, I have also
had this thought. Of being a homo sapiens sapiens sapiens. A new species of
human being.
R4: R2, excuse me.
A: Thank you for the comments, but yes, wait for R4.
R4: Thank you, Ariel, but personally [I would define it as] a mania for certain
things, for example I like war themed video games, films, especially those of Mel
Gibson.
- Youth at Social/Recreational Activity Group
The last lad goes on to describe his special interests, his anxiety and related trouble
sleeping, and accusations by his father that he is clumsy and disorganized. The final
participant in this focus group added yet another perspective, explaining “io invece penso
di non essere molto Asperger” (I on the other hand do not think I am very Asperger).
While he acknowledged that he does not always look people in the eyes when they talk,
he stated, “Per il resto... non sento proprio Asperger perché, boh, sono normale” (For the
rest… I don’t really feel Asperger because, I don’t know, I am normal). He explained that
there were those who were “more Asperger-y” than him.
The definition of autism could also be contested dialogically. In the focus group
excerpt above, one participant tells another that the trait he describes is not Asperger’s
297
syndrome, but sociopathy. At another point he tells the same youth that his special
interest is not “being autistic” but rather “being a nerd.” In an individual interview,
another youth talked about having autistic traits, but not pure autism. This youth further
explained that he saw himself as “normal” and wanted to be seen as such. Three of the
youth specifically explain that they see themselves as normal, therefore taking a
perspective against a deviance narrative of autism as something which makes them not
normal as people, though they might draw from a deficit narrative i.e. there are things I
am bad at (Jones 2012). Particularly for these highly verbal, “high functioning” youth,
Asperger’s syndrome and autism more broadly did represent a set of characteristics that
could be applied to them, or not, and negotiated.
One Social/Recreational Group focused specifically on Asperger’s. Although
Asperger’s was not commonly the central topic of conversation, the youth did
occassionaly bring up Asperger’s spontaneously. Sometimes they joked about it. Once, a
volunteer asked a youth several times in English, “Please sit down.”52 He replied, “no,
non sit down, sono Asperger.” (No, not sit down, I'm Asperger!) and again, “Io sono
Asperger, non Down.” (I'm Asperger, not Down!). From time to time, youth also
mentioned that a particular famous person had Asperger’s:
[We were playing Trivial Pursuit, and agreed that one boy would create all
the questions rather than read those provided by the game.] The question
posed to [one boy] regarded an actress who consistently messed up a
certain line. [….] We learned it was Marilyn Monroe. [….] [Another boy]
said she had sindrome di Asperger (Asperger's Syndrome). [Fieldnote
excerpt]
52
This sort of switching into English was somewhat common in this context, at least when I (a
native English speaker) was present.
298
During dinner one night, four of the young men were discussing Disney and we
had the following exchange:
Boy 1: Era autistico.
Me: chi?
Boy 1: Disney.
Boy 2: era un Asperger?
Boy 1: Si.
(Boy 1: He was autistic.
Me: Who?
Boy 1: Disney.
Boy 2: He had Asperger's?
Boy 1: Yes.)
At times, youth made spontaneously self-reflective statements regarding their particular
difficulties that were not joking:
[One of the girls] showed us at [the game table] cell phone pictures of her dog.
She said lui è molto simile a me (he's very similar to me). [An older adult
participant asked]: in che senso? (In what way) [She replied]: ha difficoltà
integrare con suoi coetani. Non sa relazionarsi con gli altri cani. (He has
difficulty interacting with his peers. He doesn't know how to relate to the other
dogs). [Fieldnote excerpt]
[One of the girls started remarking on the behavior of a child nearby]. [She]
leaned over to me and started saying in low tones that maybe he didn’t
understand, because sometimes loro non capiscono quello che non è la loro
fascinazione divertente al momento. Anche me, quando ero piccola, ho
avuto questa esperienza [….] ho difficoltà di capire con due sensi allo
stesso volta.(they don't understand what is not their current entertaining
fascination. When I was small I also had this experience …. I have trouble
understanding with two senses at the same time). [Fieldnote excerpt]
A staff member came over and the conversation continued. The young woman
made her connection more explicit, saying, ‘Quello che è Asperger è quello che ha
difficoltà parlare quando ha i emozioni’ (People with Asperger’s have trouble
talking when they are emotional). In these situations not only did youth
spontaneously address their difficulties or their diagnosis, they were able to talk
with older mentors – staff/volunteers, people with autism, or people with family
299
memebrs with autism.
The other situations in which explicit discussions of Asperger's or autism arose
were in introductory meetings with potential new participants, and in the specific
situation of a conflict between two people that one of the staff often tried to mediate
through explaining to one person how the other person's autism caused him certain
difficulties that helped fuel misunderstanding. Parents and staff in Day and Residential
Centers also talked about mediating conflict between attendees, but they did not draw on
an autism discourse to explain the other person’s behavior. In contrast, they drew on the
narratives discussed in Chapter 5, that everyone has certain strengths and difficulties.
They might use this idea to identify similarity, i.e., you have certain difficulties and need
certain; he has certain other difficulties and needs a certain other type of help.
These examples, especially the focus group, show a great diversity of perspectives
even within the same group. Some youth did not identify autism as a word that applied to
them at all, while others described in a lot of detail what the term meant as it described
them. Some youth understood and doubted the validity of the Asperger’s label, whereas
others found it useful. These perspectives changed between participants and also over
time. As one mother explained of her son some years back, upon first hearing the
diagnosis of Asperger’s, she and her then-teenage son both went to the internet to read
about it. She explains:
I immediately recognized him in it. He saw himself in it less. I understood, yes it
is true, almost all of it is him. Look. And he on the other hand had more trouble.
Now no. Now he exaggerates the other direction. He is making, he made a short
film because from, that he will show at a stadium, about Asperger, he’s in it, it’s
something in short (laugh) rather public.
- Interview, Parent at Social/Recreational Activity Group
While the issue of autism-as-an-identity will be discussed in depth in the next chapter, it
300
has been by goal in this section to have shown the great diversity and disagreement
between participants about its meaningfulness to them as individuals with certain
characteristics.
Both for those who rejected that they had autism and those who identified with it,
what were these characteristics of autism? They were quite varied. Only in the focus
group and one interview were all three characteristics of the Wing-Gould Triad listed.
Social interaction was listed in five sources, stereotypical and repetitive behavior and
interests in five sources, and social communication including the fact that some people
with autism do not speak was mentioned in three. Table 15 below lists the most
commonly mentioned additional traits. There were also several traits discussed only by
one participant or only in the focus group, which included physical traits (such as early
balding), close-mindedness, indecisiveness, a bad memory, naivety, “undervaluing” other
people, difficulty reasoning, attention problems, detachment from reality, being
disorganized, and having dysgraphia. It is worth noting that three participants talked at
length about other people’s traits, but not so much their own. Unsurprisingly, however,
two of these youth were those who contested the applicability of the diagnosis for
themselves.
Table 15: Commonly Referenced Characteristics of Autism (Youth)
Characteristic
Motor Difficulties, Clumsiness
Behavior Problems (aggression and
violence)
Anxiety
Especially Intelligent
Nerdiness
References in Interviews and Fieldnotes
4
2
2
2
2
As the table shows, there were very few recurrently important characteristics, and
301
participants experienced their autism in very different ways. Two participants specifically
drew on a “difference” discourse to describe autism. As one youth explained, “Voul dire
un modo diverso di funzionare rispetto alle altre persone” (It means a different way of
functioning compared to other people). One way to frame this difference that was
important for several youth was the discourse of “nerdiness” (they used the English
word). As other scholars, popular culture observers, and self-advocates with autism have
noted (Hacking 2009a; Jack 2014; Jackson 2002; Silberman 2001), there is a lot that is
“nerdy” or “geeky” about autism. In fact, in 2011 while I was conducting a pilot study
and using Google Translate to render an Italian article’s abstract in English, the translator
tool provided the English “geek” for the Italian “autistico.” Many of the youth I knew
had interests that were traditionally “nerdy” or “geeky” such as video games and
anime/manga. Although most youth did not identify themselves to me as “geeks” or
“nerds” there was a small group who did. When asked about characters or personalities in
the media (TV, books, and film) that they found similar to them, those in the focus group
listed several real and fictional figures ranging from Albert Einstein to the Big Bang
Theory’s Sheldon Cooper, telling me that they are similar via “Nerd Power!” Indeed, my
own interest in “nerdy” things helped me build rapport with the youth. On my very first
day at one Social/Recreational Group, many of the boys bombarded me with “do you
know…?” questions, including such pop culture references as Simpsons, Space Balls,
Nostalgia Critic, Salad Fingers, and Dragonball Z. As I mentioned my knowledge of one
particular series, one of the youth turned to another and told him, ‘she is nerdy, like us!’
Shortly thereafter, he explained to me, ‘Asperger’s is genetically nerdy.’ I was able to
build rapport with youth at both this and the other Social/Recreational Group through my
302
familiarity with many of these favorite topics. My knowledge of comic books both U.S.
and Japanese also helped me have a point of reference for conversation with youth at day
and residential centers. One youth also spoke highly of a particular teacher, saying,
among other things, “è diciamo una specia di nerd anche lui che guarda star wars ecc.
Mi ha fatto fare tutto un tema su star trek ecc quindi si è... molto, diciamo, ci capiamo
molto” (He is, let’s say, a type of nerd also, who watches Star Wars, etc. He made me do
an entire essay on Star Trek, etc., therefore … let’s say, we understand each other a lot).
In Anglophone literature, autism emerges as a key example of a “brain disease,”
especially for people thus diagnosed (Buchman, Illes, and Reiner 2011; Ortega 2009;
Ortega and Choudhury 2011). Most participants in this study did not mention their brains
(as discussed more in Chapter 7), but focused on their interests, feelings, or relationships.
The implications of the diagnosis for autism rested, for these participants, in the social
consequences. Three themes emerged. Firstly, participants talked about autism as a
balance of strengths and weaknesses. Secondly, participants talked about their fear in
terms of others not understanding them (more than anything about autism itself). Thirdly
and related, participants talked about experiences with bullying or teasing.
As we saw in Chapter 5, I asked parents how they described the characteristics or
difficulties of their child to their child him or herself, if they talked about it. Parents
described many conversations in terms of relative strengths and weaknesses, explaining
that we all have things we are good at it and things we are less good at. Two youth gave
me similar explanations, one talking about how “ognuno ha proprio carattere” (everyone
has their own character) and another explaining that a trouble with autism is that “forse
non riesco a capire bene che ho un po’ di difficoltà su alcune cose, anche se su alcune
303
cose sono molto esperto” (maybe I do not manage to understand well that I have a little
trouble in some things, even if in other things I am a real expert). Two youth (including
one of those just quoted) told me that having Asperger’s was similar to having any
physical characteristic, listing examples such as eye color and hair color. One of these
youth specifically attributed this definition to his parents, explaining that his family sees
it this way. Another youth described Asperger’s as “not a psychological profile” but a
“character,” explaining, “è come dire quel ragazzo è simpatico, quel ragazzo è antipatico,
quel ragazzo è Asperger. Per me è la stessa cosa” (it’s like saying that guy is nice, that
guy is rude, that guy has Asperger’s. For me, it’s the same thing). In this formulations,
autism works like any other characteristic: it is something they may be or have, but only
in so far as any other attribute.
When I asked youth their greatest fear, they told me not about the way autism
affected them, but the way it affected other people’s behaviors towards them. As one
young man at a Social/Recreational Activity Group explains, “Uh, ad esempio, mm, non
so, il fatto di non essere accettato da alcune persone per quello che sono. Cioè, alcune mi
accettano, magari altre no” (Uh, for example, mm, I don’t know, the fact of not being
accepted by other people for what I am. I mean, some accept me, maybe others no.)
Another young man at this group told me, “Mi fa paura che la gente pensa che io sono
diverso in senso negative, ma secondo me, io sono diverso in senso positivo” (I am afraid
that people will think that I am different in a bad way but in my opinion I am different in
a good way). A teenage girl at a Social/Recreational Activity Group, when asked if
Asperger’s caused her any problems, told me, “Sì perché ecco un po’ [sono] stata capita,
perché avevo paura di non essere capita” (Yes because, look, a little [I am] understood,
304
because I was afraid of not being understood). Parents also echoed this fear, as in the
parent at a residential center who stated, “Ma mia paura più che altro è che non venga
capita” (my biggest fear is that she won’t be understood).
Relatedly, youth related their experiences not being understood, and being
“scherzato” (teased). One youth explained, “Mi sento escluso per il fatto che io sono
asperger e gli altri non ne sono” (I feel excluded because I have Asperger’s and the
others do not). Moreover, “spesso sono scherzato [...] perché non mi sento considerato
dagli altri. Questa mi fa sentire male” (I am often teased […] because I don’t feel
considered by the others. This makes me feel bad). He explained his problems at school,
where he often met “persone pochi intelligenti [...] quando sbagliavo, loro anziche farmi
notare tranquillamente mi hanno fatto da notare in maniera sgarbata, insultandomi”
(people of low intellect [...] when I made a mistake, instead of simply telling me they let
me know in a garbled way, insulting me). They called him “handicappato”
(handicapped). He was also teased out of ippoterapia (horse therapy). With the help of
his parents, he ultimate switched “sections” at his school and took new courses with new
classmates. He explained, “veramente la mia vita è cambiata. Ho conosciuto persone
nuove e più sociali, più intelligenti. Infatti scherzavano un po’ ma non in maniera
pesante, in maniera arrogante. Invece loro gli altri facevano” (truly my life changed. I
met knew people, more social, more intelligent. In fact they were joking a little but not in
a serious way, in an arrogant way. Instead those others did).
Parents describe similar experiences their children encountered. One mother
recruited from a Residential Center was called into school by her daughter’s teachers,
who told her that her daughter had thrown a rock at another child. This mother explained
305
“secondo me [figlia] quando faceva le cose, c’era un qualcosa dietro. Non la faceva così.
Che poi non fosse la reazione giusta, quella di lanciare lo sasso, ma se l’aveva fatta, io
mamma sapevo che c’era stata qualcosa. Che non era giustificabile però... e mi dice,
‘questa bambina, mi dice, che sono, continua a dirmi, mi scherza handicapatta.’” (In my
opinion, [my daughter], when she does things, there is something behind it. She doesn’t
act like this. It’s not the right reaction, throwing a rock, but if she did it, I as a mother
know that there was something. It’s not justified but… and she told me, ‘this child told
me that I am, she keeps telling me, she teases me as ‘handicapped’). Another mother
recruited from a Social/Recreational Group complained that the other children at school
“capendo che lui ha dei problemi, lo allontano. Lo prendono in giro e lo mandano via,
proprio tante volte gli dicono ‘vai via, tu, che sei handicappato’” (understand that he has
problems and distance themselves from him. They pull his leg and send him away, really
very often they tell him ‘go away, you, you’re handicapped’). Finally two youth recruited
from Social/Recreational Groups also complained that having a diagnosis made it harder
for them to get a driver’s license, which was a point of particular annoyance and clearly
linked to life cycle milestones.
I asked youth what causes autism when feasible. Only in three individual
interviews and in the one focus group did youth clearly53 discuss the causes, and their
responses offer very little consistency. Two youth talked about autism as caused by social
construction, as they describe in the focus group excerpt above. Other causes were
suggested only once: genetics, the social environment, and “I don’t know.” One youth
53
In one interview, the youth responded by pointing to answers on the sheet. Because my writing
made him uncomfortable, I did not note this answer by writing and it is impossible to tell on the
transcript.
306
specifically rejected the idea that vaccines cause autism.
Sex, Gender, and Sexuality
As noted in Chatper 2, autism spectrum conditions are diagnosed far more often
in men than women. Studies consistently find four to six times more males with autism
than females (Baio 2012; Fombonne 2003; Fombonne 2005), with the ratio even wider
among people with higher levels of support needs (Fombonne 2005). A few professionals
even noted to be that from their experience, they suspected an even wider gap than these
reports. Youth did not often talk to me about their gendered experiences directly,
although they did sometimes talk about their sexuality (discussed below). However, on a
community level, the consequence of a predominantly male and masculine group of
young people, I would argue, potentially leads to autism spaces being gendered as
masculine spaces. As quoted above, some participants linked autism and Asperger’s with
a “nerd” identity. Although women most certainly can and do identify as nerds (as do I),
“nerd culture” has been associated with masculinity,54 whiteness,55 and their intersection
(Bucholtz 2001; Eglash 2002; Kendall 2000; Kendall 2011; Quail 2011). Explicit
trappings of “nerd-dom” could be found at two of the sites in which I worked – superhero
films, scifi/fantasy themed games, manga and comics on the bookshelves. “Nerdiness” in
other sites may have been less institutionalized and limited to the possessions and favored
topics of only a few attendees. Other sites had more traditional non-nerdy media – largely
Disney and Pixar. As mentioned in Chapter 4, at least one staff member was firmly
against childish trappings. Other adult but not necessarily nerdy media included car
54
For insight into high-profile problems around nerd masculinity in the United States, see Arthur Chu
(2014b; 2014a; 2015)
55
Popular representations of autism in the Anglophone world also association autism with whiteness
(Heilker 2012).
307
magazines, still traditionally a masculine interest. These nerdy and mechanical interests
and activities may code spaces as more masculine and create barriers for women who
lack these interests to be as included. However, most pursuit of interest and entertainment
was done solo, so I did not observe this effect.
The difference that I observed emerged in Social/Recreational Activity Groups.
One group did not have as large a gender gap (perhaps due to being the most inclusive
with respect to diagnosis – including more people without autism spectrum conditions).
This service split the participants by sex. The other, however, had an overwhelmingly
male clientele. The organizers and parents spoke openly about the gender gap and tried to
facilitate friendships between the few women present (even going out of their way to
introduce new women to me, as a woman in this masculine, nerdy space).
Another organizer spoke at length about the gendered dynamic of social
interaction, namely the way some of the boys in their late teens and early twenties
persisted in attempting to engage some of the girls in conversations on sexual topics.
Indeed, as a 24 year old woman (“geek girl”) myself, I was keenly aware of the potential
sexual/romantic tension in the space. It felt very similar to the “nerdy” peer groups I had
frequented in the United States. I took steps to appear both masculine and unavailable.
Like some of the women who participated and volunteered, I was aware that some youth
hugged and kissed us goodbye, but not necessarily the men. The experiences of girls and
women were therefore shaped by the way that these spaces could be read as men’s
spaces.
To confuse this discussion further, however, caretaking was often gendered as
feminine. Although both men and women served as staff and volunteers, a good deal of
308
these figures were women and caregiving in general is often coded as feminine (Abel and
Nelson 1990; Rosaldo and Lamphere 1974). Spaces more focused on assistive care (day
centers and residences) may therefore not be as masculinized despite the predominantly
male group of attendees, due to their focus on caretaking.
What about sexuality? Two of the youth I interviewed spoke about the idea of
having a girlfriend, but ambivalently, pondering if it was worth the trouble. Other youth
discussed girlfriends, having them or not, in casual conversation. Staff and volunteers
sometimes spoke to youth about these relationships, attempting to model or explain good
behavior.
Staff and parents were more concerned with solo sexuality. Not much literature
addresses this issue at all (for exceptions, see Koller 2000; Melone and Lettick 1983;
Nichols and Blakeley-Smith 2010), and one parent specifically lamented that Italians lack
the option of “northern” Europeans with disabilities to patronize professional sex
workers. Nonetheless, there were times in which the staff were concerned to
communicate to the youth some version of the lessons described in the literature – that it
is okay to have private time (for masturbation) in private places such as a bathroom or
bedroom. Staff also addressed self-care with women, including menstrual care and hair
removal.
In short, autism is masculine and nerdy, which is itself masculine, making autism
spaces potentially men’s spaces. Men’s and women’s experiences in such spaces should
be understood accordingly. The next section turns to youth experiences in these spaces.
Because I interviewed almost exclusively men and boys, the perspectives I present should
be understood as such as not necessarily generalizable to women’s and girls’ experiences.
309
Section 2: Interfacing with Services
In this section I will recap parents’ goals for services and add youth goals to that
list, then explore whether or not these goals seem to be enacted in participants’ daily
lives. I draw from youth interviews as well as observations from staff, parents, and my
own fieldnotes.
In Chapter 5, we saw parents’ goals for their children included gaining skills and
being occupied; being content and having somewhere to be “dopo di noi”; and social
goals of making friends, interacting with others, and being supported and understood.
Youth raised additional goals including: becoming a better person, improving the service,
spending time with other people who have Asperger’s, finding people with shared
interests, and talking with psychologists.
Being Occupied
Youth discussed their experiences with gaining skills and being occupied. Many
youth, as reported by their parents, referred to the services as school. Others considered it
more like work. In one particularly revealing instance, a youth wrote a note to his father
explaining why he was upset with him that day. In a fieldnote excerpt, I paraphrased the
note:
Dad, I’m sorry for how I behaved last night. I don’t like telling lies. Dad, I am
mad at you because you said I was fired. I was not fired. I work at the Center. I go
the Center every day and the Center is my job. Please, don’t tell any more lies.
- Fieldnote Excerpt
These examples show that youth often equated services with other forms of being
professionally occupied during the day – school and work. As discussed in Chapter 5,
Day Centers were structured not unlike work in some ways, with fixed arrival and
departure times, desks, and areas dedicated to different tasks. Youth also discussed
310
specific skills they wanted to build, such as learning to follow when others explain and
pursuing sports. Some youth also talked about particular skills they would like to
develop, such as by having the service offer courses in writing and informatica (computer
science).
Beyond skill building and professionalism, youth at social/recreational activity
groups also valued having fun and having somewhere to go. One youth explained,
“Anche perché la domenica non saprei cosa fare se non vengo a giocare” (plus, I
wouldn’t know what to do on Sundays if I didn’t come here to play). As discussed in in
Chapter 4, some youth found the diagnosis of Asperger’s or autism useful for getting
access to services that allowed them to get out of the house, have fun, and meet people –
even though they did not personally find the diagnosis that applicable to them or that
useful in terms of identity. They still valued the place, particularly at Social/Recreational
Groups, which provided them a space to meet with peers, engage in shared interests,
gather to go on guided outings to city or countryside attractions, and maybe make new
friends.
Dopo di Noi
For the most part, youth did not share parents’ concerns with the “dopo di noi.” In
one potential exception, a mother explained:
I think that, I think above all of what the Doctor [last name] says, about “after us.”
About “after us.” Because [my son] told me the other say, we were talking about
the dead, because over the course of three years I lost my husband, my father, and
my mother. And he was like, he said, “mamma, then you will die too” and I said
“[son], you know what happens when you get old, then you die.” He looked at
me, he was like, “what do you mean, that I’ll be alone?” How do you respond to
that? And I said, “[son], you have so many friends, so many people that love you
so much, and that will replace in part what… I won’t be able to give you.” But
these questions leave me with a strained heart.
- Interview, Parent at Residential Center
311
Satisfaction with Services
All of the youth I interviewed reported being content, even if a few had
complaints. Of course, I did recruit youth from these services and so, as with parents,
there is a possibility of selection bias. Even in one of the most negative statements, the
youth couched his criticism in praise:
One of my psychiatrists told me about it. I went and I found it, I found it good.
Then I started to be pissed because I realized there were, there was a shitload of
problems no one wanted to face, then I said “why am I, why do I get pissed? This
is a good place. There are people I like. I could use my superior intellect to make
it a better place.” I am still trying to.
- Interview, Youth at Social/Recreational Activity Group
Indeed, a few youth did complain about what they perceived to be a lack of organization.
Youth also expressed social complaints. In these complaints, a potential criticism
emerges that the services are in fact not actually specific enough to their needs. One
youth complained about the mix of participants across a broad range of the spectrum at a
social/recreational activity group. He repeatedly expressed concern about the difficulty of
finding a game that works for all people present and does not make anyone who has more
difficulty feel excluded nor anyone who has less difficulty feel bored and agitated. He
concluded it would be better to be split up, so people could engage in the games at their
own levels. Parent reflected on this concern as well, as explored in the section of Chapter
5 that addresses concerns about services. In a similar vein of interpersonal complaint two
youth (at the same site) complained specifically about another attendee, which transitions
us into youth relationships.
Youth Relationships
Every site had people who had affective relationships with each other, in pairs or
groups. Sometimes the youth described themselves as friends. Sometimes staff described
312
them as especially close. Sometimes youth had particular ritual or nearly ritual
interactions that staff saw as friendly (and to me certainly seemed so as well). In this
section I touch upon the variety of relationships that youth had with one another, but I
want to stress that these relationships were in many ways idiosyncratic – it was by no
means the case that all youth were friends with each other, as the complaints about
specific peers make clear. Please note that this section, and my research, focused on
relationships within the context of the services I describe, not all relationships that people
with autism have. Therefore, I did not ask about relationships with family members or
with friends outside of the service, although some youth did talk spontaneously about
these important bonds. This section asks if the services themselves were places were
youth formed friendships and relationships. As such, it touches on the potential for
“biosociality” in these groups, addressed more explicitly in Chapter 7.
At day centers, there were some youth who seemed to get along particularly well.
A staff member once told me of two ospite that ‘they are friends enough, he is enamored
of her.’ There were pairs that had their own sort of call-and-response exchanges which
seemed to be pleasurable to them, though often confusing to outsiders.56 Relationships at
residential centers were similar, with the added dimension of cohabitation. Parents told
me about particular other residents their verbal children would mention frequently –
sometimes residents who particularly agitated them, but at times also residents they
I am reminded of Oliver Sacks’ (1998) chapter, “the twins,” in which he describes 26 year old twins who
have held various diagnoses, sometimes autism. Sacks (1998:201–202) writes, “They were seated in a
corner together, with a mysterious, secret smiles on their faces, a smile I had never seen before, enjoying
the strange pleasure and peace they now seemed to have. I crept up quietly, so as not to disturb them. They
seemed to be locked in a singular, purely numerical, converse. John would say a number – a six-figure
number. Michael would catch the number, nod, smile and seem to savour it. Then he, in turn, would say
another six-figure number, and now it was John who received, and appreciated it richly.”
56
313
seemed to be fond of and want to see.
At Social/Recreational Groups, the closest of these seemed to be relationships
where people saw each other outside of the group meetings themselves. As one youth
explained, “ho fatto stretta amicizia con un ragazzo qui, adesso lo sto frequentando
spesso” (I made good friends with a guy here, now we are often hanging out). He and his
friend from the group made many outings together independently, around the city and the
countryside, ranging from small outings at a bar to longer vacations taken together. Other
pairs or small groups were known to go out together as well, and sometimes youth in
residential centers spent time together on weekends at their natal homes as well. Another
youth introduced himself to a group by explaining, ‘ho venti anni, mi piace internet e
videogiochi e [lui]’ (he touched [another young man], sitting next to him), ‘è il mio
migliore amic.’ (I am twenty years old, I like the internet and videogames and [he] is my
best friend).
Youth did credit the social/recreational activity groups for accomplishing the goal
of making friends. One youth stated, “Grazie a [servizio] sono riuscito a fare nuovi
amici” (Thanks to [the service] I succeeded in making new friends). Another explained:
Yes, in my opinion the goal is to get people to know, let’s say, to get people to
know more people because someone with Asperger’s doesn’t know how to meet
other people with Asperger’s. That is, you can’t go on Facebook and write
“people with Asperger’s” and see some friends that you can look up. Or anyway,
that is, there isn’t anywhere to find them. The only place to find other people with
Asperger’s Syndrome is to come here and maybe make friends with people
similar to you. That’s it.
- Youth in Focus Group
Although these youth all still attending the groups, at least one expressed the idea that
making friends was the only important goal, and that upon making friends perhaps they
would not need the group any more. Although I do not know of a time that someone left
314
for this reason, for at least one youth it was a possibility.
Of course, youth were by no means all friends with each other. There were those
that staff told me they made sure not to assign seats next to each other, and those that
needed more private rooms to minimize how much they were bothered by interacting
with others (as described in Chatper 4). Parents also reported their children complaining
about others, particularly those who made a lot of noice. I also observed youth actively
conflicting over favored objects or the noise level. Staff generally intervened when
conflicts arose.
Previous research (Sotgiu et al. 2011) notes that Italians with autism have large
social networks because of their extensive contact with professionals and
paraprofessionals. My research provides some thick description of these relationships.
Indeed, both parents and children identified relationships with specific staff members as
extremely important.
So... in reality we have always established exceptional relationships with the
teachers. Even today the support teachers come to visit us or the kids that came to
do therapy, therapy for [my daughter].
- Interview, Parent at Day Center
Yeah, we talk about it even now and if [we] talk about [the educatore], he is very
happy, that is, he has a great memory. Then while he was with this [educatore] in
this Italian sports center, he met a girl, the ice skating teacher, and educatrice, and
she accompanied him after [educatore] and she substituted for him a bit, let’s say
[….] She followed him so much, even with her we had good results. A girl, a
different person. He was very attached to the girl. Even now we see her. We
haven’t seen her since [my son] entered here (sigh) but at any rate the talk on the
telephone even the other day they talked therefore she is a present figure.
- Interview, Parent at Residential Center
Particularly those who went to one Social/Recreational Group spoke frequently of a
volunteer who no longer participated in the group. Youth regularly talked to me about
this man, told me about the events he used to run, asked me if I knew him, and even
315
commented on their versions of the details leading up to his leaving. Parents,
professionals, and other volunteers might mention him from time to time, but it was the
youth that most frequently spoke of him and it was so striking that I referred to him in my
field notes and transcripts as “the much missed man.” The much missed man was only
one particularly striking example. Other youth in other sites talked about former staff as
well, confirming parents’ assertions about the stress of staff turnaround.
I could easily note from my participant observation the close relationship youth
had with staff, and their parents noted it as well in Chapter 5. One participant confirmed
my observations, telling me specifically:
And then there’s... [there are] the two psychologists that help out here, well, and
you, you are kind to me. And therefore I come also for that because I like hanging
out with people like psychologists and that type of thing [….] I do not like being
here when they are not here. That is, I like being in a place where, let’s say, there
are psychologists that understand and so on. I come here more than other to be
heard by them because they always listen to me, as do you [….] I am not
interested in the other youth, the other people that come here.
- Interview, Youth at Social/Recreational Group
In the above quote this youth makes it abundantly clear that he has social goals at this
group – to be heard and understood – but that he is only interested in fulfilling them by
talking with a certain type of people (psychologists and the anthropologist), not the other
youth with autism. At other points in the interview he does talk about wanting to meet
other people with Asperger’s syndrome, but he explains here that his relationships with
the volunteers and other figures are more important to him. I noted this phenomenon in
other sites as well. Ospite went to staff for conversation. Staff sometimes had to actively
facilitate conversation between ospiti, encouraging them to speak directly to each other.
One explanation may be that people with autism have enough trouble communicating that
they communicate better with someone who has no such trouble and can make up the
316
difference. Certainly I heard this explanation from my participants. Another explanation
may be that the ospiti recognized who had power in the situation – the staff.
How did they feel about their psychologists and teachers and educatori? What
made a good one? A less good one?
And then it was like I made friends with the psychologist, I liked going there, for
me it was fun because she was a person that, let’s say, was interested to hear what
I was saying. Which very few did, I mean, no one did.
- Interview, Youth at Social/Recreational Group
R: The current one is probably the best one. He listens a lot.
A: What kind of professional is he?
R: For fu--- I haven’t understood it yet. He listens. He doesn’t patronize. He
doesn’t force medications on me. And so on. Like they say in the Simpsons, he’s
a special kind of speaking doctor.
- Interview, Youth at Social/Recreational Group
Youth also valued teachers and therapists who gave them specific help that they valued.
One youth praised the gym teacher who encouraged him to try despite his difficulty,
explaining “meglio che fai, meglio di non fare niente, fai quello che riesci a fare, meglio
di niente” (it’s better than you try, better than doing nothing, do what you can, it’s better
than nothing). This advice really helped him and even gained him applause from his
classmates when he managed to do difficult exercises. Another youth valued a therapist
who shared his own experience and empathized:
He was kind, very understanding, patient. Often he helped me. And when I was
really down because I was having trouble with my classmates, often he told me
that when he was in university even he had problems with classmates, they were
bossy with him, especially with some professor that teased him.
- Youth at Social/Recreational Group
Youth were not without criticism for professionals. There was also some tension
about an educatore that at least one youth thought was infantilizing. Another youth
describes his experience with his educatrice at school:
317
They were… they were not trying to make me better. They were trying to make
stuff instead of me. I once was so pissed that one of them wrote on my agenda,
my homework, that I threw the agenda out of the window. Yeah because I can, I
spent weeks, months telling them “I want you to stop. I have to learn to do it by
myself.” She never, she never listened, because probably she didn’t think, she
thought her job wasn’t to make me become a better person, she thought I was
some sort of a good for nothing baby who had not- who needed someone to do
everything for him.
- Interview, Youth at Social/Recreational Center
These passages show that youth valued relationships with professionals, especially
professionals who supported them through specific advice and help and treated them like
the young adults they were.
Conclusion
This chapter has answered research questions (1) and (2) – how it autism
conceptualized and how do people labelled as autistici define, manage, and experience
this condition. Many youth were not particularly aware of autism. I did not get as many
participants as I planned, but that is to be expected because day and residential centers are
generally for people with higher levels of support needs, many of whom do not
communicate verbally. Those who did participate in interviews and reflect on their
experiences with the diagnosis identified a variety of characteristics as being part of their
autism, but generally talked about stereotypical and repetitive behaviors and interest,
social interaction, social communication, clumsiness, anxiety, and intelligence.
Functional consequences of their diagnosis included a balance of strengths and
weaknesses and a fear (and sometimes realization) of teasing and mistreatment by peers.
Within autism-specific services, youth were generally content. They espoused goals that
were primarily social: making friends, being with other people, getting out of the house.
While relationships with peers at these services could be very important for the youth,
318
relationships with staff were perhaps the most important.
This descriptive chapter and those that preceded it bring us into Chapter 7. In this
final chapter, I provide a detailed operationalization of the literature on biosociality,
biological citizenship, and the related concepts that I group together as “new biopolitical
theory.” I then build upon the description we have read in Chapters 4, 5, and 6 to
investigate through specific a priori themes the ways in which participants definitions of
autism and their goals for and experiences of sociality do or do not align with the stated
characteristics of this new biopolitics.
319
Chapter 7: New Biopolitical Theory
Chapter Overview
This chapter answers research question (3), “how do the health identities and
subjectivities expressed by participants resonate, or not, with theories of biosociality and
biological citizenship?” It builds upon the descriptive and emergent themes of the
previous three chapters, and draws from an a priori coding scheme derived from the
literature on new biopolitical theory (reviewed in Chapter 1). This chapter is divided into
three sections.
Section 1, “Defining New Biopolitical Theory,” explicates the four major themes
I identify in Chapter 2: molecularization, somatic expertise, individual identities, and
collective identities and socialities. This section explores each theme in detail, identifying
and defining subthemes, and discussing examples and negative cases in the data.
Section 2, “Complicating New Biopolitical Theory,” demonstrates the complexity
of new biopolitical theory in the data through several key examples. It explores situations
in which positive and negative biopolitical themes co-exist in a single narrative. This
section demonstrates that a more nuanced understanding of new biopolitical theory
focused on multiple themes reveals how the theory applies as well as the limits to its
applicability, rising to the call of previous scholars (Beck and Niewohner 2009;
Greenhough 2011) to craft a more locally nuanced understanding of new biopolitical
theory and refuting some scholars’ (Roberts 2008; Sleeboom-Faulkner 2011) claims that
these theories are only applicable in “Europe and North America” by demonstrating that
they both do and do not apply in the Italian context. These findings are in line with
anticipated “threats” to new biopolitical theory explored in Chapter 1 –
320
deinstitutionalization and integration, lack of a consumer role, and “socialembeddedness” models of doctor-patient interaction.
Section 3, “Whose New Biopolitical Theory,” tentatively explores the
demographic distribution of new biopolitical modes of engagement in participant
narratives. It asks what factors are related to level of biopolitical sentiment: parent
demographics, youth demographics, or type of site they attended. Correlations identified
two variables as statistically significant in determining the presence of biopolitical
sentiment: Site Type (Day, Social, or Residential) and Child Sex (Male or Female). Being
recruited from Residential Centers and Social/Recreational Groups were associated with
presence of biopolitical themes in interviews, as was child sex being female. Potential
explanations and suggestions for future research are discussed in this section.
Section 1: Defining New Biopolitical Theory
As Chapter 1 explains in detail, I use the term “new biopolitical theory” to refer to
a collection of theories that comprise a reformulation of Foucault’s (1990) theories of
biopolitics following changes in biomedical technologies (Rabinow 1999). Deriving from
a close reading of the literature, I propose that new biopolitical theory can be understood
as composed of four major themes, detailed in Figure 2: molecularization, somatic
expertise, individual illness identities, and collective identities and biosocialities. In
Chapter 2, I detailed how these themes emerge from the literature. In Section 1 of this
chapter, I detail how I see these themes in participant narratives, providing several
excerpts from interviews that I have coded under each theme and subthemes therein.
There are some aspects of new biopolitical theory that are excluded from this
figure and my analysis and thus I see fit to mention them here. One is the role of
321
proteomics as a subcode of molecularization. Proteomics is an extremely important
development as a molecular component of the human body, in many ways replacing
genetics (e.g., Lock 2005). However, the topic of epigenetics and proteomics does not
arise in much discussion in my own research, only raised by professionals, so I am
exluding proteomics from this table. I am also excluding Rose’s (2007) concept of
optimization because it goes beyond a health-illness dichotomy, and my research is
situated within that dichotomy (or a similar dichotomy, as not all participants would
define autism as an illness, as discussed above). Finally, I am excluding economies of
vitality (Rose 2007) and all aspects of bioeconomics which relate closely to biopolitics
and come into play in autism research, because my focus is instead on treatment and
social organization. For similar reasons, I do not include a discussion of the political
economy of hope (Rose 2007). I acknowledge that these interrelated concepts are
excluded from my below operationalization, and are worth further investigation in studies
of new biopolitical theory in Italy that approach autism from different, more relevant
angles.
322
Figure 4: New Biopolitical Theory Themes and Subthemes
Molecularization
Geneticization
Neurologization
Microbiologicization
Somatic Expertise
Pastoral Keepers
Pilgrimages of Hope
Experiential Expertise
Patient Literacy
Responsibilization
Somatic Individuality
Individual
Identities
Autism as Identity
Cerebral Subjectivation
Autism as a Way of Being
Autism Pride
Autism as Normal
Social Model of Disability
The Family with Autism
Collective
Identities and
Socialities
Social Group Formation
Informational Biocitizenship
Digital Biocitizenship
Rights Biocitizenship
Activism and Volunteerism
323
Molecularization
Molecularization refers to a “‘style of thought’ of contemporary biomedicine”
which “envisages life at the molecular level, as a set of intelligible vital mechanisms
among molecular entities that can be identified, isolated, manipulated, mobilized,
recombined…” and so on (Rose 2007:5-6). With respect to autism, such molecular levels
of thought include the genetic; the neurological – neurochemical or neurostructural; and
the microbiological. These categories are, of course, not mutually exclusive – for
example, both genes and gut microbes can both easily be said to affect brain structure and
chemistry.
Geneticization refers to an increasing focus, particularly in the 1990s, on the
genetic basis of illness. Lippman explains, “Today’s emphasis on genetic causes and
genetic understanding seems to be part of a more expansive process I have called
‘geneticization’ (and have begun to describe elsewhere). Those in charge of healthrelated naming and helping activities increasingly now reduce differences between
individuals to their DNA codes, define most disorders and behaviors, as well as
physiological variations, as at least in part genetic in origin, advocate the adoption of
interventions that employ genetic technologies to manage problems of health and, in
general, (incorrectly) equate human biology (humanity?) with human genetics, implying
that the latter acts along to make each of us the organism she or he is” (1991:85). Though
not all genetic discourses are as reductive as in this description, Lippman also notes
another aspect of geneticization much seen in autism literature: “Packaging human health
problems – as well as a whole range of human variability – as ‘genetic’ blurs the
distinctions between disease and illness, disability and handicap” (1991:87). Locating
324
autism in the nexus of medical and psychiatric concepts often proves difficult – even
determining if it is “medical,” “psychiatric,” both, or neither.
One encounter in the field keenly illustrates this difficulty. The title of the project
under which I collected data was “psychiatry and subjectivity.” One participant, upon
reading the consent forms, commented that this title was inappropriate because autism
does not follow under the domain of psychiatry, but pedagogy. I asked him to elaborate,
and he told me, “La psichiatria è più legata ai, alla patologia, alla patologia mentale,
eh? E la persona autistica non è necessariamente patologica, mentalmente patologica”
(psychiatry is related more to pathology, to mental illness, eh? And the person with
autism isn’t necessarily ill, mentally ill). The following conversation elaborates:
Anthropologist: But for autism you said that it was instead psychopedagogy?
Respondent: Psychology and psychopedagogy. Neuropsychology, the
neurosciences. But I mean that while psychiatry has more to do with mental
illness, and what that really means is the crazy person, the insane person, the
psychopathic person, the sociopathic person, the person with personality
disorders, mm?, the person with autism is not necessarily mentally ill. They can
be, there can be a comorbidity, no? But you can’t take for granted that the person
with autism is a person under the domain of psychiatry. Not necessarily.
A: And psychology and psychopedagogy are considered a part of medicine or
something else?
R: No no no no. Historically in Italy it emerged more from the philosophical
branch.
- Interview, Coordinator
Not only is it difficult to locate autism within medical subspecialties, it can be difficult to
locate autism in the line between medicine and psychology, even medicine and education.
These difficulties are locally particular depending upon the available structures, as shown
by this participant’s allusions to the history of Italian medicine and social services,
described in Chapter 2.
As presented in previous chapters, eight parents and one youth identified genetics
325
as likely or possible causes of autism, although not necessarily solely or reductively as in
Lippman’s discussion. Genetics did, however, play a large role in participants’
understanding of autism. Many parents also explained this genetic component to index
heredity, often identifying other family members with similar characteristics of autism.
Although most discussions of genetics stopped at the simple fact of causation, one youth
went further and described autism as “essendo geneticamente repubblicano” (being
genetically republican), explaining that to him it means being “di idee ristrette, incapace
di capire un altro punto di vista, incapace di pensare più di una o due cose, incapace di
cambiare idea, ed incapace di accettare i fatti che contraddicono le tue opinioni
preconcette” (closeminded, unable to understand a different point of view, unable to
think of anything except one or two things, unable to change your opinion, and unable to
accept facts when they go against your preconceived opinions).
As discussed in Chapter 5, some parents argued that their children’s problems
were not autism, or not pure autism, because of genetic problems. In these cases, parents
geneticized their children’s problems, but did not geneticize autism. In fact,
geneticization at least partially negated autism.
Other parents did not contest autism at all, but rejected the genetic component
entirely. Many parents described the tests their children underwent when they were
younger, attempting to locate the source of their problems. The following parent
describes a typical set of tests, but evidences a particularly strong conclusion – that the
child’s problems were entirely “mental” and not physically locatable. After a series of
clinical tests there were “Risultato zero. Il bambino non aveva, al livello di... di indagine
di malattia in sé non c’era niente. Il bambino è sano. Infatti lui è un ragazzo sano. Il suo
326
problema è mentale, non...” (Zero results. The child did not have, at the level of… of
exams for disease themselves, there was nothing. The child was healthy. In fact, he is a
healthy boy. His problem is mental, not… (trails off).
Themes of neurologization touched on both neurochemical (Rose 2003) and
neurostructural (Fein 2011) selfhood. In these discourses, parents located their children’s
problems, at least in part, in damaged brains. Youth interviewees, notably, never used
these discourses to talk about autism,57 although one parent does describe her son’s
neurological selfhood. She recounted, “Una volta ha detto una frase tremenda, aveva 10
anni. ‘Il mio cervello mi fa fare delle cose che non voglio’” (One time he said a terrible
phrase, when he was ten years old. ‘My brain makes me do things I don’t want to do’).
As shown in Chapter 5, eight parents identified neurological causes such as brain
damage as the likely or possible origins of their children’s autism. Several parents
explains children’s behavior in terms of their brains:
We were thinking that it couldn’t a neurological thing, and instead I understood
from the studies that it is really something in the brain structure, a physical
problem. This is what I understood, the cause of this thing. A part of the brain that
works in a certain way.
- Parent at Social/Recreational Activity Group
In the particular case, indeed, of [my son] […] [some of the] cubbyholes of his
brain are much more advanced. No? But in certain other things, even much more
simple things like one day for example I said, “[son], open the umbrella.” He is
not able to open the umbrella.
- Interview, Parent at Day Center
She socializes maybe with the small children. Maybe not with the youth. Because
she… is a child! She is big like a woman, because she is already… but she is
small in terms of her brain, unfortunately.
- Interview, Parent at Day Center.
57
Only one youth uses neurologization discourses at all, to say that he wants to stop taking medication
because “ho letto su siti internet di medicinali che come effetti collaterali, riducono le funzioni cerebrali” (I
read online that as side effects, they reduce cerebral functions).
327
These three different examples represent three ways of locating autistic characteristics in
the brain: identifying brain structure as the cause of autism; linking specific skills to brain
structure; and identifying a mismatch between the age of the child’s body and the age of
their “brain.” Parents also talked about therapies as acting on the brain.
Others, however, explicitly questioned the neurologization of autism. As with
geneticization, they referenced specific tests that failed to locate a cause in the brain.
Even when results emerged from the tests, parents did not always reduce children’s
characteristics to their results. For example:
These ... stains, these zones, like this, emerged, but the neurologist said that they
did not justify the language delay of [our son]. They justified a delay but not a
delay as advanced as his. So she made this diagnosis that there was an intellectual
disability but autism covered this thing a bit, that is, it was more in this direction
that [our son’s] communication difficulty – always more apparent – was
justifiable as autism rather than this intellectual disability.
- Interview, Parent at Residential Center
I distinguish neurologization from future related codes of somatic individuality
and cerebral subjectivation. In some situations these codes overlap. For example, the
following passage represents both neurologization and somatic individuality:
I tried, still now, to do some things on my own, like brain gym, that is, starting
from a stimulation, doing some body movements, there is a greater fluidity of the
thought.
- Interview, Parent at Social/Recreational Activity Group
In this example, as in several of those above, autism is not only located in the brain, but
the person with autism or their parent act on them in the language of biomedicine,
connecting behaviors and therapies to brainhood. Yet more cases of neurologization do
not indicate somatic individuality.58 In these cases, autism is located in the brain, but the
58
In 65 instances of the neurologization code, only 30 were also coded as somatic individuality.
328
brainhood discourse stops there. It does not become a basis for identity or action. For
example, one parent said, “Probabilmente ci sono dei collegamenti a livello neurologico
che non sono corretti” (There are probably connections at the neurological level that are
not correct). This phrase identifies a neurological cause for autism, but does not speak to
her child’s personhood as brainhood.
As we will discuss in more detail below, the cerebral subjectivation theme rarely
appeared, and never overlapped with neurologization. I use cerebral subjectivation to
refer to identity practices linking brain “wiring” or other metaphors to a positive
valuation of autism, whereas the “brainhood” of “cerebral subjects” I retain as Ortega and
Vidal’s (2007) formulation as a type of somatic individuality. Brain discourses are not
cerebral subjectivation, on the other hand, unless they are positive valuations of
difference and identity.
Microbiologicization refers to the molecular focus on biologic agents. As Paxson
(2008) explains, “Biopolitics, then, is joined by microbiopolitics: the creation of
categories of microscopic biological agents; the anthropocentric evaluation of such
agents; and the elaboration of appropriate human behaviors vis-`a-vis microorganisms
engaged in infection, inoculation, and digestion.” In the world of autism science and
politics, microbiologicization ties most closely to vaccine causation theories – vaccines
are biological agents that cause harm – and dietary treatments – various foods and their
proteins, vitamins, and minerals can cause or alleviate harm. As some parents explain:
There’s a study in which even Italy participated, that was coming… I don’t know
how long to show it, the autoimmune origin of autism. That is, according to this
study it could be something that enters, it could be a virus, something mundane…
and I add, why not an attenuated virus like that in a vaccine?, in the pregnancy of
the first months of life, that set off an autoimmune reaction.
- Interview, Parent at Day Center
329
A gastrointerologist first started to care for children with autism, understanding
that most of them have intestinal disturbances [....] If they come into contact with
gluten, with substances… they are more intolerant than allergic, much more,
therefore it’s a less serious form but their intestine is compromised, that is, at least
at the time, when he made this diagnosis, he told me that (my son’s) intestine was
already damaged, that he was having problems that were not understood and it
was already very inflamed. Effectively, even today, even though it’s being
treated, he takes the pills for this problem, and a diet without gluten, yeah, mostly
without gluten. First also without casein, now we have reintroduced casein. That
is, he indeed has some long periods here where he is always very bloated. And
this is strange because no one managed to do the statistics. First it corresponded
with more relaxed periods. So he gained weight and was bloated. Now it’s the
opposite. Now he is bloated when he is agitated. So no one can think that it’s a
continuous intake of air, a lot of it.
- Interview, Parent at Day Center
These passages show the way parents understand these microbiological components to
affect their children’s bodies. In autism literature, this microbiologicization overlaps
heavily with neurologization, as in the book Children with Starving Brains (McCandless
2009) which links the “leaky gut” of individuals with autism to their brain functioning. A
few parents also articulated this link, for example:
Reading all this information about vaccines that caused harm, I read that in these
cheap vaccines they used mercury, which now we know that mercury damages
the brain.
- Interview, Parent at Social/Recreational Activity Group
However, making this link between microbes (or at least the preservatives and metals
associated with them) and neurology explicit was a minority perspective. For the most
part, parents’ discussions of vaccine causation and dietary treatment remains limited to
microbiologicization, not neurologization. As discussed in Chapter 5, eight parents linked
their child’s autism spectrum condition to vaccinations as a likely or possible cause.
However, some participants also rejected microbiological discourses. Two parents and
one youth explicitly rejected vaccine causation theories.
330
In summary, molecularization themes occurred in several ways when parents and
participants discussed autism, especially the causes of autism. These causes could be
genetic, neurological, microbiological, or a combination thereof. A notable minority of
participants also explicitly rejected molecular theories.59
Somatic Expertise
Somatic Expertise refers to “new ways of governing human conduct” (Rose
2007:6) which encompass somatic technologies and expertise of both patients/parents and
professionals, in fact blurring the line between them.
Professionals exercise somatic expertise through specializations on “particular
aspects of our somatic existence” and “advise and guide … individuals and families as
they negotiate their way through the personal, medical, and ethical dilemmas that they
face” (Rose 2007:6). In doing so, they act as what Rabinow (1999:413) calls “pastoral
keepers” who “help [patients and families] experience, share, intervene, and ‘understand’
their fate.” Many of these pastoral keepers were discussed in Chapter 5, when parents
sought the advice of professionals how to govern their own conduct at home. Chapter 5
discusses several therapies involving intense in-home effort including ABA and
TEACCH, DAN!, Doman-Delacato, and Feuerstein methods. Parents also spoke of
professionals advising them not necessarily on a protocol to follow, but simply how to
comportarci – how to behave, how to comport themselves. Others talked about the close
guidance they received from certain professionals in their own endeavors. For example,
on parent gathered a large group of volunteers to engage his daughter in a variety of
therapies. He explains, “Ho iniziato un lavoro abbastanza intenso con un gruppo di
59
33 interviews had passages coded PRO Molecularization. 15 had passages coded CON Molecularization.
331
volontari dopo che avevo conosciuto una professoressa a scuola” (I started a rather
intense work with a group of volunteers after I met this professor at school). This group
worked with close supervision of the professor. This activity was “Seguita sempre da
questa professoressa che visitava [figlia], preparava un programma, mi spiegava il
programma ed io poi dicevo ai volontari quello che dovevano fare” (Always followed by
this professor who visited [my daughter], prepared a plan, explained the plan to me, and
then I told the volunteers what they needed to do).
Pastoral keepers were important not only for parents, but also for youth. Pastoral
keepers, at schools and especially at the services in which I worked, created a space
specifically tailored for people with autism, for example:
In my opinion it is opportune to have kids with autism in specialized structures.
Above all because the educatori are specifically trained to work with kids with
autism. The activities are designed for kids with autism.
- Interview, Parent at Day Center
Youth also talked about important pastoral keepers in their lives, such as the youth in
Chapter 6 who described his relationship with an understanding therapist who shared his
own similar experiences having troubles with peers. This therapist also suggested several
books and films about Asperger’s and autism to this youth. Another talked about his
teacher, who “sapendo che avevo la sindrome di Asperger, mi ha detto che una sua
amica con la sindrome di Asperger frequentava questo posto. E allora mi ha dato
l’indirizzo, io sono venuto qui” (knowing that I had Asperger’s syndrome, told me that
her friend with Asperger’s syndrome attended this place. And so she gave me the address,
and I came here.” In these cases, these figures served as key pastoral keepers, helping
youth to understand their condition.
Parents could also serve as pastoral keepers for youth, often mediating
332
information from other sources and directing them to services.60 For example:
Also thanks to my parents that found this section [i.e., the service] because they
advised me and in fact it was also to their credit, if it wasn’t [sic] for them, I
would not know with whom to interact, I wouldn’t know with whom to speak.
- Youth at Social/Recreational Activity Group
These passages show the importance of pastoral keepers not only in helping parents
understand autism, but helping people with autism themselves.
The role of pastoral keepers was overwhelmingly important to parents and youth,
who spent a lot of time interfacing with professionals. However, not all professionals
were pastoral keepers – they did not all “keep” parents and youth. Some did not help
them understand anything at all. Parents in particular complained that they received no
guidance from diagnosing physicians on what autism was and what it meant. As one
parent explained, “Sull’Asperger io dal medico non ho saputo niente” (I did not learn
anything about Asperger’s from the doctor). Some parents accused doctors of really
hesitating to give a proper diagnosis. In one detailed example, a parent described:
She didn’t explain anything about autism! She didn’t tell us anything! She kept
saying that this child has problems [.…] and she never arrived at the “therefore,”
she never arrived at “therefore,” I was pretty unnerved about that, see. At the end,
it was his father who said, “but are you saying that our son has autism?” And she
said “Yes, he has autism.” See, because she never managed to arrive at the
“therefore.”
- Interview, Parent at Residential Center
Other professionals offered negative interactions. For example, a parent told me about a
particular service, complaining, “Non mi è piaciuto a volte il loro atteggiamento così non
di collaborazione ma di ostilità nei miei confronti, confronti con la famiglia” (At times I
did not like their attitude, not so much of collaboration but hostility toward me, toward
60
Many youth asserted that they themselves chose to attend services, but it was almost always parents who
identified the services and introduced youth to them.
333
the family). Complaints about professionals’ lack of guidance were particularly prevalent
with respect to support teachers at school, as Chapter 5 discusses in more detail with
respect to parents, who complained that children were “ghettoized” and excluded from
the mainstream classroom, sometimes entirely alone, sometimes not learning anything
that parents could tell. In Chapter 6, we heard from youth who similarly complained of
staff at school and services who they felt treated them in infantilizing ways, not helping
them do things but doing them for them.
Pastoral keepers whose work becomes particularly known may be the destination
for what Rose (2007:6) calls “pilgrimages of hope,” in which patients and families travel
to access cures or treatments. In the case of autism, there are several such figures, some
of whom are discussed in Chapter 5. Participants described pilgrimages to well-known
psychologists and neuropsychiatrists across Italy; to specialized, sometimes even
traveling doctors or equipe (teams) specializing in Defeat Autism Now! or the DomanDelacato Method; to Dr. Feuerstein in Israel; and to TEACCH and Sonrise parent
trainings in the United States.
Patients/parents exercise somatic expertise through the “Experiential Expertise”
(Borkman 1976; Hughes 2009; Rabeharisoa et al. 2013; Rabeharisoa and Callon 2004).
Participants exercised experiential expertise when they challenge medical authority and
assert that they may know better than the doctors by virtue of their personal and familial
experiences, for example when they instruct teachers or staff on how to work with their
particular child and when they identify signs that doctors ignore. These claims to
expertise can be based on personal or familial experience, as in those who invoked a
“mother knows best” narrative. For example, parents stated, “Penso... non sono medico,
334
ma sento di sapere cosa è bene per mia figlia” (I think… I am not a doctor, but I think I
know what is best for my daughter) or, “Non c’è miglior medico dei propri genitori, della
propria mamma. Sapevamo già prima della diagnosi che non andava bene, che ci aveva
questi problemi. Non avevamo bisogno dell’etichetta” (There is no better doctor that their
own parents, than their own mother. We already knew before the diagnosis that
something wasn’t right, that he had these problems. We didn’t need the label). One parent
explained his expertise, which gives him the ability to immediately evaluate a doctor. He
told me, “L’ho portata da non so quanti medici, alla fine ero diventato più esperto io dei
medici” (I took her to I don’t know how much doctors, at the end I become more of an
expert than the doctors). Parents also invoked experiential expertise in explaining that
they, through their familial relationship, could easily understand their child with autism
even if others could not. Parents also respected the experiential authority of others,
praising professionals who were also parents of children with autism, and seeking books
that were written by parents or people with autism themselves in order to better
understand autism.61
Youth also drew on experiential expertise to explain autism. One youth explained
that having spent much of his life in autism-specific services and settings he understood
them perhaps better than staff. Indeed, I was often struck by the detailed insight with
which participants on the spectrum described other people’s autism (though not always
their own, though often because they contested it). Another youth described with pride
At the time of my research, two prominent books about father’s experiences had recently been published
and were much discussed by many parents: Fulvio Ervas’s (2012) journalistic Se ti abbraccio non aver
paura (If I hug you, do not be afraid) which recounts the experiences of a father and son who went on a
motorcycle trip across central and north America; and Gianluca Nicoletti’s (2013) autobiographical Una
notte ho sognato che parlavi (One night I dreamt that you were speaking).
61
335
his success in self-diagnosing Asperger’s syndrome, which a psychologist then
confirmed. I asked how he came to this conclusion and he told me:
I noticed that I never look... sometimes I am afraid to look people in the face, or
when I look them in the face I look at them in a very pronounced way, that is, I
look too much, almost fixated with… I unsettle them. When I look at someone I
don’t look at her but look through her, look at a wall behind her. It was like the
person was invisible. And then very often I underestimate people a lot [….] And
then I did various intelligence tests online and they all gave me a high result.
- Interview, Youth at Social/Recreational Activity Group
At times, parents furthermore respected the experiential authority of people with autism,
supporting them when they expressed dissatisfaction with services. For example, one
parent at a Day Center told me, “Nel momento in cui mi dirà una mattina “Non voglio
stare a scuola, voglio stare a casa,” ecco questa cosa lui la sa esprimere, comincerò a
informarmi, a preoccuparmi” (The moment that he tells me some morning, “I don’t want
to go to school, I want to stay home,” he knows how to express this thing, I will start to
investigate, to worry). In this type of situation, parents stressed both that their experience
allowed them to understand their children, and that their children’s experience allowed
them to assert preferences in service use.
A few parents explicitly discounted their experiential expertise, noting that they
were not doctors and deferring to clinical knowledge. They did not always take this lack
of expertise kindly, however. As one parent explained, “Se la dottoressa dice di no,
siamo sempre lì, no? E tu ragioni da mamma e non da medico, quindi non sei razionale”
(If the doctor says no, we are always there, no? and you reason like a mother, not like a
doctor, so you are not rational). Another similarly explained, “Devi accettare quello che
dicono anche se è sbagliato” (You have to accept what they say even if it is wrong).
While some parents agentively valued clinical expertise over experiential expertise,
336
others found that their opinions were ignored or overridden. It could be difficult to
exercise experiential expertise, even for parents who were certain they had it.
Parents and youth may also exercise lay expertise by internalizing, not
challenging, medical authority. They do this through patient literacy, in which
patients/parents read up on the condition with which they have been diagnosed.
Sometimes this literature corresponds with the diagnosing physician, sometimes it comes
from other sources – competing paradigms, complementary and alternative medicine, etc.
When parents read books on the experiences of people and families with autism, they
exercised both patient literacy and a respect for experiential authority.
Parents talked about their literacy as documentarmi or informarmi (“read up on”
or “inform myself about” the topic). Parents also attended conferences and workshops
about autism. As one typical comment explains, “Quando mi hanno fatto la prima
diagnosi, quindi poi ho imparato da lì attraverso gli specialisti, convegni, insomma, libri,
ecc, che cosa significasse” (When they made the first diagnosis, I then learned everything
through specialists, conferences, in short, books, etc., what it meant). A few youth also
read up on their conditions. One youth’s therapist suggested several books and films
which he read and watched. The youth who diagnosed himself explained, “Mi
diagnosticavo da solo cercando sui libri” (I diagnosed myself just by searching in
books).
A few counter examples also emerged. One parent noted that she had little time or
energy to read. Others found studying up to be overwhelming and exhausting, as
Valentine (2010) found for some of her participants in Australia. As one parent at a
Residential Center explains, “Poi, a un certo punto ho detto: “Non voglio sapere più
337
niente” perché abbiamo capito che ci sono tante scuole di pensiero” (Then at a certain
point I said, “I don’t want to know anymore” because I understand that there are so many
schools of thought). She committed herself to certain pastoral keepers she trusted, and
followed their recommendations. Youth cautioned others against certain types of literacy.
One youth reported telling his neurotypical friends seeking to learn about Asperger’s,
“Non leggete su wikipedia perché wikipedia dice un sacco di cose che non c’entrano
niente” (Don’t read Wikipedia because Wikipedia says a bunch of things that have
nothing to do with anything).
Somatic expertise blurs the line between professional and patient/parent.
Patients/parents are responsibilized, “urged to become active and responsible consumers
of medical services and products” (Rose 2007:4), to collaborate with professionals, and to
become protoprofessionals themselves. I see responsibilization when patients/parents
make consumer decisions between services or therapies, and also when parents undertake
therapy programs that require a lot of at-home involvement, such as following an autismspecific diet or creating visual agendas in the home. Occasionally parents invoked
reproductive responsibilization, choosing not to have more children who might have a
higher risk of autism, as Raspberry and Skinner (2007; 2010) found in the United States.
Parents describe the work they do:
[It was] great because I shared it fully – and it was a therapy that involved the
family too, so when [my daughter] did therapy I did it too at the same time. And
therefore, that is, this working together really was, that is, it prepared us together,
we did things together.
- Interview, Parent at Residential Center
Anyway I believe it was Risperdol that I gave him. I tried to give it to him, I don’t
know how many days, if it was two weeks or what. I saw him getting worse and
there I, ignorantly, took it away from him suddenly, something that they told me I
should have done, I would have taken it away little by little. I didn’t know. And
338
one day I said, “Enough I won’t give it to him anymore.” But I was happy. [….]
But considering all the journeys and that [my son] had already been saying for
some time, “mamma, I am tired, I don’t want to go,” and the same psychologist
after I took [my son], maybe left after five minutes saying, “We are leaving
because he doesn’t want to work much.” Well I said, “[My son] is tired. He
doesn’t want to do these things anymore. It’s useless that we lose all this time
because by taking [my son] to therapy, he doesn’t have time to do other activities.
In fact he cannot do music, he cannot do sports.” And so at a certain point I said,
“enough, I’ll stop everything.” I said, “Enough therapies.” And I dedicated that
time to music and to sports. And [my son] had incredible improvements. See. It
was the lucky choice.
- Interview, Parent at Social/Recreational Group
In the first example, the parent was responsibilized by professionals alongside
professionals. In the second, however, the parent responsibilized herself, made her own
decisions against medical advice (if unwittingly). As this contrast demonstrates,
responsibilization took several forms in the context of several different types of parentprofessionals relationships. Indeed, parents did not always value responsibilization. Some
parents complained about being responsibilized:
[I hope] that the situation improves [at the service]. That they take on more
educatori. And that they become autonomous in the management of [my son]
because right now I follow him a lot, eh? [….] In short [I hope] that they make do
a bit better on their own.
- Interview, Parent at Day/Residential Center
With the professionals I met, I was really in disagreement with the Delacato
equipe. Really because they promoted a standardized program, not personalized
[...] [they told me that] it was I who did not succeed in carrying out the program.
But in my opinion it was the program that wasn’t adapted to her needs and above
all to her tolerances.
- Interview, Parent at Residential Center
Similarly, other parents talked about not being responsibilized, but in a positive way.
Parents at Residential Centers, for example, explained, “Perché mi sento così accudita,
dal punto di visto di [figlio] che mi ha appunto un po’ demandato a loro, no?” (I felt so
cared for, from [my son]’s point of view that he had indeed transferred a bit from me to
339
them, no?), and, “Anche in questo ultimo anno [figlia] ha subito due interventi [...] e ci
hanno detto: “Voi non preoccupatevi, ci pensiamo noi.” E quindi effettivamente c’è stata
anche lì una grande attenzione non solo medica ma anche umana” (Even in this last year
[my daughter] underwent two surgeries [...] and they told us, “don’t worry, we’ll worry
about it.” And therefore essentially there was also a great attention not only medical but
also human). While some professionals responsibilized parents, and some parents
responibilized themselves, other times parents gratefully subordinated their responsibility
to that of the doctors. The main way in which parents’ narratives contested
responsibilization is when they explained their choice of services, treatments, and the like
– even those they did not like – in terms of being consigliato or indirizzato (advised or
directed) or having the path proposto (proposed) by professionals. Parents used the
passive voice, putting the responsibility for these choices off of themselves. Moreover,
some parents explicitly contested their ability to do as well as staff:
On the other hand it is a method that works well because only an institution can
give a kid with autism the serenity, the discipline, the order that they so need. In
the family you don’t manage. The families that we know are not able to always
follow a stable, studied, scientific routine. We aren’t able. And neither are the
other families that we know.
- Interview, Parent at Residential Center
The concept discussed in Chapter 5, “genitori devono fare genitori,” also
addresses a lack of responsibilization. Parents should be free to be parents, not obligated
to be paraprofessionals too:
On the other hand, do you know where they told us there is a complete presa in
carico [patient management, lit. taking into care], a father we met 8, 9 years ago
[….] He lived in New Zealand, for several years. He said that in New Zealand
they take the baby into care from the moment he is born, “I only act as the father.”
- Interivew, Parent at Social/Recreational Group
One parent was in fact also a doctor, but explained the importance of keeping these roles
340
separate in interfacing with schools:
Yes, yes, it was all me in the sense that then after I was, let’s say I... if then at the
end I always prepared the tools I always wanted to separate my role as doctor
from my role as a mother. So I prepared all the materials and I passed then to the
child neuropsychiatrist and then I wanted her to interface with the teachers. All
this to not do, in short, the mother, the jack of all trades, at any rate there are some
roles that in my opinion there is a certain order to respect. This doesn’t subtract,
clearly we have great relationships so the teachers knew perfectly well who was
behind it, that it was me, and we had a great report but at any rate I maintain that
it is important, look, that there is, I am convinced that it is very important to
respect the rules in which a mother, even if she is also a doctor, acts as a mother
and that there is a competent neuropsychiatrist – this is another very important
thing, above all in Italy where neuropsychiatrists’ competence in autism is close
to zero – and then a receptive school with the desire to learn.
- Interview, Parent at Social/Recreational Group
One parent, however, actively resisting this idea that parents should be parents when a
professional suggested it:
I was very lucky in the second year of elementary school at a certain point, he had
a very capable teacher, very very good, who first told me, “you stay away, act as
the mother” (laughs) but then after a couple weeks she told me, “[Your daughter]
is very complicated. Let’s see about working together.” I said, “Great, let’s work
together.” So we did some things together, and she was really great. I must say
that she is really great.
- Parent at Residential Center
So far these themes – pastoral keepers, pilgrimages of hope, authority of
experience, patient literacy, and responsibilization – are modes of citizenship and
consumer behavior that are only implicitly somatic. As Eyal and colleagues (2014) note,
many of the therapies used for people with autism are “low-tech” and therefore the
biomedicalization and geneticization of autism biosociality is in question. Indeed it is this
code, “somatic individuality,” that captures the biological nature of this engagement.
Somatic individuality refers to “beings whose individuality is, in part at least, ground
within… [their] fleshy, corporeal existence, and who experience, articulate and judge,
and act upon… [themselves] in part in the language of biomedicine” (Rose 2007:26).
341
Parents and youth invoked discourses of somatic individuality linked to the
molecular themes discussed above. I also included testa (head) as somatic individuality,
though only cervello (brain) as both somatic individuality and neurologization. Some
somatic individuality discourses linked behavior to body, as in the parent at a day center
who explained, “Questa cosa già con il mangiare un po’ è - però da piccolo era molto...
con gli orari del mangiare era ... sembrava che avesse un orologio in testa” (Already this
thing about eating a bit was - but since he was small he was very... with the hours for
eating he was… it seems like he had a clock in his brain). Another parent explains her
son’s relationships with a variety of common turns of phrase, which take on a special
significance for him in light of his relationship with his diagnosis:
He couldn’t even imagine that it was anything that regarded a mental problem. I
don’t know, even fighting with his sister, if she said, or maybe I said, “what do
you have in your head?” Absolutely, it was a phrase to avoid in every way,
because it was a thing… a tragedy, he was so mad he could die. Or, “But, are you
an idiot?”, or… telling him something like this, something about mental illness,
let’s say… or “you are out of your head [i.e., mind],” making reference to the
head or to the brain, it was something absolutely… it send him into all of his
rages, clearly.
- Interview, Parent at Social/Recreational Group
Other discourses focused on therapeutic action in terms of the body, such as the parent at
a residential center who stated with respect to her son’s epilepsy, “Speriamo che trovi il
dosaggio giusto [e] che stia un po’ bene, che faccia, il suo cervellino riesca a riposare un
po’” (We hope that he finds the right dose to be well a bit, that he does, that his little
brain manages to rest a bit). Parents who talked about the relationship between diet,
supplements, and intestinal state and behavior (as mentioned above) also implied somatic
individuality. In these cases, the diagnosis of autism had important implications for
etiology and treatment. One parent at a residential center made this connection very
342
clearly, pondering, “Forse se la diagnosi fosse stata mirata un po’ prima anche durante
la scuola tanti interventi proprio anche strutturali, di organizzazione, potevano magari
far stare più meglio noi, lei, tutto, però purtroppo così è andata, basta” (Perhaps if the
diagnosis was made a little earlier, at school age, so many interventions really even
structural, in organization, could have maybe made it go a bit better for us, for her, for
everyone but unfortunately it went like this, enough).
While some parents and youth did describe their children or themselves in the
language of biomedicine, others voiced strong resistance to somatic individuality.
In their opinion, the fact that we found out “late,” in quotes, that [our son] had
autism... it was a positive thing in the sense that we had always… um… sought
to… uh… unblock his language, his doing of the things. His independence. We
always insisted. [….] But they said, indeed, the doctor said that in her opinion
anyway the fact that… that we had stimulated him really so much. He was
stimulated “over” really in many ways… then maybe we would have done it the
same way even if we know he had autism first, however.
- Interview, Parent at Day Center
I was really fortunate because in… at that point indeed [the doctor] told me that…
s/he couldn’t yet (make) a complete diagnosis, let’s say, because [my daughter]
was too young. And that anyway working a lot, there were a lot of margins, many
margins of recovery. Therefore s/he did not give us an absolute verdict. S/he told
me there was a lot to work on. And so we got down to working on it. This helped
a lot. That is, I was, in some way s/he made me feel like I was still a subject, not
victim to a pathology but still a subject capable of doing something.
- Interview, Parent at Residential Center
This statement is the exact opposite of the modes of engagement described in new
biopolitical theory literature. Instead of finding a diagnosis empowering, this parent
found the absence of a diagnosis empowering.
Several participants questioned the somatic nature of autism. Two parents and one
youth compared it to carettere or personalità (character or personality). As discussed in
Chapter 6, several youth on the spectrum found autism, and Asperger’s syndrome in
343
particular, to be meaningless.
As both parent and youth statements above show, somatic individuality did not
resonate with many participants. Such participants argued that the specific diagnosis of
an autism spectrum condition did not help them act upon themselves.
In summary, participants expressed a wide variety of sentiments – both pro and
con – regarding somatic expertise. Many of these sentiments resonate with Valentine’s
(2010) findings in the Australian context, in which parents exercised what might be
called somatic expertise in the absence of resources, and were obligated (not empowered)
to make treatment and intervention choices. These Australian parents could not opt out of
making a choice. Section 2 will explore in more detail the relationship between these
themes and the various structures of parent/patient-professional interactions. For now, we
turn to individual, and then collective, identities.
Individual Identities
Individual identities refer to those identities built around specific conditions. I
identify several subthemes in the literature which are present, in varying degrees, in my
data.
Autism-as-identity encompasses statements in which someone claims autism as
an identity label for themselves, or explains their behaviors and choices in terms of
autism. The occasional youth or parent explained autism in these terms:
When I have had to explain, like, to people with whom I was close that I had
Asperger’s really I didn’t have a way, that is, I couldn’t explain it to them. I said,
“it’s like I am” because they already… the people that I call friends or at any rate
that I keep close to me at any rate understand what there is, they see that I am
different form the others in the way I behave, the way I think, etc. And so, that is,
I say this is Asperger’s Syndrome, in theory, that unites all these factors etc.
- Group Interview, Youth at Social/Recreational Group
344
Chapter 6 presents narratives of parents who reported instances of identification, such as
the son who identified with “autistic traits” after seeing an art exhibit and the daughter
who saw herself as like a child she knew with autism. In these examples, autism serves as
a basis for understanding behavior and identifying similarity between oneself and others.
Yet several others resisted autism as an identity. One parent contrasted the
diagnosis of autism with others, noting, “Se tu hai un figlio Down dici, “è così,” eppure
anche i Down che sono comunque diversi, perchè non è che sono uguali, però dici che
c’è questa diagnosi, un figlio che è spastico, c’è questa diagnosi, un figlio che è in
carrozzina perché non riesce a camminare, è così. E noi non riusciamo” (If you have a
child with Down’s Syndrome you say “it’s like this” and even if people with Down’s
Syndrome are different, because it’s not like they are all the same, but you say that there
is this diagnosis, a son who has a spastic disorder, there is this diagnosis, a son that is in a
wheelchair because he cannot walk, it is like this. And we cannot do that). Other parents
commented on the uselessness of the diagnosis, noting, “Io non ho mai dato molta
importanza alla diagnosi” (I never gave much importance to the diagnosis) and
‘Importante [è] la concretezza dalla vita, può chiamarla come vuole’ (What’s important
is the concreteness of the life, you can call it what you want). As seen in Chapter 6, youth
also often contested autism and rejected its use as an identity. These examples show the
way in which autism can serve as an identity, but also resistance to these narratives.
“Cerebral subjectivation” refers to brain discourses in which “people define
themselves as autistics … because their brains are ‘wired’ in an atypical way” (Ortega
2009). It is therefore a particular type of brain discourse that values difference. As
discussed in Chapter 4, professionals often saw autism as a different way of being. The
345
narrative of difference-not-deficit appeared frequently in parent and youth participant
discourses. However, these differences were never discussed in cerebral terms. In fact,
the only example of cerebral subjectivation in interviews is in the case of one parent who
described his son’s class at school as “abbastanza brutta e c’erano… neurotipici, si
chiamano?” (pretty ugly and there were… neurotypicals, they are called?). Both the
pause before the word and tag question indicate that even this adoption of neurodiversity
language (a framework for cerebral subjectivation) was tentative.
“Autism as a Way of Being” refers to any description in which autism is not an
appendage, but something someone is. The previous three chapters have furnished ample
examples of these discourses with professionals, parents, and youth. In a sense negative
examples are the default – explicit but more often implicit deficit narratives. However,
only one example actually struck me as a negative case, in which the participant actively
contested the narrative. This non-Catholic Christian explained her hopes for the future:
In the Bible, we don’t know the day and time, but we hold the hope that there will
be no more disease, no more work problems, economic crises, death. A kingdom
in which there will be no more of these problems. This paradise on earth. First I
saw my daughter like the other children. It’s true that she has a problem in her
brain, and no one can change it, only God can give her a brain like all the others.
This is my future, living on the earth with no more problems, diseases [….] I will
then be able to see my daughter in reality like all the others and not living this
‘wicked life,’ in quotation marks.
- Interview, Parent at Day Center, not audio-recorded
In this example, autism is not something her daughter inherently is, but is something her
daughter has a result of her brain – something she will no longer have if God eliminates
all problems and disease. Then, she can be like the others, in reality.
Autism Pride positively values traits of autism or focuses on positive traits.
Parents mentioned positive traits like a good memory, good intuition, and a superior, if
346
different, intelligence. They described their children with terms such as avanti
(advanced), ordinato (organized), etico (ethical), and even furbo (clever, wily). One
parent also described the book Né Giusto Né Sbagliato, the Italian translation of Paul
Collins’ Not Even Wrong (Collins 2004; Collins 2005). According to my notes from the
unrecorded interview, her reading of the book was as having said ‘magari senza autistici
non ci sarebbe stata l’evoluzione – scienze autistiche’ (maybe without people with autism
there wouldn’t have been evolution – autistic sciences). Certainly the book has strong
themes of autism pride.62 Youth described themselves as buona (good), intelligente
(intelligent), avanti (advanced), superiore or meglio degli altri (better than the others),
having “nerd power,” and even una nuova specie di essere umana (a new species of
human being). One youth described his fear as follows: “Mi fa paura che la gente pensa
che io sono diverso in senso negativo ma secondo me io sono diverso in senso positivo”
(I am afraid that people think I am different in a negative way but in my opinion I am
different in a positive way). These statements demonstrate pride in autism.
As with “Autism as a Way of Being,” one could argue that negative cases are the
default implicitly. However, I also identified three explicit negative examples. One youth
called his diagnosis “more a curse than an advantage” due to the way it hindered him in
getting a driver’s license. One parent expressed disinterest in her son’s autism pride
explanations of famous people with autism. Another parent went further, ultimately
rejecting such pride:
will tell that with time it has bothered me more, the conversation about famous
people, because it is evident that the reality for people with Asperger’s Syndrome
is not of excellent but a more difficult prospect [….] If in the beginning, if you
For example, it is recommended by the United States “Sunrise Group” (not to be confused with the
Sonrise Program intervention) on their June 18, 2014 post celebrating “autistic pride day” (Crainer n.d.).
62
347
want, I had a bit of consolation thinking about these famous people with
Asperger’s Syndrome, [but] an always more profound awareness of what
Asperger’s Syndrome is, also knowing so many people, so many kids with
Asperger’s Syndrome, brought me to really be disgusted with this type of thing,
or actually, to be always more bothered by people who have a pride in being
Asperger.
- Interview, Parent at Social/Recreational Group
Autism as Normal argues that “we are all the same.” When using this discourse,
parents translate autistic behavior, explaining it in terms of neurotypical behavior and
asserts is comprehensibility. For example, one parent describes her son’s resistance to
identifying colors. She noted that he likes blue, and calls everything blue when asked,
“Anche se sa perfettamente che è rosso” (Even if he knows perfectly well that it is red)
and will say red if pressed. She explained, “In maniera come da dire ‘non mi rompere le
scatole, [con] questi colori, non voglio saperne, non mi interessano, ecc ecc.’” (In this
way it is like he is saying ‘don’t get on my nerves (lit., don’t break my boxes), [with]
these colors, I don’t want to know, it doesn’t interest me, etc etc.’). Some parents
translated their children’s experiences through metaphor, comparing autism to a foreign
language. One explained, “Come se io vado in Francia, mi trovo lì a Parigi. Parlo con
qualcuno, non so il francese, quello mi parla in francese” (Like if I go to France, I find
myself in Paris. I speak to someone, I don’t know French, he speaks to me in French). In
another example, a parent compared autism to the habits of smokers:
“Normal” obsessive behaviors, in quotes, for example those that smoke. Most of
those that smoke, they devote themselves to [doing so with] an entire pack of
cigarettes. As long as there are cigarettes in the pack I continue to smoke but I can
accept that if I finish the pack, however, I am done. Therefore there is a form of
self-discipline as a part of the psyche and in these kids with autism it is
particularly moved, particularly revealed.
- Interview, Parent at Residential Center
Another way parents normalized autism was to explain how people with autism were just
348
the same as people without autism. Chapter 5 addressed this theme, when parents
normalized their children’s attendance at day or residential centers as being like work or
having their own home and when one mother explained to her son how neither he nor she
could meet celebrity women. In short, as one parent at a day center explained in an
unrecorded interview, people with autism ‘Come tutte le persone, normali o no, vivono
loro, con le loro possibilità e capacità’ (Like everyone, normal or not, they live their own
[way], with their own possibilities and capacities). Parents also talked about autistic
experiences as perhaps extreme versions of non-autistic experiences.63 For example,
Everyone, I think, we are very convinced in this family that this goes even for socalled normal people. They are normal people, maybe very intelligent, paralyzed
by anxiety, by nervousness, by an inability to decide. With someone with autism,
oh, you see it so much more!
- Interview, Parent at Residential Center
Youth normalized autism as being just a trait like any other. One youth explained
that having autism was normal, like having a certain eye color. As another youth told me
when I asked what his diagnosis meant for him at school and work, “secondo me io vivo
come una persona normale” (In my opinion, I live like a normal person). These examples
assert a different type of normality for people with autism. Autism does not need to be
translated into “normal” language – it already is normal for these youth.
As above, the opposite of this theme may be read as the default. However, there
were three counter-examples. One youth explained that he did not feel very “Asperger”
but instead felt “normale,” which implied a difference between Asperger’s and normality.
Another youth complained about parents who ask that their children be treated as
See also Cascio (2012), in which a mother/professional in the United States defines autism as “normal
behavior to an extreme that precludes other normal behavior.”
63
349
“normal,” although in the context of complaining of the hypocrisy he saw when they later
said their children should not be held accountable for their actions due to their disability.
In a more clear-cut negative case, a parent told me about her difficulties at parent
meetings with mothers who “non accetta i deficit” (don’t accept deficits), whereas she in
contrast explained, “invece io sono cattiva, vabbé, dico mio figlio non è uguale agli altri”
(I am mean, fine, I say that my son is not the same as the others).
The social model of disability refers to autism being understood as a mismatch
between person and environment. Chapter 6 reveals that many youth vehemently
supported this social model perspective, finding Asperger’s to be a catch-all diagnosis
that doctors give to all sorts of people they don’t know how to diagnose otherwise.
Chapter 5 reveals that parents support the social model of autism as well, though in a
different way. Rather than contesting the utility and meaningfulness of an autism label,
parent narratives address this theme when parents discussed autism as a mismatch
between “their world” and the world in which people with autism were expected to
function. As one parent explains, “Il problema più che altro [è] che non possono fare una
vita normale in questa società. Però non è neanche colpa loro” (The problem more than
anything [is] that they cannot have a normal life in this society. But it’s not their fault).
One parent summarizes this view in her hopes for the future:
It seems to me that it would be worth it to have the necessary funds to do the
research necessary to help this population, which is already becoming a
population of special persons. In such a way to find a better compatibility between
our world and theirs.
- Interview, Parent at Social/Recreational Group
Another parent similarly explained:
There was this expectation to have a normal daughter that behaves like all the
others. And for this I say that respect was a little bit the key at the time. And
350
therefore at any rate having a diagnosis, an awareness of the problem, means that
it’s up to us parents to change, to find the right context, a better context, for the
kid, and not hammer away at them so that on all counts, at all costs, you enter into
that premade context.
- Parent at Social/Recreational Group
Parents also invoked this theme when comparing people with autism to Martians or
aliens. In these examples, parents identify the problem with autism not in the person with
autism, but in the incompatible world. This theme is reflected in the environment of
autism-specific services, all of which seek to create structure that is favorable to people
with autism – sometimes to change them in the sense of helping them learn, but with the
focus of change first on the environment and second on the person.
Again, the opposite could be assumed to be the default. One youth, who did not
identify as autistic himself, explicitly rejected the social model of autism, disagreeing
with the sentiment “he’s special because it’s the others who don’t understand him.”
Autism functions as an identity not only for those diagnosed, but for the whole
family. I use Stuart Murray’s (Murray 2008) term “the family with autism” to refer to
identification as an “autism parent” – an individual identity based around autism for
parents of people with the diagnosis, not people with the diagnosis themselves. As one
parent succinctly explained, “è diventato il tuo modo di vivere, della famiglia, non del
figlio solo” (It became your way of living, for the family, not just the child). Parents
invoked an identification with an imagined community of autism parents when they
spoke of their experiences as “come tutti genitori, credo” (like all parents, I believe), in
third person in terms of a generic genitore (parent), and similar phrases. These parents
were confident speaking for all parents, or a generic parent, while speaking for
themselves. A few parents also mentioned this impact extending to grandparents and
351
siblings, talking about these family members also as parts of groups of autism
grandparents or autism siblings. Parents also invoked this theme when talking of others,
such as the woman who encountered a grandmother on vacation who offered to look after
her son simply because her own grandson had autism, and she identified with the family
immediately (the mother did not leave her son with this stranger). Parents also spoke
highly of renowned professionals who began working on autism after their own child was
diagnosed, thereby taking up an identification with them. Although not every participant
expressed these sentiments, no parents or youth explicitly rejected this theme.
Collective Identities and Socialities
Collective identities and socialities refers to membership in less imagined
communities and more to collectivities around autism. Social group formation refers to
formal or informal networks of patients or parents who share resources, information, and
support. Seventeen parents (45%) reported participating in formal parents’ groups, either
groups linked to (or even founding) specific services, national parents’ organizations, or
outside support groups. As one parent at a Residential Center described, “Mi faceva bene
consultarmi con altri genitori, mi faceva bene parlare. Ho sempre partecipato io alle
riunioni per genitori per consultarci tra di noi, per darci una mano” (It does me good to
talk to other parents, it does me good to talk. I always participated in parents’ meetings to
talk amongst ourselves, to help us out). As noted in Chapter 5, ten parents took this
participation further by founding their own organizations, volunteering in established
organizations, serving on the directing boards for such organizations, or pursuing careers
related to autism and other disabilities. Similarly, six professionals interviewed for this
study were themselves parents of children with autism (sometimes before, sometimes
352
after they chose their careers). These 16 instances of parent/advocates and
parent/professionals reflect a type of collective identity and sociality based on biology, in
which having a child with autism connects parents to a social and professional network of
other such parents.
Parents also made informal connections with other parents of children with autism
in and out of services, networking with parents and developing friendships. Informal
networks also arose, as one parent described, “Conosco un sacco di genitori, ma non è
che fanno parte di un’organizzazione che faccio. In questo ambiente di disabilità,
immagino che ci si conosce quasi tutti” (I know a ton of parents, but it’s not like they
take part in any organization that I go to. In this environment of disability, I imagine that
one knows basically everyone). Parents expressed their knowledge about autism,
services, and administrative aspects through reference to what they had learned from
other parents or people with autism, frequently citing these formal and information
connections. Sometimes, professionals facilitated introductions and the formation of even
informal networks:
I thought she would just give me this phone number and tell me what I Had to do.
Instead we started talking, she told me that she was also separated [from her
husband], that she had however found a new partner, she talked about… the
problems of her son, who is 18 now, [name]. Uh, so we stayed on the phone two
hours. (laughing). We talked about everything in two hours. In fact, now she said
that… not this Sunday but maybe next Sunday we will meet, because, I said,
come on, let’s meet, get coffee while they are at [the service].
- Interview, Parent at Social/Recreational Group
Even a few years ago a mother with a young girl, they directed her to us… a bit…
she knew [our daughter], with the same problems as [our daughter], the same
mannerisms, a lot… but she was still a minor. And they had to distance her from
her mother, and she called me, crying, and I told her, “Look, it’s hard, but look at
our experiences, at the end you will see.” And now I heard from her after a while,
and she told me, “Look, you were right.”
- Interivew, Parent at Residential Center
353
Parents sometimes considered services as a way for youth to similarly build social
connections with other people who were like them. In Chapter 5, we heard from a parent
who preferred an autism-specific service so her son could meet others and they could
“help one another, exchange something.” Another parent at a Social/Recreational Group
explained, “L’ideale per lui sarebbe trovare qualcuno che ha delle problematiche simili
ma che però abbia desiderio di fare amicizia” (The ideal for him would be to find
someone that has similar problems but that wants to make friends).
Others, however, describe a distinct lack of social group formation. Parents
specified that they didn’t have “dei dialoghi con altri genitori. Sì, qualcuno lo conosco,
mi parlano, però non più di tanto” (many conversations with other parents. Yes, I know
some, they talk to me, but not much). Several parents similarly noted that they did not
know the other youth or the other parents at the services they attended very well, if at all.
One parent noted the presence of a parents’ association, but lamented the lack of group
formation:
There isn’t a group. There’s the association, but there isn’t a group. A phone
call… I spent a month and a half going back and forth from the hospital, and I
never received a phone call. Not from the parents, not from the teachers at school
here, eh? Look, this displeased me a bit.
- Interview, Parent at Day Center
When parents discussed their goals for youth, as described in Chapter 5, autismspecific settings were not always among them. Parents may value other components of
the setting over autism-specificity for social purposes. One parent expressed concern for
a peer group based more on age than diagnostic characteristics:
[My son] must go to a community for people with autism. Because this is what he
needs. Or in a community at any rate for people with disabilities because I am not
going to say that they must live with people with autism. They must live with
354
other people their own age. They can live with people with any type of disability,
in my opinion. They don’t need to be among themselves because it’s not like they
appreciate each other much, people with autism, (one for the other), really they
don’t see, right, but they need to stay somewhere where there are people their own
age.
- Interview, Parent at Day Center
These examples demonstrate resistance to the idea of autism as a basis for social group
formation.
Unsurprisingly, youth at Social/Recreational Groups stressed the positives of
social group formation in their attendance – making friends and learning to relate to
people. Some youth linked their socialization to the autism specific nature, as we saw in
the previous chapter. Yet, as parents reported in Chapter 5 and youth verified in Chapter
6, youth in autism-specific settings often interacted more with staff and volunteers than
with each other. The presence of groups of people with autism did not necessarily mean
social groups of people with autism.
Moreover, youth themselves expressed conflicting opinions on not only the role
of autism (or specifically Asperger’s) as an identity, but also the way that identity could
or should be mobilized in a group context. These conflicting opinions arose especially
clearly in a discussion of a particular Asperger-focused film that one group watched. A
small discussion followed, and some participants did identify with the protoganist and
compare their experiences. They also asked questions of the neurotypical characters
behaviors and tried to understand them. However, one of the young men complained that
the Asperger-focused content of the films was inappropriate. He explained:
[T]hey only choose films about how being mentally different makes life hard.
People don’t want to sit around and watch films about how their life will be hard.
In Charlie and the Chocolate Factory, Willie Wonka is weird, so he has trouble
relating to people. Adam is weird, so he has trouble relating to people. Big Fish…
(Trails off)
355
[....] Forrest Gump is retarded, so he has trouble relating to people! I know this is
an Asperger center, but not everything has to be about how we are weird! [....]
You don’t go to a place for handicapped people and watch films about how not
having legs makes your life hard. [Fieldnote excerpt]
His views were not necessarily those of the mainstream. Certainly young adults with
Asperger's had recommended and critically praised some of the selected films. Others did
support an Asperger’s focus for many activities. This young man also participated in an
Asperger’s focused discussion group, but he described it not as a place to discuss
Asperger's, but a place where ‘discutiamo come migliorare Social/Recreational Group 2’
(we discuss how to improve Social/Recreational Group 2). Others did focus on the
Asperger’s aspects of these activities, such as a man this youth complained ‘want[s] to
add the prefix Aspie to everything.’ For some people, this collective identity was
important; for others, it was actively resisted.
The other codes refer to activism and advocacy behaviors – often in groups but at
times enacted by one individual mobilizing the claim of membership in a collectivity
(e.g., citizens with autism). Rose describes three forms or characteristics of
biocitizenship: informational, rights, and digital.
In informational biocitizenship, patients/parents use “specialized scientific and
medical knowledge of one’s condition” (Rose 2007:135) – I use it here to mean the role
of collectivities and parent networking in sharing information both scientific/medical and
administrative. Participants never offered counter examples, and indeed described several
cases of informational exchange from other youth or parents. Two youth heard about the
social/recreational activity group through personal networks of people on the spectrum,
friends of friends. Parents got a lot of administrative information from other parents, for
example:
356
Ah, In Italy it’s a bit different in the sense that there isn’t this paper that says,
“[my son] has autism” and they tell you “you have rights to this, you have rights
to that,” here you have to do it yourself in Italy. In the sense that when I went to
that center when [my son] was two years old, I learned from another mother than
[my son] could have the right to a little pension of 200€ which they call the
allowance. I didn’t know anything. When I returned I Informed myself. When
(my son) found out he had autism, no one told me anything, that is, it wasn’t like,
OK, if you leave the hospital they gave me the exception ticket. For the public
health service. But when I had this paper, I don’t know anything. 5, 6 years ago,
when I came to the [service], the association, I saw children like [my son] or even
more serious than [my son] that had different pensions, that had passes for their
cars. I said, “but [my son] no” because these things, you know them when you are
in the association, when you are among friends. No one comes to you, there isn’t
much information in Italy. There isn’t a paper that says “[your son] has autism,
you must do this this this this.”
- Interview, Parent at Social/Recreational Group
Parents also received practical information, and described talking with other parents
about solving daily life problems like finding comfortable clothing their children would
wear, and so on. In additional to one time informational exchanges, some parents
specifically followed the educational path of a family who went before them, a family
with a slightly older child. These long-term exchanges form a type of informational
biocitizenship. Parents who participated in associations also exercised informational
biocitizenship when they provided information to other parents, for example:
We organized some, um, training courses for the school teachers [....] And they
were both theoretical and practical courses, and we didn’t stop at just TEACCH.
Because in our opinion one association that represents parents must not be
sectorial, that is, the association must not say “only TEACCH works.” Because
some parents, let’s say, had some good results with facilitated communication and
believed in it. And well, as an association, in our opinion we had to do not just
what we as the association thought right but listen to all of the camps a bit.
- Interview, Parent at Residential Center
This parent discusses not only providing information, but providing it in a “democratic”
way, so to speak, ensuring that parents can access a broader range of information than
might be available elsewhere.
357
In rights biocitizenship. patients/parents engage in “campaigning for better
treatment, ending stigma, getting access to services, and like” (Rose 2007:135). Parents
exercised rights biocitizenship when they filed denuncie (reports) against
schools/services or individuals who mistreated them or their children, when they
advocated (sometimes with lawyers) to get the proper number of hours of support staff at
school, to keep their children in school, to graduate their children with the proper
credentials,64 and so on. Parents who participated in associations worked to train teachers
and educatori about autism, raising their awareness and improving their professional
competencies, and to monitor the activities of particular services as an advisory
committee. Though these actions were mostly limited to parents, recall also the mother in
Chapter 6 who described her son’s awareness raising efforts through creating a film about
Asperger’s. In these examples, parents and youth fought to raise awareness, access stategiven rights, and improve services.
There were also, however, counter examples, in which parents explained their
reasons for not fighting for rights or access. Some felt they could not, because they did
not have enough power against certain administrators or doctors, particularly if they were
in charge of multiple aspects of their child’s care. Others, however, did not thing it was
necessarily the right balance for their child’s wellbeing:
Fighting with professionals, in my opinion, would never have been a good thing.
At most if I wasn’t satisfied, more than saying so, I would let it go and look for
something else.
- Interview, Parent at Residential Center
Therefore when we found the right teachers it was really a good work, because
then I always sought to collaborate with the school. Actually, if they told me,
“look tomorrow” as to say “it’s better than he leaves first because the support
64
Diploma versus certificate of attendance, explained in more detail in Chapter 2.
358
teacher isn’t there,” I took him home. I didn’t insist on leaving him there where
maybe they would have put him in a corner. Regardless of the fact that he has all
his rights but I always privileged [my son]’s wellbeing over the rights that at any
rate he could have had.
- Interview, Parent at Residential Center
In digital biocitizenship, patients/parents participate in online communities around
the condition and find information, resources, and connections online. Parents reported
searching for services and doctors online, informarsi (informing themselves) through
online research, and participating in online listservs (especially linked to
social/recreational activity groups). Some older parents specifically explained that the
internet had opened up a lot more information than they had when their children were
first diagnosed. These comments still support the idea of digital biocitizenship, which
these parents identify as now present and lament not accessing. No parent specifically
resisted narratives of digital biocitizenship. Although youth did report using the internet,
recall the youth in Chapter 6 who explains, “That is, you can’t go on Facebook and write
“people with Asperger’s” and see some friends that you can look up.” In these examples,
the internet did provide an important avenue for social group formation and informational
exchange, but also had limitations.
I also include a code for activism, distinct from rights biocitizenship in that
instead of campaigning for access to resources and services provided by the state or other
actors, patients/parents create their own grassroots services and movements or volunteer
for existing associations. Three of the sites in which I conducted fieldwork were founded
by parents, embodying this ideal. Several parents participating in this foundation directly
or as peripheral supporters or funders. Parents also volunteered in these and other
services, particularly social/recreational activity groups (autism specific and not) that
359
required chaperones. Parents also worked to create their own plans, with greater or lesser
state support. As one mother explained:
My expectations are to succeed, are very ambitious in reality. To succeed to make
an absolutely respectful world around her. That is, I really have this expectation.
And therefore I will invent something if she doesn’t manage to follow the
scholastic path, I will invent a school tailored to her. I will build a job tailored to
her in such a way that she can have a paycheck but be very content. These seem
like children’s dreams but I believe in them a lot.
- Interview, Parent at Social/Recreational Group
This mother expressed a willingness to move into activism if official channels did not
support her daughter.
At one Social/Recreational Group, youth brought several ideas to the table and
took a leading role in planning activities. Youth in interviews expressed goals to organize
vacation trips, classes on topics of interest, fundraisers, and film nights. People with
autism also served as tutors or volunteer aides for other people with autism or other
disabilities, as discussed in Chapter 4 under the framework of auto-mutuo-aiuto. Both
parent and youth examples show the presence of activism and volunteerism.
Scholars of these theories will note that there are some I exclude, most
prominently Rose’s “economies of vitality” and all aspects of bioeconomics
(bioavailability and so on). My study was designed to focus on identity and subjectivity.
While these things relate, they are tangential to the present discussion. This section has
shown the major themes of new biopolitical theory that are relevant to identity and
subjectivity, and has fleshed out examples and negative cases. The next section
complicates new biopolitical theory by discussing the relative weight of different themes,
demonstrating the co-existence of both positive and negative cases, and analyzing the
overlap between particular themes.
360
Section 2: Complicating New Biopolitical Theory
The previous section defined new biopolitical theory, explicating its themes and
providing a more nuanced understanding than previous literature. This present section
complicates new biopolitical theory, exploring the relative weight of these themes in
participant narratives. It demonstrates the complexity of new biopolitical theory in the
data through several key examples. It explores situations in which positive and negative
biopolitical themes co-exist in a single narrative. This section demonstrates that a more
nuanced understanding of new biopolitical theory focused on multiple themes reveals
how the theory applies as well as the limits to its applicability, crafting a more locally
nuanced understanding of new biopolitical theory and refuting previous claims that these
theories are only applicable in “Europe and North America” by demonstrating that they
both do and do not apply in the Italian context. These findings are in line with anticipated
“threats” to new biopolitical theory explored in Chapter 1 – deinstitutionalization and
integration, lack of a consumer role, and “social-embeddedness” models of doctor-patient
interaction.
Frequency of Themes
361
Figure 5: New Biopolitical Theory Themes
Average % per Interview (N=48)
New Biopolitical Theory Themes
18
16
14
12
10
8
6
4
2
0
Molecularization
Somatic Expertise
Individual Identities
Collective Identities and
Socialities
Theme
PRO
CON
Figure 6: Molecularization Subthemes
Molecularization Subthemes
Average % per Interview (N=48)
1.2
1
0.8
0.6
0.4
0.2
0
Geneticization
Neurochemical/Neurostructural
Selves
Subtheme
PRO
CON
Microbiologization
362
Figure 7: Somatic Expertise Subthemes
Average % per Interview (N=48)
Somatic Expertise Subthemes
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1
0
Subtheme
PRO
CON
Figure 8: Individual Identities Subthemes
Individual Identities Subthemes
Average % per Interview (N=48)
0.6
0.5
0.4
0.3
0.2
0.1
0
Autism as
Identity
Cerebral
Autism as a Autism Pride
Subjectivation Way of Being
Subtheme
PRO
CON
Autism as
Normal
Social Model The Family
of Disability with Autism
363
Figure 9: Collective Identities and Socialities Subthemes
Average % per Interview (N=48)
Collective Identities and Biosocialities Subthemes
3.5
3
2.5
2
1.5
1
0.5
0
Social Group
Formation
Information
Biocitizenship
Digital
Biocitizenship
Rights
Biocitizenship
Activism and
Volunteerism
Subtheme
PRO
CON
The figures above demonstrates the presence of themes and negative examples,
calculated by averaging the percentage of each interview that was coded with each theme
(including those interviews in which the theme was completely absent). It considers
interviews with both parents and youth, and youth interviews both individual and in the
focus group. The most commonly discussed theme was Somatic Expertise, followed by
Collective Identities and Socialities, followed by Individual Identities, and finally
molecularization. Negative cases actually exceeded positive examples in two cases:
geneticization and autism as identity.65
This relative balance of themes indicates we might question the ‘bio’ in
biosociality. The themes of Molecularization, Somatic Individuality, and Cerebral
Subjectivation specifically address biology. Molecularization is the least present theme
overall. Within Somatic Expertise, Somatic Individuality is one of the least present
65
As the coding schema was designed on new biopolitical theory literature, it is designed to capture new
biopolitical theory and also identifies negative cases. It is not designed to truly compare presence versus
absence of these themes as absence in and of itself is hard to operationalize.
364
themes, along with Pilgrimages of Hope. However, somatic individuality has several
negative examples as well. Within the category of Individual Identities, cerebral
subjectivation is by far the least prevalent theme.
Although most participants cited organic, biological, neurological, or genetic
causes of autism (as reported in Chapters 5 and 6) they did not often talk about their
experiences in a biological frame. While examples of somatic individuality, such as those
presented in Section 1, outweighed negative cases, the negative cases were one of the
most prevalent negative themes. Participants rejected somatic individuality when they
questioned whether a characteristic was linked to the diagnosis or just a personality trait
and when they discussed the meaningless of the diagnosis, as discussed above. The
weakness of biological themes raises the question of whether the sociality expressed by
participants – factors of social group organization, patient literacy, authority of expertise,
and responsibilization which are definitely present – is really “bio” sociality. This finding
is in line with Eyal and colleagues’ (2014) observation that autism treatment,
intervention, and care is not biomedicalized, particularly because of the “low tech”
strategies involved (Hart 2014). Because it is not biomedicalization, Eyal and colleagues
argue, it is not Rabinow’s biosociality (which is just one form of the politics of life). The
authors nonetheless identify important forms of biosociality, while challenging both the
biomedicalization and geneticization of autism which are important components of some
theories of biosociality and the politics of life itself.66 My work unpacks in more detail
For other examples of scholars who have problematized the “bio” in biosociality, see C. Roberts’ (2007)
concept of bio-sociality which reflects her “desire to insist that the implosion of the biological and the
social is not as advanced or wide-ranging as [Rabinow, Rose, and Novas’s] work sometimes implies”; and
Marslands’ (2012) concept of (bio)sociality in Tanzanian social groups based around HIV, where Marsland
emphasizes the social. For another variation, see Beck and Niewohner’s (2009) “bio-cultural intimacy,”
66
365
the various components of new biopolitical theory, articulating how individual themes
may be more or less present. The next subsection turns to the overlap of these themes in
my data.
Overlap of Themes
By breaking new biopolitical theory into its component parts, this study can look
at the overlap between themes. Within positive examples of new biopolitical theory,
several domains substantially overlapped. Collective Identities and Socialities
substantially overlapped with Somatic Expertise.67
The domain of Individual identities also substantially overlapped with Somatic
Expertise.68 Specifically, The Family with Autism overlapped substantially with Patient
Literacy, as parents frequently sought out literature written by people with autism and
their families.69
The domain of Molecularization also overlapped substantially with Somatic
Expertise.70 Specifically, Neurologization and Somatic Individuality frequently
overlapped.71 Participants exercised somatic individuality often in terms of brain
discourses.
which merges biosociality with Michael Herzfeld’s (2005) cultural intimacy to “analyse historical, cultural
and social contingencies in the way biomedical options are appropriated and embedded into everyday life
via meaning-making practices.” See also E. Roberts’ (2008) argument against biosociality’s applicability in
Ecuador due to Ecuador’s local biology which does not hold the same meaning as the biology of
biosociality.f
67
35% of instances coded “Collective Identities and Socialities” were also coded “Somatic Expertise.” 11%
of instances coded “Somatic Expertise” were also collected Collective Identities and Socialities.”
68
24% of instances coded “Individual Identities were also coded “Somatic Expertise,” though only 4% of
instances coded “Somatic Expertise” were also coded “Individual Identities.”
69
23% of instances coded “The Family with Autism” were also coded “Patient Literacy,” though only 6%
of instances coded “Patient Literacy” were also coded “The Family with Autism.”
70
50% of instances coded “Molecularization” were also coded “Somatic Expertise,” though only 5% of
instances coded “Somatic Expertise” were also coded “Molecularization.
71
46% of instances coded “Neurologization” were also coded “Somatic Individuality.”34% of instances
coded “Somatic Individuality” were also coded “Neurologization.”
366
The domains within Collective Identities and Socialities strongly overlapped.
Social Group formation overlapped with Informational, Digital, and Rights
Biocitizenship. In other words, participants who described one type of Collective Identity
or Sociality often described more than one.
Perhaps more interesting are the several instances in which positive and negative
examples of new biopolitical theory overlapped. 11% of the instances coded positively
for biopolitical theory were also coded negatively, and 46% of instances coded negatively
for biopolitical theory were also coded positively. Common areas of overlap occurred
within Somatic Expertise,72 particularly in the relationship with pastoral keepers.
Interactions with professionals were overwhelmingly important to participants,
unsurprisingly. Whether or not these professionals acted as Pastoral Keepers played
heavily into participants’ discussions of Experiential Expertise and Responsibilization.
Parents were often responsibilized in the absence of pastoral guidance,73 and the presence
of pastoral guidance often overlapped with negative examples of responsibilization74 as
well as experiential expertise.75 This situation is strikingly similar to Valentine’s (2010)
Australian study, in which parents made consumer choices about treatment and
intervention “in response to shortfalls in resources.” Valentine notes, however, that
patient choice and activism still effectively lead to new forms of identity and community
(such as, I would argue, those described by new biopolitical themes of individual
12% of instances coded “Somatic Expertise” were also coded “Somatic Expertise CON” and 52% of
instances coded “Somatic Expertise CON” were also coded “Somatic Expertise.”
73
25% of instances coded “Pastoral Keepers CON” were coded “Responsibilization” and 6% of instances
coded Responsibilization” were coded “Pastoral Keepers CON.”
74
85% of instances coded “Responsibilization CON” were also coded “Pastoral Keepers,” whereas 14% of
instances coded “Pastoral Keepers” were coded “Responsibilization CON.”
75
57% of instances coded “Experiential Expertise CON” were also coded “Pastoral Keepers,” whereas only
2% of instances coded “Pastoral Keepers” were also coded “Experiential Expertise CON.”
72
367
identities and collective identities and socialities).
Although pastoral keepers – that is, autism professionals including doctors,
teachers, and social workers – often did guide families and help them “understand”
autism, almost as often they did not. Parents lamented not being told anything about the
diagnosis or about particular therapies. They lamented being kept out of rooms where
therapies were conducted. On the other hand, they praised professionals who
communicated often. Situations often arose where pastoral keepers did guide parents, but
did not responsibilize them. Parents often explained their choices in terms of who
suggested the services or therapy to them. Many parents made decisions about therapies
and services not based on their own research and decisions but because ci hanno proposto
– they proposed it. In several instances parents continued down certain paths – often
pharmaceutical, but also sometimes therapeutic – at all or longer than they wanted to
specifically because their doctor or another professionals told them to.
Several of the aspects of new biopolitical theory that are found lacking in these
discourses tie closely to important factors in the Italian context. As discussed in Chapter
2, several factors in the Italian context may limit the applicability of new biopolitical
theory. Firstly, in the 1970s Italy initiated a world-renowned movement of deinstitutionalization (Scheper-Hughes and Lovell 1986) followed by policies of social and
educational integration. Secondly, Italian medicine remains, for the most part, nonprivatized; therefore patients lack the consumer role seen in systems like U.S. healthcare
(Cola and Crocetti 2011). Thirdly, doctor-patient relationships are often characterized by
what some might call “paternalism” and Gordon and Paci (1997) call the “socialembeddedness narrative.”
368
For these reasons, new biopolitical subjectivities may be minimized in the Italian
context. Biosociality is exclusive by its specificity, whereas social integration is
inclusive. A lack of a consumer role for patients may minimize their mobilization of lay
expertise, as would paternalistic doctor-patient relationships. This section has
demonstrated that biosociality may be limited by the lack of a strong emphasis on its
‘bio’ components. Parents and youth were ambivalent about autism specific services, not
always seeing them as the right social context for people with the same type of condition.
In fact, many parents and youth noted that they preferred to interact with staff than each
other. This ties into the issues of doctor-patient relationships. These findings indicate that
parents’ and patients’ use of new biopolitical modes of engagement may be a response to
a lack of professionals’ use of these modes of engaging with them.
This section has looked at the complexity of new biopolitical theory by looking at
the ways the individual themes defined in Section 1 overlap. Positive and negative
examples of biopolitical theory often overlapped in a single narrative, even a single
passage. This finding demonstrates that it is not enough to say that new biopolitical
theory does or does not apply in a particular context, but to understand how it does or
does not apply. In the Italian context, much of this complexity comes from parent/patientprofessional relationships and negotiations of the role of expertise between them. Another
level of complexity comes from the extent to which autism is or is not understood as a
discrete biologically-based diagnosis in the Italian context, rendered less important by the
general disability nature of Italian integration laws and policies.
Section 3: Whose New Biopolitical Theory?
This final section turns to the question “whose new biopolitical theory?” It
369
tentatively explores potentially significant predictor variables that may impact the extent
to which an interviewee expressed the themes of new biopolitical theory. Most studies of
new biopolitical themes focus, quite rightly, on thematic analysis and the explication of
participant narratives. These methods are in line with the meaning-centered literature
from which these studies emerge and the relatively homogenous, focused samples that
best address this literature. They are also the analytical methods to which I dedicate most
time in this dissertation. Indeed, statistical analysis is limited in this relatively small and
non-representative sample. However, I would be remiss if I did not attempt to comment
on possible demographic factors impacting the relevance of new biopolitical themes for
participants in my study.
Although a few studies have looked at variation in parents’ mobilization of new
biopolitical theory, there is little data with which to compare my own work, which
generates hypotheses for important future research that fills a notable gap in the literature.
In a study of parents of children with Down Syndrome, Rapp (2000a:298) found, “In
New York City, active members [of support groups] are likely to be middle-class, white,
and parents of first children, or parents with the financial resources to use a host of
commercial services, including nannies and baby-sitters. Far less likely to rely on the
support groups are parents whose child with Down syndrome is a younger sibling; who
do not have much discretionary time or income; or who come from community
backgrounds with strong church or ethnic-group affiliation.”
My study looks at the following variables: parent age, parent sex, parent
interviewed singly or as a couple, parent income, parent religion, child age, child sex,
child being verbal or non-verbal, and type of site child attended. I have operationalized
370
these variables as follows:
Parent Age: Age in years of the parent. This variable is complicated by the
presence of couples. Due to the predominance of mothers over fathers, the mother’s age
was used for the nine couples. The average of all mothers’ ages (55) was used in place of
the actual age of the three fathers interviewed alone.
Parent Sex: Couples were marked as female.
Couples: A separate variable called couple accounted for the male. This variable
was created as a yes/no response, with the nine couples being marked yes.
Religion: In Chapter 2, I identify participants’ religions, based on open-ended
responses, as Catholic, Non-Practicing Catholic, Other Christian, Other Non-Christian,
and Atheist. However, as Chapter 2 shows, there is only one respondent identifying as
Atheist and two each as Other Christian and Other Non-Christian. For correlations, I
therefore categorized religion as Catholic and Non-Catholic.
Income: Income was categorized in a scaled range: €0 - €15.000, €15.000 €28.000, €28.000 - €55.000, €55.000 - €75.000, more than €75.000, and Don’t Know/No
Response/Not Asked.
Child Age was classified in years, Child Sex as male or female, and Child Verbal
or Non-Verbal via the procedure described in Chapter 3.
The site type was categorized as Day, Residential, or Social/Recreational.
Although these categories are closely related to specific site (the seven described in
Chapter 2), there are some notable exceptions. Because Residential Centers may also
offer Day Center services, not all participants recruited from Residential Sites currently
attended a Residence. Therefore, two interviews recruited from Residential Centers were
371
categorized as “Day Center” respondents. Moreover, one participant recruited from a Day
Center also attending a participating Residential Center. This respondent was categorized
as “Residential,” giving predominance to residence. Both of these categories were
organized into dummy variables with yes/no responses for calculated correlations.
The output variables for these regressions were the percentage of the interview
coded with new biopolitical themes. This rough metric gives some idea of the
relationship between demographics and the applicability of new biopolitical theory.
However, the open ended character of semi-structured interviews means that the metric
can only give a general idea and does not measure these associations with precision. The
following results should therefore be considered tentative and hypothesis-generating, not
hypothesis-testing.
I calculated correlations using Pearson’s regression. Overall, few areas of
statistically significant association were identified through this analyses. However, those
that were identified have implications for the study of new biopolitical theory: Child Sex
and Site Type.
Having a child who was female was correlated with a higher percentage of the
interview being coded with new biopolitical themes, r = .331, p < .05. Finding child sex
to be a potentially important indicator of new biopolitical theory highlights yet another
implication of the gender imbalance in autism, discussed in Chapters 1 and 2. The
experiences of women and girls with autism may be different from that of men and boys
in a way that substantially impacts the utility of new biopolitical themes. There may be
several reasons for this difference. To investigate these specificities in more detail, I also
looked at the percentage of the interview coded with each of the four subthemes:
372
molecularization, somatic expertise, individual identities, and collective identities and
socialities. Having a female child was correlated specifically with themes of Somatic
Expertise, r = .450 p < .05. As discussed in Section 2, Somatic Expertise was a theme in
which negative and positive examples often overlap, warranting a more detailed
investigation. Specifically, having a female child was associated with the themes of
professional expertise: Pastoral Keepers, r = .370, p < .05, and Pilgrimages of Hope, r =
.329, p < .05. These results suggest that parents of girls and women with autism spectrum
conditions may search farther (making pilgrimages) to find appropriate treatment and
services for their daughters. Further investigation into gender differences and new
biopolitical theory may shed light onto the reasons for this difference.
Site type was also significantly associated with the presence of new biopolitical
themes in interviews. Specifically, participants recruited from Residential Centers and
Social/Recreational Groups expressed more new biopolitical themes than those who were
recruited from Day Centers.76 Looking at subthemes, the site not being a Day Center was
correlated with the percentage of the interview coded for Collective Identities and
Socialities, r = -.497, p < .05. On the other hand, themes of Collective Identities and
Socialities were correlated with the site being a Social Group, r = .518, p < .05. The site
being a Residential Center was correlated with higher percentages of the interviews being
coded for Individual Identities, r = .383, p < .05.
These results suggest that structural factors may impact the relevance of new
biopolitical theory to participants. Indeed, Residential Centers and Social/Recreational
76
Percentage of the interview coded with new biopolitical themes was correlated with the site not being a
day center, r = -.394, p < .05
373
Groups often involved greater parent activism to get started (both Social/Recreational
Groups and one Residential Center were created through the efforts of parents’
associations). These parent activism responded to a lack of services already provided by
the State. As Valentine (2010) notes, such action in response to a lack of resources may
generate new forms of individual and collective identity. The State does, on the other
hand, provide a range of Day Centers either directly or through accreditation. Day
Centers are more entrenched in the Italian management of disability than autism-specific
Residential Centers or Social/Recreational Groups.
This finding, thought tentative and preliminary, points ot the importance of place.
New biopolitical theory explores the relationship between individuals and institutions,
generally conceiving of the institution of biomedicine broadly conceived. This
dissertation has highlighted the variation within biomedicine, not only between different
nations but between different smaller institutions within Italy. As Chapter 5 shows,
parents unsurprisingly had different experiences of and expectations for different types of
services. This study focused on three general types of services that had autism-focused
programs: Day Centers, Residential Centers, and Social/Recreational Groups. Day
Centers provided places for people with autism to go, post school-age, from roughly 9am
to 4pm. Many people with autism and their parents talked about them as being similar to
school or work and serving similar functions. Residential Centers provided 24/7 staff and
programming, sometimes in coordination with day services. They served as not only
residences but homes for people with autism, and addressed professional and parent
concerns about a “dopo di noi.” Social/Recreational Groups served a younger population,
providing places to gather in leisure time in evenings and weekends. They provided peer
374
group formation and friendship making opportunities. All of these services created
opportunities for people with autism and their families to meet each other. However, the
extent to which these meetings and groups enacted new biopolitical themes varied, as this
result suggests. This suggestion makes sense in light of the different functions and daily
rhythms of different services, as well as the different levels of state support and therefore
the origins of different types of services. It also raises avenues for future research into the
impact of different services types with the same overall biomedical system. Specifically,
future research should investigate the difference not only between different autismspecific services, but also services which are mixed-disabiltiy and which are
“mainstream” or completely inclusive. Are new biopolitical themes stronger in autismspecific services than inclusive services? Such an investigation would be particularly
appropriate in Italy, in which the ideology of integrazione scolastica and other policies of
social and educational intergration may mitigate the influence of new biopolitical theory
in contrast to, for example, the United States’ tradition of identity politics following the
Civil Rights Movement.
Correlations within the predictor variables may also provide insight into the
appeal of new biopolitical theory to different types of people. Social groups were
correlated with younger child age, r = -.651, p < .05, and Residential Centers were
correlated with older child age, r = .502, p < .05. Unsurprisingly, Social Groups were
also correlated with younger parent age, r = -.422, p < .05 (child age was correlated with
parent age r = .686, p < .05). The respondent being male was also correlated with the
child attending a Day Centers, male r = -.363, p < .05, but this is likely an artifact of the
data in that there were very few interviews conducted with only males (n = 3).
375
These very tentative results suggest some elements that may be important in
understanding for whom new biopolitical theory may be important. Understanding
demographic variables may help providers and activists decide how to work with
different parents, avoiding some of the stress and burden of choice Valentine (2010)
describes in Australia.
I propose future research that investigates more systematically the potential
themes identified in this analysis and the preceding presentation of ethnographic data. My
research identifies important differences based on type of service. While my research
design here focused on Day Centers, Residential Centers, and Social Groups, participants
also discussed the importance of the educational context and the differences between
autism specific, mixed-disability, and inclusive settings. I envision targeted survey
research that incorporates these salient categories, and compares the experiences of
respondents with autism and parents of children with autism based on their basic
demographics (age, sex, religion) but also their experiences in these different kinds of
services throughout the life cycle. Such a study will have the potential to identify factors
that influence parent service choice as well as the saliency of new biopolitical modes of
engagement.
Conclusion
This chapter answers research question (3), “How does new biopolitical theory
apply, or not, to social organization around autism in Italy?” It does so in three ways:: it
defines and operationalizes new biopolitical theory; it complicates new biopolitical
theory by exploring the overlap, or lack thereof, in its four key themes; and it explores
demographic factors that may affect the relevance of new biopolitical theory to different
376
participants. I argue that new biopolitical theory can be understand as composed of four
main themes: molecularization, somatic expertise, individual identities, and collective
identities and socialities. Future researchers can use these themes to understand the
applicability of new biopolitical, or lack thereof, in different contexts with nuance that
allows for the understanding of different local biopolitics under different local
biomedicines. In my research context, explicitly biological themes of new biopolitical
theory were found most lacking. Themes of somatic expertise were most common, but
also the source of the most overlap between positive and negative examples. I find that
parents and youth mobilized experiential expertise primarily in the absence of pastoral
keepers, and did not mobilize experience expertise in the presence of pastoral keepers.
Pastoral keepers did not cultivate patients’ expertise. Rather, patients used experiential
expertise when they did not have professional experts on whom to rely. In this sample,
parent demographics were not significantly correlated with influence of new biopolitical
themes in interviews. Child sex was significant, with percentage of new biopolitical
themes coded in interviews being correlated with having a female child. The type of site
the child attended was also significant. These results provide suggestions for future
research into the factors that impact the influence and relevance of new biopolitical
theory for different individuals.
Overall, my research suggests that new biopolitical theory may be shaped in
reaction not to new biomedical technologies alone, as the literature sometimes suggests,
but to the healthcare and social services network in which they are embedded. In this
way, my research both challenges and expands new biopolitical theory, and therefore has
implications for anthropological theory and scholarship beyond autism, encompassing all
377
studies of biopolitics, biological citizenships, and biosocialities.
378
Conclusion and Implications
Research Questions and Answers
This dissertation has explored how autism is defined, managed, and experienced
in the Italian context. Overall, it demonstrates that the local particularity of the Italian
medical system – broadly defined as including health and human services, education, and
a range of medical subspecialties – impacts the way autism is conceptualized in Italy and
the ways that people with autism and their families define, manage, and experience this
diagnosis. The preceding six chapters have provided a detailed exploration into this
health and human services network and the social world of autism in Italy. Chapter 2
provided an overview of autism history and management, including the three types of
services discussed most in this dissertation: Day Centers, Residential Centers, and
Social/Recreational groups. Chapter 3 provided detailed descriptions of life inside these
centers and groups. Chapters 4 through 6 answered research questions 1 and 2.
Research question (1) asks, “How is autism conceptualized in Italy?” Through
participant observation at autism-specific services and a detailed exploration of the
narratives of professionals, parents of youth with autism, and youth with autism
themselves, this study provides several insights. The professional conceptualization of
autism defints it as a constellation of symptoms in line with the DSM-IV, plus behavioral
problems, rigidity, sensory and perception problems, and special strengths. The three
most commonly mentioned characteristic of autism by both professionals and parents
were the three characteristics of the Wing-Gould triad: qualitative impairment in social
interaction; qualitative impairment in communication; and restricted repetitive and
stereotyped patterns of behavior, interests and activities. The three most commonly
379
mentioned characteristics of autism by youth on the spectrum were qualitative
impairment in social interaction; restricted repetitive and stereotyped patterns of
behavior, interests and activities; and motor difficulties or clumsiness.
Each group also speculated on the possible causes of autism. For professionals,
the top three possible causes were genetics (hereditary or non-heriditary); a broadly
understood biological, neurological, or organic cause; and genetic predisposition
combined with environmental triggers. For parents, the top three things they said caused
or maybe caused autism were vaccines, neurological causes, or causes related to fetal
development. Few youth were interviews and even fewer speculated on the cause of
autism. The only cause that was raised by more than one youth (in a focus group
interview) was social construction – the idea that Asperger’s specifically was a diagnosis
physicans gave a wide range of people and that it was meaningless. Both professionals
and parents agreed that even if the cause of autism was uncertain, it was certainly not
caused by “refrigerator mothers.”
Professionals, parents, and youth all drew in varying degrees on narratives of
autism as difference (but not necessarily deficit), which resonates with philosophy of
neurodiversity. For example, professionals talked about autism as a modo (a way) of
being, functioning, or perceiving, such as the professional who explained, “a person with
autism is a person that lives with parameters different than ours.” Parents drew on this
discourse as well, though fewer parents than professionals used the language of a way of
being. Parents more often spoke of being with autism as living in their own world,
sometimes in relationship to the idea of autism-as-difference but often to describe the
characteristic of autism-as-being-isolated. However, when parents spoke to their children
380
they described autism as part of a typical human balance of strengths in weaknesses, as in
the mother who stressed, “Each person is able to do some things, but is not able to do
others.” Finally, youth also described autism as a different way of being or functioning,
as a trait like any other physical or personality trait, and as normal. Youth also sometimes
used autism pride or otherwise overtly positive narratives to talk about autism such as
“nerd power.”
Related to this conceptualization of autism as a holistic way of being, most
professionals favored treatments and interventions tailored for people with autism based
on their characteristics, under various philosophical umbrellas but all favoring structuring
the environment to help people with autism learn rather than fitting people with autism
into a hostile world. The top three specific approaches favored by professionals (alone or
modified/integrated with other approaches) were TEACCH, ABA, and pharmaceuticals.
The top three specific approaches favored by parents were pharmaceuticals,
psicomotricità and (tied for third) Horse Therapy or Music Therapy. Youth talked less
about specific approaches and more about specific therapists who helped them.
In general, my research has shown that the on-the-ground conceptualization of
autism in Italy is as a condition with characteristics consistent with the DSM-IV-TR and
DSM-5, emerging from largely unknown biological causes, leading a person with autism
to have a qualitatively different way of being, functioning, or perceiving that merits
interventions which seek to structure the environment and facilitate learning and
adaptation for people with autism.
Research question (2) asks, “How do people defined as autistici and their families
define, manage, and experience that label?” Through participant-observation with youth
381
with autism participating in autism-specific services and interviews with these youth and
their families, this dissertation generates several findings regarding autism experienes. As
discussed above, people with autism and their parents defined autism in a wider variety
of ways, still often related to the characteristics of the DSM-IV-TR. Notably, people with
autism and their parents did not always find the diagnosis of autism meaningful, and
many contested the diagnosis in various ways as described in Chapters 5 and 6. Some
parents argued for other diagnoses on or not on the autism spectrum. Some also contested
the association between autism and intellectual disability. Parents also talked about “pure
autism” as a particular archetype and compared and contrasted their children’s autism
with this idea. People on the spectrum – particularly youth with lower levels of support
needs participating in Social/Recreational Activity Groups – sometimes challenged the
validity of the diagnosis at all, particularly Asperger’s Syndrome. Some highlighted the
ways it is socially constructed, and many explained the way they saw themselves as
“normal” despite a neuropsychiatric label.
Indeed, people with autism and their parents often described autism using
normalizing language of various sorts. Parents normalized participation in autism-specific
services, such as the parent who said his daughter’s move to a Residential Center was “as
though she were married” because she left home at the same age he would have expected
with marriage. Youth used very explicitly normalizing language, explaining how they
saw themselves as normal. Parents who discussed autism with their children often talked
about a balance of strengths and weaknesses, as discussed above. Youth sometimes drew
on this narrative as well. Youth discussed autism primarily in terms of the social
consequences of the diagnosis including this balance and their social fears. As one youth
382
explained, “I was afraid of not being understood.” Other expressed fears of and
experiences with bullying and mistreatment. The fears these youth had related not to the
characteristics of autism itself (such as clumsiness, difficulty with social interaction, etc.)
but the social consequences of not being accepted due to their characteristics.
This study also explored the relationships people with autism and their families
had with professionals. While relationships with peers could be important for many
youth, relationships with staff were perhaps the most important. Both parents and youth
talked about specific professionals with whom the youth with autism had strong, often
ongoing relationships. Both lamented relationships that had ended as staff left the job.
During participant-observation, I keenly felt the absence of certain popular staff
members, as youth spoke of them frequently. In terms of parents’ relationships with
professionals, this study shows the many ways that parents searched for competent
experts to care for their children, but did not necessarily become experts themselves.
In general, my research has shown that people with autism and their families
define, manage, and experience autism in a range of ways, often contesting the diagnosis,
but also drawing on many discourses that render their difference normal and
understandable.
Chapter 7 focused on the biopolitical subjectivity of people with autism and their
parents. It answered research question (3), “How does new biopolitical theory apply, or
not, to social organization around autism in Italy?” It finds that biopolitical theory applies
in some ways, but in other ways it does not. Many biopolitical themes were present in
participant narratives, most prominently those related to somatic expertise. Explicitly
biological themes of new biopolitical theory were found most lacking. Althought parents
383
and youth exercised many aspects of new biopolitical theory as I have operationalized it –
such as patient literacy, social group formation, autism pride, and activism – they do not
talk about their experiences within an explicitly biological framework. My results
therefore call into question the “bio” of new biopolitical theory. Another area in whih my
research challenges and expands new biopolitical theory is in the domain of patientprofessional interaction. Parents and youth mobilized their personal experiential expertise
primarily in the absence of pastoral keepers. Rather than parents and youth working
alongside pastoral keepers to cultivate their own expertise and become collaborators in
their care, my research shows that many parents and patients used their own expertise
only when they could not rely on professional experts. Often, they did not use their own
expertise when they could turn to the guidance of professional experts instead. My work
also generates hypothesis for future research by exploring variability between participants
with respect to their use of new biopolitical discourses. Factors that may impact the
influence of new biopolitical theory are child sex and type of site the child attended.
These results point to the need for more research into gendered experiences along the
autism spectrum; and to the role of smaller institutions such as services in promoting or
mitigating the influence of new biopolitical themes on participant narratives. These
results highlight the importance of macrostructural factors in impacting individual
subjectivity, a central concern to psychological anthropology and anthropology more
broadly.
In general, my research has shown that some aspects of new biopolitical theory
apply in the Italian context, while others do not, based on locally particular features of
Italian biomedicine and health and human services.
384
Contributions to the Literature
In this section, I tie the findings of this study back to the key bodies of literature
discussed in Chapter 1. Chapter 1 reviewed the anthropology of biomedicine, the
anthropology of autism, new biopolitical theory, and the anthropology of youth. It
explained how the research questions emerge from these bodies of literature. I now
explain how this study has contributed to these bodies of literature.
This study contributes to the anthropology of biomedicine and of Europe and the
Mediterranean by continuing the anthropological project of de-homogenizing
“biomedicine” and “Western medicine,” elucidating several locally particular features of
the Italian context. It provides an in depth description of the Italian healthcare and social
services system as it relates to autism spectrum conditions and disability management
more broadly. Looking at the Italian healthcare and social services system through the
lens of autism highlights several important factors important to participants experiences
including the division between child neuropsyciatry and adult psychiatry (and their
respective histories), the history of deinstitionalization and subsequent policies of social
and educational intergration, and the organization of social services outside of schooling
– particularly the Day Centers, Residential Centers, and Social/Recreational Groups
described herein.
My research also contributes to the anthropology of autism through both critical
autism studies and the cross-cultural study of autism. It provides an ethnographic
description of autism experiences in this Italian context. This experience-near
representation furthers critical autism studies by presuming the competence and value of
the perspectives of people with autism and seeking participant-observation insights that
385
also include the experiences of youth with autism with high levels of support needs. This
study used several methods to access the experiences of youth with autism across the
spectrum, and reflections on the relative utility of these methods will be useful for
encouraging future researchers to include these experiences as well. This study also
contributes to the cross-cultural study of autisms worldwide and the globalization of
academic conversation about autism, through the specific case of Italy. It includes the
perspectives of professionals, parents of youth with autism, and youth with autism
themselves in the Italian context – allowing for implicit comparison with research
findings in other parts of the globe. Because this study does not presume a definition of
autism, it allows for a rich exploration into the many meanings of the autism concept in
this local context.
This study challenges and expands new biopolitical theory through a combination
of foci drawing form the previous two liteartures. It refines and operationalizes
definitions about these theories and exploring the limits of their applicability in the key
case of autism in a Western context distinct from Austria and the United States where
autism was first described. Literature on new biopolitical theory has often found social
organization around autism to be an exemplary case, both for people with autism and
their parents. Much of this research has taken place in the U.S., U.K., and Canada. This
dissertation has explored this case in detail in a different Western biomedical context –
Italy. It begins by operationalizing new biopolitical theory into four main themes as
identified in a close reading of the literature. It then investigates the applicability of these
themes in the Italian context, creating a nuanced description and rising to the call of
previous scholars to increase locally and historically particular understandings.
386
Furthermore, my research suggests that new biopolitical theory may be shaped in reaction
not to new biomedical technologies alone, but to the healthcare and social services
network in which they are embedded.
Finally, this study contributes to the anthropology of youth through its focus on
adolescents and young adults aged 14-34 participating in autism-specific services. It
explicates the life cycle perspectives on professionals and parents, and explores their
efforts to create opportunities for adult roles for young people at this transitional age.
Professionals and parents worked on creating an age appropriate distancing between
parents and children, and parents searched for “a little peace” for themselves and their
children in adulthood. I argue that a rights-based approach to the life cycle of people with
autism can challenge and improve the anthropological definition of adolescence and its
conclusion of “full adult roles” by defining adulthood as an amalgamation of materials
and roles to which all individuals have a right based on the standard of their peers.
Future Research
The strong ethnographic foundation of this study raises several avenues for future
research. Focused studies should explore in more detail several questions raised in this
dissertation. Future research on autism diagnostic practices and understanding should
compare diagnoses reported in different ways for the same individual: by a physician, by
the local health board, by service coordinators, by parents, and by the individual himself
or herself. Such a study would have implications for understanding the saliency and
desirability of an autism diagnosis for different groups. Additionally, future research
should address demographic factors affecting the relevance of new biopolitical theory for
different groups, with specific attention to women and girls with autism. This study is
387
largely unable to comment on differences in experiences in autism-specific contexts
when compared to mixed disability contexts and fully inclusive contexts. Future research
should include participants in these other contexts to determine if mixed-disability or
fully inclusive settings impact the relevance of new biopolitical theory.
Broader Impacts
This conclusion has focused primarily on the contribution of this dissertation to
the field of anthropology, but I would like to end by outlining my hopes for its broader
impacts. The participants for this research including professionals, parents, and youth
with autism. I hope that this work is of interest and of use to each of these groups.
For professionals, I hope that the findings of this dissertation can help foster
international communication and collaboration by providing an English-language
description of Italian work. I furthermore hope that it can help foster professional-parent
collaboration in Italy and elsewhere by sharing parent perspectives, and by suggesting
potential ways to identify who may find biopolitical modes of engagement and
citizenship useful and therefore prefer to work as a co-expert alongside professionals, and
who may be seeking to use professional expertise without becoming a co-expert.
For parents both in Italy and elsewhere, I hope that the narratives of parents in this
dissertation resonates with their experiences. I hope that parents may find it interesting to
read about others’ experiences even halfway around the globe, and I hope I have drawn
sufficient attention to the issues that are important to them in order to share those
priorities with readers of all types.
For youth with autism, I hope that this dissertation has in some small way helped
them have a greater voice. I hope that youth feel represented in this work. It was very
388
important to me to conduct research focused on the lived experiences of people with
autism, and not writing about people with autism without including people with autism. I
hope I have done this task justice.
This dissertation has provided a rich ethnographic exploration of the social world
of autism in Italy. It therefore creates a strong foundation for future cross-cultural
research in the anthropology of autism. A self-conscious cross-cultural study of autism
that approaches the subject from a meaning-centered perspective will be of interest to
anthropologists, practitioners, advocates, and people affected by autism through their
own diagnosis or the diagnosis of a loved one, with implications for research, clinical
practice, and advocacy.
389
Appendix 1 – Interview Guide Topics
Staff and Professionals
1)
2)
3)
4)
5)
6)
7)
Vorrei cominciare con qualche domande demografiche e professionali.
a.
Età:
b.
Sesso:
c.
Qual è la Sua religione?
d.
Dove è nato?
e.
Qual’è la Sua professione?
f.
Dove ha studiato?
g.
Cosa ha studiato?
Questo studio è focalizzato sull’autismo. Quindi vorrei sentire come
definirebbe l'autismo (sindrome di Asperger/DPS) nelle parole Sue?
a.
Quali sono le caratteristiche?
b.
Cos’è la differenza tra autismo è Asperger? (Cos’è la relazione tra
Asperger è lo spettro autistico?)
c.
Cosa è il processo per una diagnosi?
i. Da chi?
ii. Quale esame o test?
iii. Una persona con una diagnosi, cosa riceve in seguito in termine di
diritti, servizi, aiuta, ecc?
Ora che abbiamo discusso cos’è l’autismo, potrebbe dirmi un po’ riguardante
il suo lavoro? Che cosa fa?
a.
Coma va una settimana tipica?
Personalmente, come ha cominciato a lavorare con autismo/DPS?
Prima di questo lavoro, Lei ha conosciuto qualcuno con autismo?
Questa organizzazione/servizio
a.
la storia di questo organizzazione:
i. Mi può raccontare la storia della organizazzione?
ii. Quando è stato fondato? Da chi? Come mai?
b.
Che cosa fa questa organizzazione? Come mai?
i. Qual’è la filosofia di intervento qui?
ii. Quali sono gli approcci usati?
1. Quali sono i vantaggi del questo approccio?
2. Ci sono alcuni svantaggi?
c.
Lavora con famiglie che usano altri approcci? Quali sono questi
approcci?
i. È utile, o sbagliato?
gli obiettivi dall’organizzazione per gli utenti con DPS
a.
Quali sono gli obiettivi di questa organizzazione?
i. Per gli utenti con autismo?
ii. Per i loro genitori e le loro famiglie?
iii. Per italia, in senso della politica, la rapporto con la communità, e la
quadra ampia?
b.
Hanno cambiato dall’inizio?
390
8)
9)
10)
11)
12)
Com’è questa organizzazione coinvolta con altri servizi per persone con
DPS/autismo/Asperger?
a.
So che il sistema scolastico è molto importante per persone con DPS
che usano servizi di insegnanti di sostegno, ecc. Com’è questa
organizzazione coinvolta con servizi scolastici per persone con
DPS/autismo/Asperger?
b.
Quali sono i suoi pensieri/reflessioni riguardante servizi scolastici per
gli studenti con DPS/autismo/Asperger.
i. Ci sono problemi? Descrivergli.
ii. Quali tipi di servizi vorrebbe avere alla scuole?
Abbiamo quasi finito, ma vorrei chiederle anche sulle cause di autismo. So
che ci sono molte teorie – genetiche, neurologiche, ambientali. Secondo Lei,
cosa è la causa più probabile?
a.
Ci sono cause organiche?
b.
Cause sociali o nella famiglia?
c.
Cause ambientali?
È possibile guarirlo?
Che tipo di ricerca dovremmo sostenere?
Che altro devo conoscere su questa organizzazione? Sul Suo lavoro?
Sull'autismo?
391
Parents
1)
2)
3)
Vorrei cominciare con qualche domande demografiche e professionali.
a. Età:
b. Sesso:
c. Qual’è la Sua religione?
d. Dove è nato/a?
e. Dove abita?
f. Qual’è la Sua professione?
g. Potrei chiederle qual’è il Suo reddito familiare per anno?
i. €0 - €15.000
ii. €15.000 - €28.000
iii. €28.000 - €55.000
iv. €55.000 - €75.000
v. Più di €75.000
h. Dove ha studiato?
i. Cosa ha studiato?
j. Quanti figli/e ha?
i. Le età degli figli/e:
ii. Chi ha una diagnosi? Solo ___?
iii. Qual’è la diagnosi?
iv. Ha avuto diagnosi precedente?
v. Ha attualmente diagnosi aggiuntivi?
vi. Ha un altro termine o espressione per descrivere i problemi di
Suo/a figlio/a?
Ora, vorrei chiederle sulle Sue esperienza prima della diagnosi.
a. Potrei chiederle della gravidanza? [se sì]: Ci sono stati alcuni problemi o
malattie durante la gravidanza? Può descriverceli.
i. Dove ha partorito? (In ospedale? A casa?)
ii. Come ha partorito (naturale, parto cesareo, parto nell’acqua, parto
pretermine, ecc.)?
b. Ora vorrei chiederle di Suo/a figlio/a nell’infanzia: ha avuto problemi e/o
malattie?
c. Quando Suo/a figlio/a ha avuto problemi di salute o difficoltà per la prima
volta? Quanti anni aveva Suo figlio/a?
i. Che cosa è sucesso?
ii. Chi ne ha notato? (Amici? Parenti?)
iii. Che cosa hanno detto?
iv. Che cosa le hanno suggerito?
d. Come ha deciso di chiedere aiuto per il Suo/a figlio/a?
i. Quanti anni aveva Suo/a figlio/a questa volta?
ii. Da chi (che tipo di professionista)?
iii. Perché questo tipo di aiuta?
Ora vorrei farle delle domande specifiche sulla diagnosi
a. Quando ha realizzato che questi problemi potrebbero essere un disturbo
pervasive dello sviluppo (DPS)?
392
4)
5)
6)
b. Chi ha fatto la diagnosi?
i. Uno professionale o molti?
ii. Che esami ed indagini hanno effetuato?
iii. Che cosa le hanno detto su [questa diagnosi]?
iv. Quali trattimenti le hanno suggerito?
c. Quali sono le implicazioni della diagnosi? Che cosa succede in seguito:
servizi, aiuti, sostegni, ecc.
d. E’ in disaccordo con qualcuno?
e. Che cosa pensava su queste diagnosi quando ha sentito per la prima volta?
f. Che cosa pensa ora?
Ha detto che la diagnosi del Suo/a figlio/a è [diagnosi]. Che cosa signica per
lei?
a. Che problemi vi ha causato?
b. Che cosa le fa più paura a dei [questa diagnosi]?
c. Quali sono i problemi o le preoccupazioni attuali?
d. Quali sono le vostre aspettative per il futuro?
e. Quali sono le vostre speranze?
f. Quali sono state le reazioni dalla Sua famiglia alla diagnosi di Suo/a
figlio/a (parenti, fratelli, marito/moglie)
Le ringrazio molto per raccontami la Sua storia. Ora vorrei farle qualche
domande sull’autismo in particolare.
a. Con parole Sue, cosa è l’autismo [o la diagnosi particolare dal Suo/a
figlio/a].
b. Quali sono le caratteristiche/quali sono i tratti di suo figlio che vede come
autismo?
c. Che cosa l'ha causata (per suo figlio)?
d. Come ha causato? Come funziona la causa, cioè, qual’è il meccanismo?
e. Ci sono alcuni altre cause?
f. Perché è successo quando è successo? (collegata con l’età, eventi
specifici?)
g. come descrive i problemi dal Suo figlio ai amici o colleghi?
h. Come descrive i problemi dal Suo figlio ai sconosciuti?
i. Come descrivi i problemi dal Suo figlio a Suo figlio, se parlate tra di voi?
j. Una persona nella vosta famiglia ha mai avuto problemi simili? In che
senso?
k. Conosce qualcuno, come un amico o collega, che ha mai avuto problemi
simili? In che senso?
l. Lei ha sentito parlare di una persona con questi problemi nei media? In
che senso?
Gradirei parlare un po’ di più sul Suo figlio oggi.
a. Quali sono le cose che gradisce meno?
b. Quali sono i suoi interessi particolari?
c. Quali sono le sue abitudini o comportamenti abituali?
d. Come va Suo/a figlio/a con le attivita` di vita quotidiana (andare al bagno,
fare la doccia, cucinare, mangiare, vestirsi)?
e. Come va la sua communicazione?
393
7)
La ringrazio di questi racconti della vostra vita. Ora vorrei sentire le sue
reflessioni sui trattamenti e gli interventi.
a. Quali interventi ci sono stati nella scuola (insegnanti di sostegno, servizi e
attivita`)? Come andava?
i. Quando è partito da scuola?
b. Quali tipi di terapia o intervento avete provato, e per quale difficoltà?
i. Programmi di terapia (musicaterapia, psicomotricità, ecc)?
ii. Usate alimenti particolari?
iii. Usate medicina/farmaci? Cosa tipo?
c. Ci sono altri trattimenti o interventi utilizzati?
d. Come va ogni intervento?
e. Quali sono i risultati più importanti che spera di ricevere da ogni
trattamento?
f. Per questi trattamenti, cosa si aspettava di ricevere che non ha ricevuto?
Tipo di therapia
8)
Per quale
difficoltà
Risultati aspettati
Qualcosa si aspettava
che non ha ricevuto
La ringrazio molto, le sue reflessioni sono molto importanti. Abbiamo quasi
finito. Ora, gradirei conoscere la vostra esperienza con questa organizzazione.
a. Come è venuta/o conoscenza del servizio?
b. Come ha cominciato ad usufruirne? Come è stata la prima visita?
c. Perché ha scelto qui per Suo figlio?
d. Ha visto altri opzioni che non ha scelto? [Se sì] Come ha deciso tra qui e
lì?
e. Quali sono i vostri obiettivi della frequentazione? Quali sono i risultati più
importanti che spera di ricevere dalla frequenza?
f. Come va la frequentazione per suo figlio? Cosa dice o esprime su di qui?
g. C’è cosa si aspettava di ricevere che non ha ricevuto?
h. Suo figlio frequenta altri organizzazioni o servizi? Quali?
394
9)
i. Cosa fa di là?
ii. Come è venuta la conoscenza?
iii. Quali sono i vostri obiettivi in quelle situazioni? Quali sono i
risultati più importanti che spera di ricevere dalla frequenza?
iv. C’è cosa si aspettava di ricevere che non ha ricevuto?
i. Frequenta Lei come genitore altri organizzazioni o servizi? Quali?
i. Cosa fa di là?
ii. Come è venuta la conoscenza?
iii. Quali sono i vostri obiettivi in quelle situazioni? Quali sono i
risultati più importanti che spera di ricevere dalla frequenza?
iv. C’è cosa si aspettava di ricevere che non ha ricevuto?
C’è qualcos'altro che dovrei sapere, qualsiasi altra cosa che è importante per
Lei?
395
Youth with Autism – Individual Interview
1) Prima, vorrei chiederti alcune domande su di te.
a) Quanti anni hai?
b) Sei maschio o femmina?
c) Qual’è la tua religione?
d) Dove è nato?
e) Dove abiti?
f) Cosa fai per lavoro?
g) Potrei chiederti qual’è il tuo reddito familiare per anno?
i. €0 - €15.000
ii. €15.000 - €28.000
iii. €28.000 - €55.000
iv. €55.000 - €75.000
v. Più di €75.000
h) Dove hai studiato? Dove stai studiando?
i) Cosa hai studiato? Cosa stai studiando?
2) Come abbiamo detto, sto conducendo uno studio sulla esperienza di persone con
diagnosi (o auto-diagnosi) sullo spettro autistico. Hai una diagnosi? [se no, o no
ho capito, chiedi: sai cos’è l’autismo?]
a) cos’e`?
b) cosa voul dire (secondo te)?
i. Che problemi [questa diagnosi] ti ha causato?
ii. Che cosa ti fa più paura di [la diagnosi]?
iii. Hai qualche diretto o accesso a servizi a causa della diagnosi?
Quali sono?
iv. Quali sono i comportamenti o carrateristiche che ti attribuisci a
causa della diagnosi?
v. Che cosa significa per la tua famiglia, per la tua scuola, per il tuo
lavoro, ecc?
vi. Usi un altro termine per descrivere la tua difficoltà/diversità?
3) Grazie mille, sono molto interessata. Continuiamo? Vorrei sentire come funziona
[la tua diagnosi].
a) con parole tue:
i. Cos’è l’autismo?
ii. Quali sono le caratteristiche?
iii. Come funziona [la tua diagnosi]?
iv. Che cosa l’ha causata?
1. C’è qualcosa che è sucesso all’interno del tuo corpo? Cosa?
2. C’è qualcosa che è accaduto nella tua famiglia, al lavoro, o
nella tua vita sociale?
3. Per te, la diagnosi è correlata ad eventi specifici accaduti
nella tua vita? In che senso?
b) Come descrivi la tua diagnosi ai altri?
i. Ai compagni?
ii. Ai sconosciuti?
396
iii. Ai cari?
c) C’è qualcuno nella tua famiglia con le difficoltà/caratteristiche simili? In
che senso? Come è questa persona, simile a te? Diversa rispetto a te?
d) Hai amici o compagni con le difficoltà/caratteristiche simili? In che senso?
Come è questa persona? simile a te? Diversa rispetto a te?
e) Conosci qualcuno in TV, in film, o in libro con le difficoltà/caratteristiche
simili? In che senso? Come è questa persona? simile a te? Diversa rispetto
a te?
4) Grazie per questi racconti. Dimmi, ti ricordi quando sei stato diagnosticato? [If
no] Hai sentito dettagli dai genitori o qualcuno altro? [If no, skip to section 5].
a) Quali sono i signali precoci di questa diagnosi – chi lo ha notato? (Tu?
Genitori? Altri parenti? Insegnanti? Amici?)
b) Che cosa ti hanno detto?
c) Che cosa hanno suggerito?
d) Quanti anni aveva a questo punto?
e) Cosa è successo dopo?
f) Come hai ricevuto la diagnosi – da chi?
i. Che cosa è successo alla visita con [professionista]? Che cosa è
sucesso dopo?
g) Hai avuto altre diagnosi precedenti? Aggiuntivi?
5) Grazie di nuovo, hai fatto benissimo. Ora, vorrei parlare sui professionisti che hai
incontrato per [la tua diagnosi].
a) Come è andata la scoula? Con chi hai lavorato? Hai avuto insegnanti di
sostegno? Hai avuto educatori?
b) Hai vistato una psicologa? Cosa avete fatto insieme?
c) Hai vistato una neuropsichiatra? Cosa avete fatto insieme?
d) Hai vistato un altra terapista? Chi? Cosa avete fatto insieme?
e) Cosa mangi? Cosa non mangi? Perché?
f) Prendi medicina? Cosa? Perché?
g) Fai qualcosa altro che devo conoscere?
6) Abbiamo quasi finito! Vorrei sentire sulla tua esperienza qui, a [nome della
organizzazione]
a) come hai fatto a saperlo? Chi ti ha detto sulla organizzazione?
b) Hai deciso di frequentare, o è stato la decisione di un altra persona? Chi?
c) Come è andata la prima visita? Cosa è successo?
d) Cosa fai qui? Cosa hai fatto oggi?
e) Hai detto che fai [elenco delle attività].
i. Qual’è la tua preferita?
ii. Preferisci [attività A o attività B]?
iii. Quali attività non ti piacciono molto?
iv. Perché fai [attività]
f) Perché vieni qui?
g) Quali sono i tuoi obbiettivi per la frequenza qui?
h) Cosa vuoi [questa organizzazione] fare?
i. Ti lo fanno fare?
397
i) C’è qualcosa che non va?
j) Che cosa dovrebbe cambiare?
7) Hai frequentato altre organizzazioni?
a) come hai fatto a saperlo? Da chi?
b) Come è andata la prima visita di là?
c) Cosa fai di là? Cosa hai fatto di là?
d) Hai detto che fai [elenco delle attività].
i. Qual’è la preferita?
ii. Preferisci [attività A o attività B]?
iii. Quali attività non ti piacciono molto?
iv. Perché fai [attività]
e) Perché vieni qui?
f) Quali sono i tuoi obbiettivi per la frequenza qui?
g) Cosa vuoi [quella organizzazione] fare?
i. Quella organizazione fa così?
h) Come è [quella organizzazione] simile a [questa]?
i) Come è diversa?
398
Youth with Autism – Group Interview
1) Your motivations for participating
a) Is this organization for people with Asperger’s/autism/ASD?
b) What is Asperger’s/autism/ASD?
c) What does it mean to you?
i. Does it cause you problems? Describe them.
ii. What do you fear most about it?
iii. What are the implications of a diagnosis? Does it give you
something: services, help, opportunities?
iv. Do you have another term to describe it?
v. What behaviors to attribute to it?
d) How does Asperger’s/autism/ASD work?
i. How does it affect you?
1. What are the causes?
2. When did it start? Why did it start at this time?
3. What happens in the body to cause it?
4. Is there something that happened in your family that caused
it? At school or work? In your social life? In what way did
this cause it?
5. Is it related to specific events in your life? What events?
How are they linked?
e) How do you describe Asperger’s/autism/ASD to other people?
i. Classmates or co-workers
ii. Strangers
iii. Loved ones
iv. Does someone in your family have similar problems (or another
word pulled from the response, as appropriate)? How are they the
same or different from yours?
v. Does a friend or coworker have similar problems? How are they
the same or different from yours?
vi. Have you heard about someone in the media who has these
problems? How are your problems the same or different from
someone you’ve heard about?
f) How did you learn about this organization?
g) Did you decide to come here, or another person decided? Who?
h) For what reasons do you come here?
2) This organization
a) What are the usual activities here?
b) What else have you done?
c) What are your favorite activities?
d) What activities do you not like very much?
3) Goals
a) What are the goals [of these activities]?
b) What do you want this organization to do? What are your goals for
participating?
399
c) Does it do these things?
4) Changes
a) Do you have any problems with this organization? Describe them.
b) What would you change?
5) Are there other topics you want to discuss?
400
Appendix 2 – Codebooks
Table 16: Emergent Codebook
Code
Definition
1. What is Autism?
Statements regarding the definition of
autism, characteristics of a person with
autism, and ways of identifying
(diagnosing) autism. Includes
comparisons and contrasts with other
disabilities. Includes tests used to
diagnosis it.
2. What Causes Autism
Statements regarding the definition of
autism
3. Treatment and Management
Statements regarding different ways of
treating, managing, and intervening with
autism. Includes specific intervention
programs, descriptions of specific
programs, descriptions of rights and
services, descriptions of professions and
practitioners.
4. Life Cycle
Comments on aging, life stages, age- or
stage- related milestones. Comparisons
and contrasts between different age
groups. Statements on the nature of
bambini, ragazzini, ragazzi, adolescenit,
adulti, anziani, ecc. Comments related to
changes in availability or execution of
services due to child’s age or level of
schooling.
5. Vergogna
Instances of this word using the stem
vergogn*
Subcode
Definition
Contesting the DX
Of an autism spectrum disorder, not
epilepsy, intellectual disability, or
others.
“Autismo Puro”
References to “pure,” “true,” or
“classic” autism
Wing-Gould Triad
References to the class “triad of
impairments” used in the DSM-IV
Specific Characteristics Referenced
Specific Causes Referenced
Specific Treatment or Intervention
Referenced
School
Discussions of schooling experience,
teachers, administrators, diplomas,
etc.
401
6. Non Accetta la Diagnosi
Any reference to someone non
“accettare” someone else’s autism. Must
use the word “accettare.” The direct
object could be the diagnosi, autismo,
che il figlio ha un problema, or anything
of that nature.
7. Criticism
Whenever someone makes a negative
Professional on Professional
comment about someone else. Coded
Professionals criticizing other
separately 1. For cross-referencing 2. So
professionals, organizations,
I can be aware of the most sensitive
approaches to autism, or the state.
information. This code may be abolished Professional on Parent
if it is found unuseful. However, I
Professionals criticizing parents,
anticipate it being useful in the final
including the part afterwards where
chapters to not only compare and
they say they understand. Includes
contrast respondent groups, but put them
descriptions of families “non
in conversation with each other
accettare”
Professional on Client
Professionals truly criticizing
clients. Not just describing
difficulties the clients have but
legitimately complaining about
something the client does that
bothers them personally.
Parent on Professional
Parent on Parent
Parent on Client
Client on Professional
Client on Parent
Client on Client
8. Complements
The inverse of Criticism.
Professional on Professional
Limited to genuine compliments, not
just positive evaluations of methods.
Things like sono in grado, capace, in
gamba, gentilissime, ecc. Also
talking about how education
integration is the best law in the
world.
Professional on Parent
Includes 1. Genuine compliments
about parents being capace, ecc., 2.
Sympathetic statements about
parents working very hard, trying
hard, having a lot to deal with.
402
Professional on Client
Clients are bravi, gentili, solari, what
I learned from them.
Parent on Professional
Parent on Parent
Parent on Client
Client on Professional
Client on Parent
Client on Client
9. Gender and Sexuality
Statements about the relationship
between gender and autism, the
gendered experience of autism, gender
differences, or sexuality
10. Youth Relationships
Statements about the affective or
friendship relationships of youth with
autism. Excluding siblings for now
because it isn’t relevant.
11. Services
Particular section dealing with responses
to the questions about services.
12. Child’s Experience
Statements parents made about their
child’s experiences to be used in
Chapter 6. Includes statements about
things child says or expresses. Includes
direct and indirect dialogue. Does not
include observation of actions unless
they are linked to saying or expressing
something.
13. Serenità/Tranquillità
Instances of these words using the stems
seren* and tranquil*
14. Asperger
Instances of this word, including also
“Aspie”
Youth-Youth
Youth-Staff
Heard about
From whom? How?
Goals for Participation
Parents’ or youths’ stated goals or
motivations for having their child
participate in this service
403
Table 17: a priori Codebook
Code: Definition
Molecularization: “First,
molecularization: The “style of thought”
of contemporary biomedicine envisages
life at the molecular level, as a set of
intelligible vital mechanisms among
molecular entities that can be identified,
isolated, manipulated, mobilized,
recombined, in new practices of
intervention, which are no longer
constrained by the apparent normativity of
a natural vital order” (Rose 2007: 5-6).
Somatic Expertise:
“These developments are giving rise to
new ways of governing human conduct,
and the rise of multiple subprofessions
that claim expertise and exercise their
diverse powers in the management
Subcode: Definition
Geneticization: Focus on the genetics of
a disease. “Today’s emphasis on genetic
causes and genetic understanding seems to
be part of a more expansive process I have
called ‘geneticization’ (and have begun to
describe elsewhere). Those in charge of
health-related naming and helping
activities increasingly now reduce
differences between individuals to their
DNA codes, define most disorders and
behaviors, as well as physiological
variations, as at least in part genetic in
origin, advocate the adoption of
interventions that employ genetic
technologies to manage problems of
health and, in general, (incorrectly) equate
human biology (humanity?) with human
genetics, implying that the latter acts
along to make each of us the organism she
or he is.” (Lippman 1991:85).
“Packaging human health problems – as
well as a whole range of human variability
– as ‘genetic’ blurs the distinctions
between disease and illness, disability and
handicap.” (Lippman 1991:87)
Neurochemical/Neurostructural Selves:
Focus on neurological molecular
components (neurons, neurochemicals) of
a disease
Microbiologization: “Biopolitics, then, is
joined by microbiopolitics: the creation of
categories of microscopic biological
agents; the anthropocentric evaluation of
such agents; and the elaboration of
appropriate human behaviors vis-`a-vis
microorganisms engaged in infection,
inoculation, and digestion” (Paxson 2008)
Somatic Individuality: “beings whose
individuality is, in part at least, grounded
within . . . [their] fleshy, corporeal
existence, and who experience, articulate
and judge, and act upon . . . [themselves]
404
of particular aspects of our somatic
existence—geneticists specializing in
particular classes of disorder working in
alliance with groups of patients
and families, specialists in reproductive
medicine with their public or private
clinics and devoted clientele, stem cell
therapists whose work becomes known
across the world via the Internet and who
become the focus of pilgrimages of hope
for cures for everything from spinal cord
injuries to Alzheimer’s disease. Around
these experts of the soma cluster a whole
variety of new pastoral experts—genetic
counselors are perhaps the best
exemplars—whose role is to advise and
guide, to care and support, individuals and
families as they negotiate their way
through the personal, medical, and ethical
dilemmas that they face” and also the field
of bioethics (Rose 2007:6)
in part in the language of biomedicine”
(Rose 2007:26)
Patient Literacy: A form of “lay
expertise” in which patients or parents
read up on autism after receiving the
diagnosis.
Experiential Expertise: A form of “lay
expertise” (term credited to Gibbon and
Novas 2008:8 as it relates to biosociality,
also). “We argue that this range of
understanding constitutes lay expertise
about the new genetics. People are all
experts about their own lives. And as
social actors we engage with a range of
other actors and institutions and therefore
develop a unique set of knowledge from
which to judge new experiences.
Expertise is therefore not solely the
province of professionals, but lay people
have valuable knowledge and
understanding of the social world which
equips them to discuss the new genetics in
a sophisticated and reflexive manner.”
(Kerr et al 1998)
“If the [Disabled People’s Movement]
valorizes the experiential knowledge of
disabled people, biosocial associations are
more likely to embrace a more complex
epistemological position that has been
described as a ‘coalition or hybridisation
of experiential knowledge, lay expertise
and counter-expertise’ (Hess 2004, 697)”
(Hughes 2009)
“Experiential knowledge is truth learned
from personal experience with a
phoenomen [….] Besides experiential
knowledge, there is experiential expertise
[…] which refers to competence or skill in
handling or resolving a problem through
the use of one’s own experience [….]
Thus experiential expertise can serve as a
basis for leadership, a higher status in the
group, and a source of authority for
decision making.” (Borkman 1976)
Responsibilization: “patients are
increasingly urged to become active and
405
responsible consumers of medical services
and products (Rose 2007:4)” Situations in
which patients are actively responsible for
their own care either via consumer
decisions (shopping for a doctor/service)
or technologies of the self (structuring
their own home, creating agendas,
becoming paraprofessionals, working on
at home treatments such as Doman
Delacato). Patients have the onus of
responsibility and decision-making.
Pastoral Keepers: See quote to the left.
Also, “Such groups will have medical
specialists, laboratories, narratives,
traditions, and a heavy panoply of pastoral
keepers to help them experience, share,
intervene, and “understand” their fate”
(Rabinow 1999:413)
Pilgrimages of Hope: See quote to the
left.
Individual Identities
Social Model of Disability: Autism is a
mismatch between person and
environment, not an inherent impairment
Autism as a Way of Being (Difference
not Deficit): Autism is not an appendage.
Rejection of the disease model. Can be
positively or negatively valued, point is
that it is something someone is, not has.
Language that describes autism as a
“diversity”
Autism as identity: Mostly limited to
people with ASD. Discourses in which “I
am an Autie/Aspie” or “I do X because I
have autism.” Parents/professionals may
say “I don’t do X because I don’t have
autism”
The Family with Autism: The moral
career of a parent of a child with autism.
Identification as an autism parent in some
way. Identification with a (physical or
imagined) group of autism parents. I, like
all parents... or I, like these other
parents… Distinct from biosocial group
formation, but clearly linked.
406
Autism Pride: In which autism is
described as something that brings
positive characteristics such as honesty,
computer skills, etc. Ways in which
autism is different, not deficit.
Autism as normal – We Are All The
Same: Ways in which we are all autistic,
or ways in which autistic behaviors
parallel “normotipico” behaviors
Cerebral Subjectivation: “people define
themselves as autistics (rather than as
having autism) because their brains are
‘wired’ in an atypical way [….] The
neuroscientistic [sic] language, often
imprecise, serves to construe autism as a
positive attribute, and to stress the natural
difference from non-autistic (often called
neurotypcal) experience and identity”
(Ortega 2009:427)
Collective Identities and Biosocialities
Social Group Formation: Social
organization around disability/illness
identities
Activism/Identity Politics: Term taken
from Sahra and Gibbon 2008:8. Lobbying
for the disease (both in terms of directing
research and in receiving state resources).
Also fundraising and creating own
services (grassroots).
Informational Biocitizenship: “The
forms of citizenship entailed here often
involve quite specialized scientific and
medical knowledge of one's condition”
(Rose 2007:135). I use it to mean the role
of collectivities and parent networking in
sharing information and the mobilization
of information as part of a group.
Rights Biocitizenship: ““campaigning for
better treatment, ending stigma, getting
access to services, and the like”
Digital Biocitizenship: participation in
electronic communities around the
condition. Finding information, resources,
and connections online.
407
Appendix 3 – Italian Block Quotes
Chapter 4
p. 158
Pervasivo è qualcosa che ti cambia la vita. È un ragazzo che è incapace di avere
relazioni sociali, non è generalizzato, come dire “eh beh, è un po’ mattacchione
perché ogni tanto fa così, però… si fa amici in qualsiasi posto vada”, per
esempio. No, se è pervasivo, ti viene a incidere, a danneggiare in qualche cosa.
Quindi pervasivo è più pesante.
Parliamo spesso di disturbo pervasivo dello sviluppo, però quello è anche molto
ampio, no? [...] quello che noto molto spesso è che in medicina non facilmente
danno subito una diagnosi precisa, preferiscono aspettare [....] visto che abbiamo
tanti bambini che magari diamo una diagnosi di autismo atipico o un disturbo
pervasivo dello sviluppo non altrimenti specificato, quindi diciamo per le forme
‘borderline’ di autismo, che spesso ai genitori (danno) la falsa idea che sono
bambini che possono guarire piuttosto che non sono così gravi.
Sono simili, il disturbo generalizzato dello sviluppo è l’autismo però per i genitori
dire “autismo” è proprio un’etichetta che magari non c'è neanche conoscienza,
quindi si immaginano loro figlio isolato, incapace, come se non... se non fosse
capace di provare emozioni. Il disturbo generalizzato dello sviluppo si parla
ancora dello sviluppo, quindi dà una sensazione di cambiamento, di crescita, e, e
quindi la vedono quasi meno come una condanna.
p. 163
Um, alcuni tra gli intellettuali che si occupano dell'asperger dicono anche che le
persone asperger non hanno l'empatia, ma questo molti asperger negano, dicono
semplicemente che loro modo di empatizzare è diverso da quello che chi si
occupa di analizzare loro vorrebbe vedere. Quindi con loro che studiano
Asperger magari non sono capaci di comprendere loro modo di empatizzare.
Una cosa interessante che scopri è che anche con la persona con autismo [è
possible creare una relazione, nonostante] che prima di lavorarci insieme sai che
sono persone che fanno fatica entrare in relazione. In realtà usano canali
privilegati, comunque utilizzano canali diversi [rispetto a] quelli che si usano
quella maggior parte delle persone, [ma nonostante questo si può arrivare a]
stare in relazione anche molte significative. Non è che non possono entrare in
relazione, è che usano canali diversi.
p. 164
Anthropologist: Ci sono altre caratteristiche che sono importanti?
408
Respondent: Uhm… Eh, direi queste. Forse non per tutti… l’esistenza di un
mondo parallelo, cioè, come se vivessero inseriti in una realtà diversa da quella
che percepiamo noi.
L'autismo è una compromissione nella relazione, nella comunicazione, nell'area
della immaginazione. E questa è la definizione tecnica. Se poi vogliamo vedere
cos'è una persona autistica, una persona autistica è una persona che vive con le
parametri diversi dai nostri.
p. 166
La persona con autismo ha un ritardo mentale, mentre la persona Asperger non
ha questo ritardo mentale, ma un quoziente intelletivo nella norma. Comunque,
l’Asperger ha grosse difficoltà sempre nella triade, cioè nella comunicazione,
nella realzione, e negli interessi che chiaramente, avendo un quoziente intellettivo
nella norma, è molto più evidente, in un asperger poter verificare gli interessi
ristretti e stereotipati, cosa che per una persona con autismo con un ritardo
mentale grave, è più difficile magari, poter osservare.
p. 168
Mi veniva più facile [la differenza] fra autismo ad alto funzionamento e Asperger.
[....] La differenza in realtà tra le due malattie, cioè, le due diciture più che le due
malattie, è che negli asperger il linguaggio compare da subito normale con l'età
[...] mentre ad alto funzionamento può comparire anche un po' dopo, magari il
linguaggio può arrivare anche intorno ai 4, 5 anni.
p. 169
Alcuni pensano che l’asperger è diversa dall’autismo, [....] Di fatto per [la mia]
esperienza le caratteristiche distintive sono il ritardo di linguaggio, - nel senso
che se un bambino ha nella sua storia da piccolo avuto una storia di ritardo
linguistico quindi – poi magari arriva a parlare e anche molto molto bene, e
arriva a livelli cognitivi buoni, ma era partito da una situazione di ritardo, allora
parliamo di autismo ad alto funzionamento. Se invece il bambino ha una storia di
sviluppo completamente nella norma, anzi, precoce nell’acquisizione, [...] in quel
caso [sarà] più sindrome di asperger. Qualcuno dice anche che nella sindrome di
asperger un aspetto caratteristico è la goffagine [...] però questo non è un
elemento così distinttivo nel senso anche che dei ragazzi che hanno un quadro di
autismo ad alto funzionamento possono esserlo [....] Il quoziente può essere
comunque alto in entrambi casi. In genere, però, il quoziente delle persone con la
sindrome di Asperger prevede che il quoziente verbale, quindi le abilità verbali,
siano più alte del quoziente si dice di “performance.” Mentre nei soggetti con
autismo ad alto funzionamento è contrario.
409
La difficoltà per le persone che hanno fondato proprio associazioni sull’Asperger,
[è] la paura che si possa, poi, in un unico calderone, perdere l’aspetto distintivo
dell’alto funzionamento e dell’Asperger.
Capisco che adesso si è deciso di eliminar[e la sindrome di Asperger] dal nuovo
manuale diagnostico, anche se è un dolore enorme per le persone che hanno una
diagnosi e che si riconoscono come “Aspie.” E quindi, questo dal punto di vista
etico io trovo veramente una difficoltà notevole.
p. 175
Nonostante le linee guida [...] loro ti dicono “Non è giusto, perché non è
sufficiente, perché le persone con l’autismo hanno una dimensione emotiva, una
dimensione simbolica”, dico: “ok, anch’io ci lavoro – anche il cognitivocomportamentale non significa che non lavori sulle emozioni, altro! Però loro te
lo mettono solo in termini di comportamento di… - poi tutto quello che leggevo
anche ieri era: non tutto può essere validato scientificamente, perché le persone
hanno delle dimensioni individuali, ma certo! Le conosciamo anche noi! Però
ancora c’è davvero questa diatriba e molti – si riacc[endendo], non è assopita
per niente, si sta riaccendendo, secondo me, e riacutizzando.
p. 176
Ogni tipo di intervento ha il loro pro e il loro contro, tante che noi usiamo aspetti
del comportamentismo classico, aspetti di DIR, aspetti delle tradizioni
psicodinamiche, quindi usiamo aspetti tecniche comportamentisti che però
appunto in questo contesto con continua attenzione agli aspetti della soggetività
che è di derivazione dinamica.
p. 178
È un approccio ovviamente di tipo relazionale, che mette in relazione il bambino
prima di tutto… con - si cerca di farlo relazionare col genitore, poi con
l’ambiente, con le altre persone, con l’estraneo, che sono… che è il terapista e
con il mondo che lo circonda… attraverso un processo di progressiva regolazione,
autoregolazione, no? E di progressiva consapevolezza di quello – di attenzione su
quello che – sia sull’ambiente, ma sulla situazione, cioè, una capacità di leggere
l’ambiente e la prevedibilità della situazione [partendo dagli] stimoli. Ma questo
vuol dire riuscire a portare la sua attenzione su ciò che lo sta circondando… in
quel momento. Quello che noi vediamo sono dei bambini che migliorano
tantissimo dal punto di vista proprio delle competenze di aggancio, sguardo, di
risposta al nome, di tenuta, di condivisione dell’attenzione sulle cose.
Io credo che la vostra sia più “speech therapy” e “speech” è il linguaggio
parlato, e invece la logopedia si occupa di comunicazione e di linguaggio, quindi
già nel nostro lavoro la comunicazione è già prevista, in ogni forma, cioè: visiva,
410
verbale, gestuale, qualsiasi. Ci occupiamo di comunicazione.
p. 179
Se ti faccio un test logopedico standard, in cui ti chiedo di ripetere o di nominare
delle cose e tu non hai il linguaggio, è chiaro che… uhm.. il mio test risulterà…
non, non attendibile. Però logopedia vuol dire comunicazione, e la comunicazione
la si può valutare, la si può insegnare anche in altri… in altre modalità.
p. 180
Allora, per quella che è la mia esperienza, devo dire che per quanto riguarda
l’integrazione di persone che per esempio hanno un ritardo dell’apprendimento,
piuttosto che un deficit sensoriale, io ho vissuto delle buone esperienze, dove
l’integrazione in effetti ha funzionato, e dove quindi in effetti ci sono anche stati
appunto dei buoni risultati grazie a questo. Invece, per quanto riguarda il
disturbo dello spettro autistico, devo dire che intanto, sono arrivate qua tante
persone che nella scuola tra virgolette “normale” hanno vissuto anche dei
momenti di frustrazione e dei momenti proprio di difficoltà, ma proprio perché la
scuola non è attrezzata, non ha probabilmente ancora le conoscenze e le
competenze per poter fare un discorso di integrazione all’interno della scuola. E
poi ho visto – però qua, nel gruppo soltanto di ragazzi autistici – proprio perché
ci sono le stesse modalità di comunicare, le stesse caratteristiche, lo stesso modo
di affrontare determinate situazioni, che i ragazzi hanno vissuto in maniera
comunque più serena, perché hanno trovato comunque un ambiente più adatto a
loro, e dove sono nate anche delle amicizie, perché comunque si sono trovati
ragazzi con lo stesso tipo di interesse [....] Perché dove invece ho visto situazioni
di scuola superiore, dove viene richiesto anche maggiormente l’aspetto così dello
stare in classe, dell’ascoltare la lezione, del fare la verifica, del fare
l’interrogazione, dove vengono fatte appunto tutta una serie di richieste che per i
nostri ragazzi sono delle richieste non alte – sono troppo alte, ma non dal punto
di vista della competenza e della capacità cognitiva, ma proprio alte nel reggere
l’ansia, nel reggere lo stress, è molto faticoso questo per loro. Mentre invece, una
situazione più a loro misura sicuramente li ha fatti sentire più a loro agio.
p. 181
Il problema è che attualmente stiamo applicando in italia I programmi terapeutici
secondo me tradizionalmente vecchi, quindi psicomotricità e la logopedia che le
linee guide per autismo e le ricerche internazionali hanno detto che non sono
adeguati per l'autismo perché hanno bisogno di interventi psicoeducativi e
strutturati, intensivi ma non ossessivi.
p. 182
Cognitivo comportamentale quindi psicoeducativo quindi l’idea è concentrare
411
l’attenzione su insegnare alle persone con autismo tutte quelle abilità che sono
utili per la vita quotidiana, per la loro vita, per potersi per avere una vita diciamo
di successo nel senso realizzata, dignitosa, felice, e di sostenere in questo anche le
famiglie e poi le persone che lavorano con loro. Io faccio molto rifiermento alla
filosofia della divisione di TEACCH.
p. 183
Il modo migliore per aiutare le persone con Sindrome di Asperger è aiutarle dal
punto di vista educativo. Cioè, correggere alcuni loro comportamenti, aiutare ad
imparare degli strumenti nuovi che loro non possiedono, per relazionarsi meglio
con gli altri, per organizzare meglio la propria vita, per gestire le situazioni [....]
Ci sono diverse modalità di approccio. Ma tutti quanti dovrebbero basarsi
proprio sull’aiuto di tipo educativo e quindi aiutare la persona a comprendere la
realtà, decodifare la realtà per lui, e dare degli strumenti migliori per interagire e
capire quali sono le proprio lacune e migliorarle.
Il cognitivo-comportamentale è un attimino meno... allora, l'approccio
comportamentale di una volta è un addestramento, è considerato come un
addestramento. Quindi tu impari per riflesso cioè non perché hai imparato
veramente, no, ma perché sei stato condizionato ad imparare. E quindi. Però in
altre situazioni tu questa cosa non saresti in grado di farla, no? Invece il
cognitivo-comportamentale tu lavori molto invece sul... uh... più sul fargli
imparare che quello serve per fare questo questo questo e quest'altro, non
soltanto una cosa. Cioè, quello che stai imparando questo non è perché io ti sto
addestrando a fare questa cosa come riflesso di un comportamento che metti, no?
perché c'era la cosa, vabbé, io ti do la caramella e tu mi rispondi, se io non ti do
la caramella, tu non mi rispondi, no? Questa. Ma che io ti dico, uh, che devi fare,
mi devi rispondere non tanto perché ti do la caramella ma perché ti serve a fare
tante altre cose.
p. 184
Sempre partendo dal presupposto che per me psicoeducativo vuol dire utilizzare
tutte le strategie e le conoscenze che abbiamo ehm… a seconda della persona per
sviluppare le sue abilità. Per cui, come ti dicevo l’altra volta, ABA, TEACCH,
Denver model, ehm… non so, l’CAA, la Comunicazione Aumentativa Alternativa,
il FloorTime, il Sonrise, che è quello inglese… cioè, ce ne sono mille di cose. [....]
Quindi, l’approccio è sempre quello, appunto, comportamentale, neocomportamentista, non lo so, chiamalo come vuoi, con quelle tecniche lì.
p. 185
[Cerchiamo] di integrare le tecniche comportamentistiche tradizionali e nelle loro
evoluzioni anche più recenti con l’attenzione costante agli aspetti delle
motivazioni, gli effetti, e la persona. Perché non può esserci un buon lavoro
412
riabilitativo e non si può garantire una buona qualità di vita alle persone di cui ci
occupiamo se le trattiamo solo come una somma di comportamenti da correggere
senza interrogarci su cosa c’è sotto quel comportamento, quali sono, per esempio
il disagio o la sofferenza che un comportamento problema o problematico può
esprimersi [....]Il problema è che si tratta di integrare tutte le tecniche
comportamentiste con una costante attenzione a degli aspetti della soggettività,
degli affetti, e delle emozioni della persona. Non siamo degli addestratori di cani,
giusto? Abbiamo che fare comunque con le persone.
p. 186
Allora, la filosofia è … in parte comportamentista, in parte umanista. Sono [così],
perché comunque siamo italiani (ridendo) e secondo me molto umanista. Nel
senso che ci piace che queste persone, oltre che lavorare sul comportamento,
sull'abilità, lavori, uh, abbiano anche delle opportunità di sperimentare, delle
esperienze che sembrano aver poco valore dal punto di vista dell'apprendimento,
ma che hanno un grosso valore umano, come possono essere i compleanni, le
feste di compleanno, le vacanze [....] Noi siamo, sì, secondo me siamo molto
umanisti secondo me come filosofia. Che secondo me è anche una caratteristica
molto italiana, secondo me, sì.
Lo strumento più importante è la relazione, capire [...] che [è] impossibile che
quel bimbo non abbia nulla di dirci e se non ha nulla da dirci siamo noi che non
riusciamo a leggere. Questo per cui la relazione. Chiar[amente in] un contesto
accogliente e un contesto attento. Questo [è] fondamentale
p. 187
Non è così e se tu non entri in relazione, se tu non hai una relazione con chi hai di
fronte, non insegni mai niente. Mentre l'idea è che noi invece non ha... non
entriamo in relazione con loro. Facciamo le maestrine, facciamo gli
addrestratori, senza avere (ridendo) senza tener conto di tutta la parte invece
relazionale-affettiva. Non può essere. Perché non può essere. Perché soltanto il
fatto di stare qui a parlare con te, comunque c'è qualcosa, no? Se io comunque
non entro in relazione con il ragazzo, il ragazzo mettelo far vedere, non ti
risponde neanche. Infatti ci sono persone, ci sono dei ragazzi che con alcuni di
noi non funzionano e con altri sì, e viceversa. Questa comunque è una cosa che è
difficle da far capire.
Infatti io molto spesso mi sono trovata non molto d’accordo con dei colleghi
perché è ovvio che devi avere una buona relazione con il paziente. Ma questa, lo
devi e (claps) basta. Ma però devi costruire al livello di realtà delle cose, no? E
quindi devi per forza fare anche degli esercizi o comunque avere un metodo. E
questo mi dice “Non importa. Tu hai un metodo. Io un diverso metodo.”
Il nostro approccio è più – come dire – mi sembra più ecologico, cioè… come…
413
che il linguaggio debba nascere nel suo contesto naturale e non… per… Poi
invece sul trattamento ABA in sé, come trattamento complessivo trovo che è molto
efficace nelle abilità, nelle autonomie del bambino, questo è efficacissimo. Però
sul linguaggio bisogna stare attenti, più attenti.
p. 188
Allora, [la nostra filosofia è la] creazione di un contesto che noi abbiamo
chiamato ecologico, ma che vuol dire, ecologico non vuol dire “in compagna.”
Vuol dire armonica, vuol dire che attività, interventi, frequentazione di persone,
tutta dev’essere armonizzata e ogni intervento che si fa con le persone con
autismo dev’essere contestualizzata, eh? Non si propone una cosa che non c’entra
niente in quel momento lì o in quell’ambiente. Questa è la premessa. Dopodiché,
ogni intervento, attività di lavoro che viene proposta, deve essere significativo e
questo significato deve essere spiegato, con le modalità adatte a quella persona,
in modo che possa essere compreso. Inoltre, riteniamo che sia molto importante
curare la soggettività della persona con autismo e quindi cercare di ampliare al
massimo gli strumenti di comunciazione, ognuno a suo livello, e capire qual è la
loro motivazione intrinsica perché questa è qualcosa che migliora la qualità della
vita, anche per noi. [....] Infine... il fare insieme. Come ha detto Meltzoff, la
famosa frase “from shared actions to shared minds.” Cioè quindi facendo
insieme, si impara a stare insieme e forse anche si inizia a condividere il pensiero.
La psicomotricità deriva culturalmente della psichiatria e dell’educazione
motoria della danza, della ritmica, quindi del movimento spontaneo. [....] Mentre
per noi il movimento è il mezzo per l’espressione, per la comunicazione e per
l’apprendimento. Non è un fine.
p. 189
Allora, diciamo che con un bambino autistico, io essendo psicomotricista, ho un
approccio da psicomotricista, col bambino autistico. Il fatto che ci sia l’animale,
ovviamente, mi fa modificare alcuni aspetti… però non mi cambia radicalmente il
mio modo di approcciarmi. Per cui quando un bambino entra in stanza, io vedo
lui a cosa si rivolge, a cosa è interessato, cosa attira la sua attenzione. Perché
specialmente con un bambino autistico, devi entrare un pochino nel loro mondo di
interessi. Poi, cerchi di aprirli… al nostro mondo.
p. 193
Sì, più o meno penso che siano questi due, cioè l'approccio TEACCH e
l'approccio dinamico-relazionale, quello sicuramente. Tutto in una cornice molto
flessibile, secondo me. [Per] flessibile intendo che non ci si (attiene) al manuale
TEACCH in maniera così rigida ma si cerca anche poi di cogliere anche
sfumature al di fuori dalla filosofia TEACCH, insomma, perché non esiste solo
TEACCH.
414
p. 194
L'approccio usato è l'approccio psicocognitivo che è molto ampio e comunque
abbiamo scelto, come dire, di considerare gli approcci classici quindi il
TEACCH, ABA, soltanto per le parti che ci sembravano funzionali, quindi
all'interno dell'approccio psicoeducativo abbiamo scelto soltanto quelle cose che
ci sembravano utili per il nostro tipo di intervento, che è così particolare perché
appunto noi non abbiamo una struttura. [Non ce l’abbiamo]. E quindi, come dire,
il nostro lavoro è un lavoro itinerante. Quindi abbiamo considerato sicuramente
la strutturazione del tempo, dell'ambiente, delle attività, quindi la pulizia del
contesto che è molto importante, la consequenzialità delle azioni. Un’altra cosa
molto importante, e... e questo poi ci aiuta chiaramente a dare loro un mondo
prevedibile, conosciuto.
Qui in italia, c’è, si tende un po’ a sposare un approccio tipo “o fai TEACCH, o
fai l’ABA, o fai...” no? È tutto un po’, non so dire, categorizzata, posso dire così?
Invece ho un approccio, una formazione molto più ampia per cui cerco di
prendere tutti gli approcci che ho conosciuto quello che serve per questo specifico
bambino. Quindi delle strategie che derivano dal’ABA, strategie che derivano
dell TEACCH, strategie che derivano anche dal DIR; ecc.
Volevamo dare segnali alle famiglie perché spesso ci dicevano, (mimicking,
angry), “eh, ma voi fate il TEACCH.” “Sì, va bene, anche.” “ma nel TEACCH ci
sono solo le scatolette.” “No, non è solo questo.” “Ma il TEACCH vuol dire che
siete rigidi.” “No, non è solo questo. Qui entra in gioco l'educazione strutturata, I
comportamenti e l'educazione-comportamento, cioè ci sono altre cose. Certo è
tutto inserito nella filosofia TEACCH.” “Eh, no, ma è il metodo TEACCH.” “No,
non è un metodo, è una filosofia, un programma, è diversa la cosa.”
p. 195
Allora, a me piace – nonostante sia un metodo, mi sono ritrovata nel metodo
Floortime, ma perché [...] il metodo Floortime, come tutti i metodi, ha delle
regole precise, per cui tu devi – hai presente? – devi stare venti minuti con tuo
figlio a casa, poi, 10 minuti no, poi altri 20 minuti – tipo otto volte in una
giornata dedicare 20 minuti di attività e di interazione con il bambino. All’inizio,
sulla carta, a me sembrava troppo rigida come cosa. Poi ho visto dei filmati di
bambini… quando ci sono dei genitori adeguati in effetti è molto più utile. Allora
io magari non farei 20 minuti otto volte al giorno, cioè mi sembra un po’ rigido
questo. Però posso capire che è, come dire, un incentivo al genitore a dedicare dei
momenti al bambino, di gioco e di attività e di interazione per aprire un po’,
appunto, l’interesse, la gamma di interesse del bambino. Allora, probabilmente,
viene data come una medicina, una ricetta, perché alla famiglia serve avere
un’indicazione così precisa. Però, effettivamente… diciamo, le indicazione su
come approcciarsi e su che tipo di attività fare sono molto utili. Allora, questo mi
415
piace, poi ti dico, io ho proprio sposato il principio di partire dal loro interesse,
che è… è scritto subito nel Floortime, proprio quello, secondo me è la carta
vincente.
Se uno dice, ”io faccio TEACCH, solo TEACCH,” è come dire leggo solo questo,
allora questo diventa, però questo, secondo me, non solo per il TEACCH. Nella
nostra vita questo vale un po’ per tutto. Allora il rischio a volte è dialogare
sempre solo tra le stesse persone mentre è importante secondo me aprirsi anche a
altri tipi di esperienze, integrare [....] Secondo me lo svantaggio può essere
rimanere [...] poco flessibile, insomma. Già che i nostri ragazzini sono poco
flessibili! (ridendo).
Secondo me, ad esempio, sia L'ABA che TEACCH di base hanno delle buon idee.
Anch'io ogni tanto utilizzo della loro tecnica, però in modo poco rigido, cioè, il
rischio di questa tecnica è creare una sorta di rigidità, quindi insegnare in
sequenza va bene, però rischio che il ragazzo si irrigidisca, quindi se non fai la
sequenza giusta, non va bene. È l'unica cosa che non condivido molto con questi
metodi. Cioè, condivido il pensiero, ti faccio un esempio. Um, loro per insegnarti
ad apparecchiare, ad esempio mettono un rettangolo verde, un cerchio giallo e un
rettangolino rosso. Il ragazzo deve mettere la tavaglietta sopra il rettangolo
verde, i tovaglioli sul rettangolo rosso, ed il piatto sopra il cerchio giallo. Quindi
l'idea di per sé è funzionale, però io lo vedo molto poco naturale. Noi, io, per
insegnare ai miei ragazzi ad apparecchiare, utilizzo lo stesso metodo ma in modo
più naturale. Per cui ad esempio io lo metto il tovagliolo e la tavoletta, il ragazzo
sa che deve mettere le posate sopra tovalgiolo e deve mettere il piatto sopra la
tavoletta. Quindi la stessa tenica, però vista in modo meno rigido e più naturale.
p. 196
Io credo che poi effettivamente una buona alimentazione aiuti tutti, però è una
dieta rigida, togliendo tutta una seria di alimenti, poi rischia di provocare altri
problemi di mancanza di alcuni principi, ecco, quindi questi sono effettivamente
interventi un po' lontani dal nostro pensiero.
p. 197
Floortime, proprio quello, secondo me è la carta vincente. Gli altri, appunto,
Delacato, il TEACCH, sono troppo rigidi. Però con alcuni bambini… funzionano.
Io partirei però, con i bambini piccoli, da un modello più flessibile, e a una certa
età partirei con qualcosa di più rigido nel momento in cui il bambino ha bisogno
di essere incanalato. Ma da piccoli [...] sono per destrutturare, aprire, rendere
flessibili, perché questa è la nostra realtà. Se no io li irrigidisco, quando sono già
rigidi. Voglio prima provare invece ad ammorbidirli.
p. 198
416
Poi ci sono invece magari esperienze piacevoli, utili e buone, per i bambini, ma
che vengono vendute come terapie, no? Non so… pet therapy, music therapy,
horse therapy… che sono cose buone! …Ma non si sa ancora quanto è terapia.
Esperienze piacevoli, che i bambini fanno volentieri, ma non sappiamo ancora se
possono essere pensate come un vero e proprio intervento… terapeutico.
Poi ci possono essere altre cose, non so, l’ippoterapia o delfinoterapia, non è che
è inutile, però non è specifica per l’autismo. Allora se a questo bambino piace
l’acqua, piacciono gli animali e poi sia un ambiente stimolante e motivante per
lui, e che dentro di quel contesto ovviamente lui può acquisire abilità
sociocomunicative, ben venga. Ma non può fare solo quello. Cioè quello non è un
intervento specifico sull’autismo. Poi se i genitori vogliono fornirgli, insomma,
anche quel tipo di esperienza, perché no?
p. 211
Abbiamo tutto una seria di laboratori a volte [...] proprio per permettere al
ragazzo di crescere, diventare adulto, prendersi la responsabilità di alcune
incombenze che sono legate al suo benessere perché è importante che un
ragzzino, adolescente, poi diventato adulto, sia in grado di occuparsi.
p. 214
Questo è un posto che come obiettivo ha quello di consentire un graduale
distacco dalle famiglie perché, se noi parliamo di età adulta, i nostri ragazzi che
non potranno conseguire altri obiettivi che hanno i normodotati: a laurearsi,
sposarsi, eh, tranne 2, 3 casi che forse in un domani potranno vivere nel paese
qua vicino. Però la maggior parte di loro vivrà qua dentro perché sarebbero in
istituto se non ci fosse [questo servizio]. Quindi diciamo che un primo obiettivo
con la famiglia è quello di favorire il distacco.
Chapter 5
p. 222
C’erano 3 colloqui al anno con la neuropsichiatra.Ogni tanto diceva disturbo
generalizzato dello sviluppo, ma ogni volta ritardo mentale. È arrivata piano
piano la diagnosis. Sentendo la mamme nella associazione, ho reso conto che
vuole dire autismo. Ho chiesto la neuropsichiatra e ha detto di sì.
Infatti ho chiesto la neuropsichiatra, “mi scusi ma, è autistico o no?” Lei mi ha
detto “no, non è autistico perché gli autistici hanno un comportamento molto
diverso dove non miglioranno.” Così lei mi ho detto. Invece [figlio] migliora.
Questa mi ha detto.
Io penso che è una diagnosi un po’ vaga che comprende tante [...] cioè non è
417
come tipo la sindrome di down che è più... come dire? Più caratteristica. Si
capisce. Invece questa un po’... po’ generica.
p. 225
Con parole mie, l’autismo è... non è una malattia ma è un modo di essere [....]
dovremmo imparare fino da asilo che ognuno di noi è diverso dall’altro, quindi
anche gli autistici non sono da guardare come delle persone strane, ma come le
persone diverse da noi. Molto diverse da noi, chiaramente, certo. [....] Bisogna
avere rispetto, ecco. Non togliere il rispetto che ognuno di noi individui sulla
terra merita. Loro allo stesso modo hanno bisogno, hanno diritto dello stesso
rispetto.
p. 227
Io una cosa che ho insegnato a [mio figlio] che fortunatamente lui ha presso
molto bene, io gli dico sempre “ogni persona ha delle difficoltà. Ogni persona è
capace di fare una cosa, ma non è capace di farne un altra. Quindi tu non
riuscirai, per dire, in matematica come altre persone che sono bravissime, però tu
per esempio hai una memoria visiva eccezionale e quindi potrai fare qualcosa di
molto bello utilizzando per esempio queste tue capacità.”
Siamo tutti un cerchio, nel senso che in questo cerchio abbiamo tutti pregi e
difetti, ma siamo tutti un cerchio. Quindi dove c’è più di una cosa ce n’è più di
un’altra. Quindi, voglio dire, tu avrai altre caratteristiche positive che non ha
[sorella]. Quindi ognuno ha le sue specificità e le sue doti positive e ognuno ha le
sue negatività.
p. 228
Le dico: “[figlia], vedi? Tu in quelle scuole non puoi andare perché tu non
parli”, le dico. E lei comincia: “Ma-ma-ma-ma”, capito? Perché è così. E allora
le dico: “Tu vorresti andare in quella scuola?”. E lei mi dice: “No”. E… però le
dico: “Vedi, [nipotina] adesso a settembre [la nipotina] inizia la scuola
elementare”. E le dico: “Lei va dove ci sono tutti i bambini, che fanno i compiti,
disegnano, mentre tu non riesci a farlo, però quello che riesci lo puoi fare lo
stesso con la mamma e anche al centro dove tu vai.” Però io parlo, perché lei ti
ascolta.
p. 229
Allora, c’è stato un momento qualche anno fa in cui lui ha preso conoscienza. Mi
rendevo conto perché piangeva spesso, era in angoscia. Ha preso coscienza un
po’ dei suoi problemi. E allora cioè io l’ho detto nello stesso modo. Non è che mi
sono soffermata a dare spiegazione a [figlio]. Ho detto “[figlio], tu sei una
persona autistica, e quindi hai un tipo di comunicazione diverso da quello che ha
418
la mamma e la mamma voule cercare di capirti per poterti aiutare perché
altrimenti è molto difficile aiutarti.” Ecco ancora oggi. Lui, ci siamo soffermati
un attimino alla spiegazione. Dico “tu devi aiutarmi a capirti altrimenti io non ce
la faccio perché sono meno intelligente di te.”
p. 232
Lei sa un po’ sicuramente più di me quindi io mi remetto. Però io ho capito
perché leggendo ... di qua e di là io ho capito che l’autismo non è solo una
situazione, cioè non è, uh, ci sono vari tipi di autismo. Ci sono vari gradi. E [mio
figlio] non ha un autismo pesante. Ha un autismo medio. Che non so se si può
iscrivere nel Asperger oppure no. Questo non lo so.
Poi dopo mi sono un po’ distaccata perché ho cominciato a leggere “mio figlio
andrà al liceo scientifico, mio figlio andrà al liceo classico, mio figlio andrà” e
faccio “forse [figlio] non è così Asperger! (laughs) Non posso mandarlo a liceo,
no?” mi sono sentita un po’ una pesce fuori d’aqua a leggendo questi forum dove
parlano di Asperger [...][figlio] per certi tratti sarà asperger. Ma per certi no nel
senso che come posso dire, un Apserger è un ragazzo che può andare al liceo, e
mio non può andare al liceo.
p. 234
Noi qualche dubbio l’avevamo poi quando [figlia] è venuta qua, dopo i primi
tempi, ne abbiamo parlato anche con dottor [cognome], abbiamo detto “ma,
ascolti, Lei concorda con la diagnosi, ci possono essere altre?” Lui mi ha detto
“no, no, concordo, in pieno.” Ecco. E quindi questo c’è un po’... non è che ci ha
tranquilizzato, nel senso che il problema è così però per lo meno dici “ti muovi in
questa direzione, ti muovi”
p. 235
Io dico sempre loro non hanno un ritardo mentale, loro hanno una diversa
dimensione di apprendere quello che gli viene detto, quello che gli circonda, ma
non hanno un ritardo mentale perché io mi rendo conto che [figlio] è in grado di
fare, di pensare, e di mettere in atto delle strategie per arrivare a un suo obiettivo
che noi non riusciamo neanche a pensare [....] Lui è molto più furbo di noi. Ecco.
E quindi non posso pensare che una persona così abbia un ritardo mentale.
Mi hanno detto che probabilmente porta un ritardo cognitivo. Ma riusciva
combinare guai quando è vicino noi. Se porta questa grave ritardo, perché è così
sveglio, pronto di scappare, prende le cose di fratello? È furbo. [...] la cosa
strana che non riuscivamo capire per quando lo psicologo dice ritardo cognitivo,
ma è molto più sveglio che il primo filgio. [....] Non riuscivamo capire perché
questo tanto intelligente di fare guai ma non voleva parlare, non seguiva quello
che facevano gli altri. [….] Non parlava. A scuola con gli altri non disegnava,
419
non distinguiva le colore. Mi hanno detto per cui questo è ritardo.
p. 244
Un giorno la direttrice ci ha convocato e ci ha detto che a [figlio] dovevamo
somministrare dei farmaci perché altrimenti era troppo agitato, eccetera. Noi le
abbiamo detto: “No, noi di farmaci non gliene somministriamo, perché il suo
medico non ritiene opportuno dargli dei farmaci. Dovete voi cercare il modo di
intervenire sui comportamenti di (figlio).”
p. 248
Qui ci danno tanti consigli anche prattici, qua. Ad esempio ci hanno fatto vedere
anche al centro diurno. So che diciamo così utilizzato metodo TEACCH quindi
tutto fatto impostazione molto non so le schematiche, le impostazioni contro il
muro. Uh o tutto immagini visivi che per [figlia] hanno funzionato bene perché
anche tu dici le cose 10 volte, cerchi di riassuricarla su quello che viene dopo, su
quello che viene dopo ancora. Ma verbalmente la cosa non funziona. Quando
abbiamo visto sia in centro diurno che fuori dalla communità il suo panello con
la sua foto, con tutte le fasi visivi, con lo staccare questa cosa. Ci siamo detti “ma
caspita, bastava una cosa così forse per aiutare [figlia] a scuola, per aiutarci noi
a casa, a darle rimando della scansione del tempo, delle fasi, delle cose che
faceva, visivo, e non invece diciamo sempre verbalizzandole.”
Anche per noi era un aiuto perché lo psicologo è venuto a casa, ha visto come noi
ci comportavamo con [figlio], quindi ha fatto tutta una sua diagnosi anche sulla
base di qual’era il nostro atteggiamento, ci ha dato anche dell’indicazioni su
come intervenire su quei comportamenti problematici che si erano evidenziati
[....] per esempio, certe volte ci ha suggerito di ignorare cioè di non continuare a
dire “lascia stare, lascia stare” ma di ignorare il comportamento in modo tale
che prima o poi questo comportamento comunque si spegneva.
p. 251
Se poi alla fine gli strumenti ho sempre preparato io ma ho voluto tenere separata
il ruolo di medica rispetto dal roulo di mamma. Quindi preparavo tutto il
materiale e lo passavo alla neuropsichiatra infantile e poi volevo che fosse lei ad
interfacersi con gli inseganti.
p. 255
Che cosa facesse veramente nelle ore di sostegno, quello che dicevo prima:
adesso dice che l’insegnante di sostegno dormiva. Ne ha avute molte di
insegnanti di sostegno, però il ritornello è sempre quello. Allora l’ora di sostegno
era il ghetto. Cioè, il sostegno come veniva effettuato? I ragazzini venivano presi
e venivano portati nell’aula di sostegno, e lì facevano… cosa non si sa. Il
420
sostegno ritengo che sia – debba svolgersi nell’aula, insieme agli altri, perché il
sostegno serve a far sì che il portatore di handicap venga integrato nella classe, e
invece allora funzionava così.
Non sempre ci sono i... presidi o i direttori che sono sensibili a questi problemi
quindi anche lì ho dovuto fare delle belle lotte perché in prima elementare gli
avevano dato solo 6 ore di sotegno. Non aveva una class dove andare, doveva
stare in classe con gli altri qindi in pratica era un distastro.
p. 256
M: Poi è andato al elementaria. Volevo che stasse con i bambini normodotati per
imparare stare con loro. In quarto elementare, mi sono arrabbiata.
A: Perché?
M: Ha grandissima difficoltà. Ho tolto e ho mandato in una scuola speciale. (Una
scuola per bambini con problemi gli occhi)
A: come andava?
M: [....] Molto bene. Soltanto due anni. Poi è andato alle medie. Ho fatto primo e
secondo a scuola pubblica. Un disastro. Basta. Gli insegnanti non riescavano
gestire la situazione. Le altre mamme non accettano. [Lui era] sempre da solo,
isolato in una stanza con due insegnanti di sostegno.
E cosa mi ha detto: “[Signora], non fare (anche tu) l’errore più grande della mia
vita, che ho fatto, di mandarla alle scuole medie, perché è un incubo”. Perché poi
dipende tutto dagli insegnanti, poi, che trovi, eh?
Allora, l’elementarie è stata una fabula, Ariel, una fabula. Perché c’erano questi
insegnanti molto disponibili, i bambini sono tutti amici, avevo bambini in casa
tutti giorni, lui andava alle feste. Cioè è tutta una fabula, ti dico. Insegnanti,
educatori, tutto bene. Una fabula. Alle medie è andata abbstanza bene nel senso
che uh in classe stava un po’ meno però aveva una classe di sostegno dove
c’erano un bel gruppo di ragazzini con vari tipi di problemi, non solo disabile
intellittivo ma anche disagio di un ragazzino straniero, o un ragazzino addotatto,
o un ragazzino con altre problematiche. Quindi erano un bel gruppetto. E direi
che è andata abbastanza bene. Dal punto di vista di come dire didattico. Dal
punto di visto di relazionale ti dico che ha perso tutti i suoi amici, e quindi suo
gruppetto era quello di questi 4, 5, ragazzi qua.
p. 258
Noi conosciamo persone che hanno figli con grossi handicap. E uno di questi ha
preso una laurea. Quanto sia stata guadagnata questa laurea… per me è stata
regalata. Però è stato fatto un danno, a mio parere. Perché questo ragazzo ha
una laurea, e quindi ha delle aspettative, quelle aspettative che il fatto di avere
quella laurea gli danno il diritto di avere. Ma purtroppo non le avrà mai, non
saranno mai soddisfatte, queste aspettative. Perché vivi in un contesto in
421
cui…mmm… non – non c’è lavoro per i – per i soggetti normali. Oltretutto poi,
ha anche delle problematiche che fanno sì che la sua laurea non potrai mai
essere applicata in qualcosa di pratico. Per cui questo ragazzo sta veramente – è
entrato in crisi, è entrato in crisi – sta veramente male! Se la sua famiglia, invece
di mirare così in alto, avesse mirato molto più in basso e l’avesse inserito in un
contesto magari lavorativo, a quest’ora magari avrebbe un lavoro, sarebbe
soddisfatto.
p. 259
Per i ragazzi come [figlio] non c’era possibilità di entrare in una scuola
superiore perché nelle scuole superiori pretendono un autonomia di 80% e cioè
allora vado io a fare la scuola superiore, cioè mio figlio non ci può andare. E
quindi dopo i 4 anni di medie, si è appoggiato alla [servizio], di centri diurni.
C’era il problema di trovare qualcosa che lo inserisse in altri tipi di attività. Non
ho voluto inserirlo in una scuola superiore perché dato che mi occupavo di
scuole, come lavoro, sapevo che non erano attrezzate per quei problemi.
E poi niente, ho trovato questo posto perché doveva iniziare le scuole medie, e
allora… cosa la mando a fare? Lì è peggio, nelle scuole medie, perché già aveva
13 anni, 14, è peggio. Perché i ragazzini già sono più grandi. E allora… ho
deciso di mandarla in una scuola come… ho trovato questo posto, che è stata la
mia salvazione [= salvezza], guardi.
p. 261
E in quella struttura io ritenevo che la situazione per [figlia] - non perché non
svolgeressero bene il loro compito ma perché quella struttura a mio parere non
era compatibile con la patologia di [figlia]. E allora mi sono rifiutato. E ho avuto
uno scontro con questa perché credeva di essere l’ottimus, la persona che saper
dire, [...] si permette di giudicare una ragazza che ha visto mezz’ora e che io
conosco tutta la mia vita. Permette che fosse un genitore sa meglio quali sono i
problemi della figlia. Che posso ascoltare o il dottore o le persone che sono qui al
centro, [mela] conoscono ormai da tanti anni. Se mi dicono una cosa, ascolto
perché so che loro vivono quotidianamente. Siccome mi fido, dico, “va bene.”
[....] Ma se tu credi di capire una persona per un solo colloquio che hai fatto, e di
indirizzarla dove credi tu più opportuna perché lì c’è posto, lì c’è la tua
situazione che ti fa comodo, no, questo no.
p. 263
L’isolamento degli autistici non lo trovo giusto, perché secondo me, mio figlio se
può andare insieme agli altri che magari uno è un po’ ritardato, l’altro ha un
handicap motorio, eccetera, si integra molto meglio che quando è tra di loro.
Perché ognuno di loro è abbastanza isolato [....] ma qua [al centro diurno],
422
diciamo per l’istruzione così, va bene, ma per le vacanze, oppure anche se fosse
per la residenza penso che sia meglio che siano misti, secondo me.
All inizio per [mio marito] diciamo l’idea di mettere [figlia] in un ambiente di
prsone con la sindrome di asperger era un modo per ghettizarla. Non riusciva a
vedere la possibilità invece finalmente di una interazione per lei. Ok? Quando
finalmente poi è venuto e si è reso conto che finalmente sua figlia interagiva con
qualcuno.
Però l’ho guardato, ho detto no, perché la mia impressione che ho sempre avuto e
anche un po’ su era, facciamogli frequentare sia posti dove ci sono le persone
come lui ma anche posti dove ci sono le persone che sono diverse da lui. Dopo di
(ecco) avevo capito che non funzionava così, che lui con le persone ‘normale,’
chiamiamole così, non si riusciva. E me metteva angoscia questo [servizio] come
idea prima che lo vedesse, o parlasse con qualcuno, perché avevo l’esperienza di
questa mia amica che ha cominciato con una associazione per i bambini Down da
cui era scappata perché, mi diceva, e questo (lo vivono pochi), tutti genitori
dicono il stesso problema, secondo me non è che si sollieva d’animo. Ecco. Ci si
angoscia di più. E quindi primo ho detto no.
p. 264
Io sono rimasta molto stupita perché ho trovato molto silenzio. Cosa che non
c’era a [l’altro servizio], che c’era una confusione (inaudible). Io sono entrata e
ho detto: mi sembra quasi un paradiso qua, per questi ragazzi, lo giuro! Perché
là era un caos indefinibile. E dicevo: “Come fanno questi ragazzi autistici a
essere qui impegnati?” perché li ho visti al loro tavolo, che facevano le loro cose,
non so che cosa, però… molto contenuti, molto impegnati. Ecco, questa è stata la
mia prima impressione: il silenzio, di questi ragazzi, occupati ai tavoli e tutto
silenzio. Gli educatori che giravano tra i tavoli… Questa è stata la mia
impressione. Perché a [figlio] dava molto fastidio la confusione, il rumore… e ho
detto: “mah, forse questa è la struttura giusta per lui”.
[A scuola andava] male. Veniva promosso. In realtà è arrivata qui non sa
leggere, scrivere, nulla [....]Tempo perso era. Sempre discussione con professori,
ligiti, iperattiva, non sta alla banca. Qui primi giorni non sta alla banca. Era
convinta che l’unica cosa per andare a casa era butta sulla terra. La scuola mi
chiamava, riprendevola. Ho spiegato all’educatrici, e mi hanno detto no, la
ragazza stava re-educata cento per cento.
p. 265
Però sicuramente ha trovato personale preparato che comunque di fronte a
queste cose non si sono tra virgolette né spaventati né... perché poi la sua
preoccupazione era sempre da dire “non mi volgiono più, mi mandono via.”
Perché il suo vissuto era di quella che faceva così e che poi veniva comunque
423
allontata. Allontanata dalla scuola, allontanata dal centro dove è andata,
allontanata dal centro diurno, allontanata da noi, allontanata quindi.
p. 270
Spero che in questo modo mio figlio sia impegnato. Ne fare cose che piacciono a
lui. E stare in mezzo agli altri. E di fare attività sportiva e... e... di essere
diventare così sempre più autonomo.
Gli piace. Inizialmente qualche volte non ci voleva andare lì, su quando... sì. Poi
... piano piano perché prima appunto faceva fatica anche a proprio... a doveva
uscire in quel determinato momento. Invece poi è diventata anche una routine,
una cosa abitudinale, quindi... si è un po’ abituato, no? ad avere questo impegno.
Ma a me piace tutto. Guardi. Per pur che lui faccia qualcosa, vede gli interessi
[....] Poi tutto il resto mi piacerebbe appunto che potesse fare qualcosa di, non a
pagamento per carità ma fare qualcosa che non sia solo divertimento [....] Se
facesse qualcosa magari di, non solo di divertamento, magari di ... potesse fare
qualcosa di impegno come più se, l’impegno proprio, magari potrebbe essere
utile, come non so se è mai stata pensata di una cosa così.
p. 272
Io spero che per lui diventa un po’ veramente come la sua casa. Adesso è ancora
un po’ presto. [....] [servizio] rapprasenta il suo posto di lavoro dove lui va a
lavorare da lunedì a venerdì. [....] Per me è da intendere che [figlio] lo usa, usa
[servizio] veramente come suo luogo di lavoro, la sua attività. Però poi c’è una
casa dove invece ha le sue cose. Il futuro potrà essere che lì si riarreda la sua
stanza. Secondo me il momento in cui lui comincierà a riaddersarsi la sua stanza
forse sarebbe venuto il momento dove la sua casa diventerà [servizo] però manca
ancora tanto tempo. Secondo me quando diventerà veramente un uomo grande.
Piano piano inzierà fare questo processo.
p. 274
E quindi siccome ero stato lasciata abbastanza sola e ero... non ero più in grado
di gestire la sitauzione, mi rendevo conto. Stavo male io, stava male lui, stavamo
mali tutti quanti. E niente, l’ho portato a [communità]. E in effetti in una
settimana là, le cose erano decisamente migliorate. Lui stava decisamente meglio.
Quindi, ho detto “va bene, una settimana di monitoraggio l’ha fatta, adesso
riproviamo a tornare a casa.” Non eravamo ancora arrivati a casa, era tutto
come primo. Tutto come primo. Infatti mi ricordo che ho durato ancora un mese,
ho tenuto duro [....] alla fine ho ceduto, ho detto che “lo mandiamo in
communità” anche se io non ero per niente d’accordo, perché io non volevo
staccarmi da un ragazzo di 16 anni. Però non vedevo alternativa, ecco, perché
siccome mi hanno detto che con i farmaci non si ottiene nulla, non si riesce a
424
calmarlo, ed era vero.
Poi sa le gente, parenti, alcuni “ah! L’avete messa in istituto, eh!, come mai!”
Cioè un istituto che poi, va be’, diciamo che la communità è un’altra cosa. Va
be’, ma io queste cose non... non è che mi importa, mi importa che la scelta di
[figlia], e per lei è stata la cosa più giusta, ecco [....] se devo essere sincera,
quando andiamo portato qua [figlia] com’è, la, il passo successivo era quello poi
di poter tornare a casa. Che [figlia] potesse tornare a casa. Con noi. Da poter
stare noi. Però siccome questo passo l’abbiamo già fatto, e forse come mamma
ho anche capito che non sempre stare con la mamma e il papà è la cosa più
giusta. Magari può sembrare a noi, ma no per lei.
p. 276
P: Gli obiettivi che lei diventi sempre più autonoma e faccia una vita, e abbia
anche un futuro buono. Gli obiettivi sono quelli.
M: Serena, e che sia sempre contenta di venire qua. Perché altri posti dove …
non è possibile. Anche loro andamenti, la loro quotidianità.
Io vorrei una cosa o così o comunque un apartamento però protetto nel senso, da
solo non può stare, ovviamente. Qualcosa... vorrei... essere tranquilla, dire OK
mio figlio è in grado anche da stare da solo.
p. 277
Noi parliamo di serenità psicologica, cioè la capacità di sentirsi sicuri e quindi di
poter prevedere quello che succede quindi in grado di scegliere. E non è soltanto
imparare ma credo proprio poter affrontare le scelte e realizzare delle cose, dei
comportamenti nuovi, delle abitudini [....] Tutti, credo, siamo molto convinti in
questa famiglia che vale molto anche per le persone cosidette normali. Sono
persone normali magari molto intelligenti paralizzate dell’ansia, del nervosismo,
dall’incapacità di decidere. In uno soggetto autistico, oh, si vede molto di più!
l’obettivo primario è – [figlio] è solo, ha bisogno di un gruppo amicale quindi un
bel gruppo amicale. Cioè lui aspetta il venerdì per vedere i suoi amici e il gioco
di squadra, lo stare insieme. E l’andar via anche da solo senza i genitori. Lo
rende più autonomo possibile. Quindi il raggiungamento di un distacco, di un
autonomia, ma sopratutto il sentirsi bello cioè mio gruppo di amici, ok? Anch’io
ce ho mio gruppo di amici
Per fortuna c’è [servizio] che è una cosa molto postiva per lui e alla domenica
almeno ha un momento di ritrovo con delle altre persone, con degli altri ragazzi.
E lo vedo molto contento [….] Proprio da subito ho capito che lui ha capito.
Perché quando io ho detto “guarda, andiamo a un posto dove ci sono dei ragazzi
che fanno un po’ fatica fare amicizia, come te.” Basta. Lui è stato al suo.. forse
lui si è sentito uguali agli altri.
425
Chapter 6
p. 286
P: Ma non lo se lei rende conto di quello che non ha, di quello che le manca.
M: Non si riesce a capire.
P: Per lei [è] la sua vita, per lei [è] sempre una vita buona. Una vita M: Sì, ha le sue cose P: Però non so se si rende conto che le manca quello che ha una persona
normale, ecco.
M: Anche se in realtà quando andiamo dalla sorella che ha i bambini... però non
P: - non più di tanto M: Non penso che lei riesca a capire nel senso di dire “mia sorella c’è l’ha
bambini, ha un marito, io no.” ecco non penso che [abbia] questo. Forse con
ritardo...
P: Non lo so nella sua mente cosa M: - cosa pensa P: - cosa pensa.
M: Perché lei ascolta.
P: Ascolta tutto.
M: Non le scappa niente.
p. 287
Io sono sicura che lui in parte percepisce i suoi limiti, e in parte no. Allora, la
parte più fortunata è quella che non li percepisce, perché per esempio ho sempre
pensato che [qualcuno che conosco con un’altra disabilità] ha avuto, avesse
avuto una vita così infelice un po’ perché aveva un cattivo carattere e un po’
perché era molto molto consapevole di avere dei limiti. E questa [cosa] debba
veramente... [essere reso] infuriato.
p. 288
nel mondo in cui viviamo adesso io penso che un soggetto autistico, la cosa
fondamentale forse è che non si accorga della diversità. Perché al momento in
cui, nel quale [figlio] se dovesse accorgere che è diverso degli altri, sarebbe un
dispiacere grande, per lui eh? E di conseguenza sarebbe un dispiacere anche per
me. Se devo pensare che [figlio] autistico infelice direi proprio di no. io direi anzi
che nella sua ingenuità, è tanto felice. Il problema che ho io in confronto del
mondo con autismo di mio filgio è che ho paura che il mondo non lo comprenda.
p. 290
Gli abbiamo confermato quello che lui diciamo già notava, che ha questi... questi
426
suoi interessi. Che abbiamo fatto notare [che] dei suoi coetani avevano altri
interessi, che lui non ascoltava tanto, che si isolava. E poi il fatto che a volte
parla da solo. Um. Diciamo io ho fatto notare i suoi comportamenti, abbiamo
ragionato su quelli. [....] Uh, lui dice “tratti autistici” perché una volta siamo
andati a una mostra sull’autismo e c’erano delle, tipo, degli esempi di
comportamenti di ragazzi autistici, e lui si è riconosciuto, ha detto, tipo ha letto
delle cose, ha detto “ah, ma anch’io sono così. Ah, tipo come faccio io.”
Ma sì, nel senso: ho qualcosa, so quello che ho, la chiamo così, non mi fa piacere
ma almeno so che cos’ho. Non sono un deficiente, perché tutti gli hanno sempre
detto che gli Asperger sono così intelligenti, lui stesso ha letto delle cose, io gli ho
fatto leggere anche dei libri che abbiamo comprato [inaudible]. Per cui non è
stato semplicissimo, magari, però non è stato neanche così traumatico. Io credo
che [figlio] avesse bisogno di sentirsi comunque compreso in qualcosa, cioè, di
dire… ho la febbre, so che ho l’influenza e non ho la broncopolmonite, per dire..
Io sinceramente a 14 anni mi [ero] già auto-diagnosticato la sindrome di
Asperger. Però continuavo ad andare da uno psicologo, pensavo di avere
qualcos’altro. Infatti sono andato più volte dallo psicologo per garantire che
avevo la sindrome di Asperger. Avevo scritto su un foglio le mie possibili diciamo
sindromi. Avevo scritto che potevo avere o un disturbo ossessivo-compulsivo,
oppure potevo avere la sindrome di Asperger. Allora io sono andato dallo
psicologo circa un annetto fa e io ho chiesto “ma secondo Lei, è un disturbo
ossessivo-compulsivo?” e lui mi ha detto “no, assolutamente no.” e così ho
capito che, e così ho escluso una delle 2 possibilità che ho detto allora, ho la
sindrome di Asperger perché io già a 14 anni avevo iniziato... avevo fatto l’ipotesi
che io avevo o disturbo ossessivo-compulsivo o la sindrome di Asperger. E poi a
17 ho avuto la conferma di avere la sindrome di Asperger [....] infatti appena lo
psicologo mi ha detto “tu hai la sindrome di Asperger” io ho sorriso. eh sì,
perché ero contento che da solo mi sono riuscito a diagnosticarmi la cosa.
p. 293
Allora prima sono stato diagnosticato, uh, depresso. Poi anche prima Asperger.
Prima ho avuto un deficit di attenzione. Prima ancora pensavano che io fossi
assolutamente autistico. Prima pensavano che io fossi psicopatico, e prima
ancora pensavano che io fossi ansioso, e prima ancora neurotico [....] Chissà
cosa penseranno che io sia dopo!
p. 294
R2 Secondo me, Ariel [...] la sindrome di Asperger non esiste. Cioè, uno che gli è
definito strano dagli altri o in pratica non si sa che cosa più parli, gli dicono che
la ha sindrome di asperger. Io sinceremente non credo di aver la sindrome di
Asperger anche perché se (vai a leggere) i sintomi dalla sindrome di asperger,
praticamente tutto il mondo ha la sindrome di Asperger. Visto che (quanti)
427
sintomi ha la sindrome di asperger, 50 sintomi.
R3 [....] Tutto quello che non è un sintomo di qualcos’altro e anche un po’ di robe
che sono sintomi di qualcos’altro sono sintomi di asperger.
R2Se sei troppo intelligente hai la sindrome di Asperger [....] Se ti muovi male in
educazione fisica, puoi avere la sindrome di Asperger. Se fissi le persone negli
occhi mentre parli, puoi avere la sindrome di Asperger, se non le fissi, puoi avere
la sindrome di asperger. Se sei geek puoi avere la sindrome di asperger. Ogni
cosa puoi avere la sindrome di asperger.
R3 Se sei disorganizzato hai la sindrome di asperger, se sei troppo organizz--, se
sei metodico, hai la sindrome di asgerger.
R2 Se scrivi male, la sindrome di asperger. Qualsiasi cosa. Io sinceremente credo
di... non ho la sindrome di Asperger. Io R3 Se hai interessi, se hai degli interessi, se hai degli interessi part-, se hai degli
interessi particolari che ti piacciono più degli altri, hai la sindrome di Asperger
R4 EccoR2 Sì, se non ti piace legare con le altre persone perché semplicemente le
consideri inferiori al tuo livello intellettivo e talmente stupide da non riuscire a
capire niente di quello che (fai) R3: [....] Non sei un Asperger, in quel caso se (inaudible) perché li consideri
inferiori sei semplicemente un sociopatico.
R2: Ma io vengo considerato Asperger. E solo perché, (uno) viene portato dallo
psicologo da bambino solo perché tipo non ti piace uscire con gli altri bambini
oppure non... non vuoi fare le cose, non rispetti bene le regole, ecc.
R3: Se hai un’opinione che ai tuoi genitori non piace, vieni portato dallo
psicologo e loro non (mollano finché non ti classificano) qualcosa. Ma visto che il
fatto non c’è ne sa una patologia vera per cui classificare semplicemente
un’opinione diversa, finisci comunque classificato Asperger, perché gli psicologi
- essendo comunque dei medici - hanno un’informazione professionale anche loro
abbastanza patologica, devono classificare tutto come (una serie comunque gli
mettono) l’etichetta a tutto. Ormai con tutti (inaudible) ci smettono di considerare
i pazienti come delle persone, le considerano semplicemente un ammassa di
sintomi.
A: [R4], cosa pensi?
R4: E, cosa?
R2: Infatti anch’io quando R3 dice che si sente come una nuova specie di razza
umana, anch’io molto mi ha capitato di pensarlo. Di essere un uomo sapiens
sapiens sapiens. Una nuova specie di essere umano.
R4: (R2), scusa
A: Grazie per i commenti ma si, aspetta (R4).
R4 Prego, Ariel, ma io personalmente [mi definirei come] una mania così di certe
cose, per esempio mi piacciono i videogiochi di guerra, i film, prevalentamente di
Mel Gibson.
p. 298
E ho ritrovato subito lui. Lui si è ritrovato meno. Io ho capito, sì è vero, quasi
428
tutto è lui. Ecco. E lui invece fa più fatica. Adesso no. Adesso essagera dall’altra
parte. Sta facendo, ha fatto un filmato perché da, che faranno vedere allo stadio,
sull’Asperger, in cui c’è lui, che è una cosa insomma (laugh/snort) abbastanza
pubblica.
p. 308
Papà, mi dispiace per come mi sono comportato ieri sera. Non mi piace dire
bugie. Papà, sono arrbbiato con te perché hai detto che sono licenziato. Non sono
licenziato. Lavoro al Centro. Vado al Centro ogni giorno e il Centro è mio
lavoro. Per favore, non dire bugie di più.
p. 309
Penso che, penso soprattutto a quello che dice la dottoressa [cognome], al “dopo
di me.” Al “dopo di me.” Perché [figlio] me l’ha detto l’altra volta, parlavamo
dei morti, perché nel giro di 3 anni ho perso mio marito, mio padre e mia madre.
E lui mi fa, mi ha detto “mamma, poi morirai anche tu” e io ho detto “[figlio],
sai, succede quando si diventa vecchi, poi si muore.” Lui mi guarda, lui fa “cosa
voul dire, che resterò solo?” Cosa si risponde così? E dico “[figlio], avrai tanti
amici, tante persone che ti vorranno bene, e che sostituiranno in parte quello che
... non ti potrò dare io.” Però sono domande che lasciano il cuore stretto.
p. 310
Uno dei miei psichiatri me l’ ha detto. Sono andato a trovarlo, mi sono trovato
bene. Poi ho cominciato ad essere incazzato perché ho realizzato che c’erano,
c’era un sacco di problemi che nessuno ha voluto affrontare, poi ho detto “perché
sono... perché sono incazzato? È un bel posto. Ci sono le persone che mi
piacciono. Potrei usare la mia intelligenza superiore a renderlo un posto
migliore.” Sto provando ancora.
p. 312
Sì, secondo me l’obiettivo è quello, far conoscere diciamo far conoscere più
persone perché una persona diciamo Asperger non sa neanche come fare a
conoscere gli altri Asperger. Cioè non può andare su facebook e scrivere
“persone con la sindrome di asperger” e far vedere degli amici che si può
trovare. O comunque cioè non c’è un posto dove può trovarle. L’unico posto per
trovare anche persone con la sindrome di Asperger è venire qui e magari legare
più amicizie con persone simile a te. Basta.
p. 313
Quindi... in realtà poi sempre con gli insegnanti abbiamo sempre instaurato dei
rapporti eccezionali. Ancora oggi insegnanti di sostegno vengono a trovarci o i
429
ragazzi che venivano a fare terapia, terapia a [figlia].
Beh, ne parla tuttora e [figlio] se [gli si] parla di [educatore], lui è
contentissimo, cioè ne ha un ricordo bellissimo. Poi mentre era con questo
[educatore] in questo centro sportivo italiano, ha conosciuto una ragazza,
maestra di pattinaggio, e educatrice, che lo ha affiancato dopo [educatore] e che
l’ha un po’ sostituito diciamo così [....] L’ha seguito tanto, anche con lei abbiamo
tenuto dei buoni risultati. Una ragazza, una figura diversa. Lui ci è molto legato
alla ragazza. Ancora la vediamo. Non l’abbiamo vista finché [figlio] è entrato
qua (sigh) ma comunque si sentono al telefono anche l’altro giorno si sono sentiti
quindi è una figura che è presente.
p. 314
E poi c’è... [ci sono] i due piscologi che vengono qui ad aiutare, va be’, tu mi
state simpatici. E quindi vengo anche per quello perché mi piace passare tempo
con persone tipo psicologi o robe del genere. [....] Non mi piace stare dove non ci
siano loro. Cioè a me piace stare in un posto dove diciamo ci sono gli psicologi
che capiscono ecc. Io vengo qui più che altro per essere ascoltato da loro perché
loro mi ascoltano sempre, anche tu [....] non mi interessa gli altri ragazzi, le altre
persone che vengono qui.
p. 315
E poi tipo ho fatto amicizia con la psicologa, mi piaceva andare lì, per me era
divertente perché era una persona che diciamo si interssava ad ascoltare quello
che dicevo. Che pochissimi lo fanno, cioè nessuno lo fa.
R: Quello attuale è probabilmente il migliore. Ascolta tanto.
A: Che tipo di professionista è?
R: Porc—Non ho ancora capito. Ascolta. Non mi tratta con condiscendenza. Non
mi costringe di prendere farmaci. Ecc. Come dicono nel Simpsons, è un tipo
speciale di “speaking doctor.”
È stato simpatico, uno molto comprensivo, paziente. Spesso mi aiutava. E quando
ero molto giù perché avevo problemi con i miei compagni di classe spesso mi
raccontava che lui quando era all’università anche lui ha avuto problemi con i
compagni, facevano i prepotenti con lui, sopratutto con qualche professore che
lui prendeva in giro.
Stavano… non stavano provando a rendermi migliore. Stavano provando a fare le
cose al posto mio. Una volta ero così incazzato del fatto che uno di loro ha scritto
sull’agenda mia, il mio compito, che l’ho buttata fuori dalla finestra. Sì perché
posso, ho passato settimane, mesi, a dirgli, “Vorrei che tu smetta. Devo imparare
a fare da solo.” Non ha mai, non mi ha mai ascoltato, porbabilmente perché non
ha pensato, ha pensato che il suo lavoro non era quello di rendermi una persona
430
migliore, ha pensato che io fossi qualche sorta di bambino buono a nulla che non
ha – che ha bisogno di qualcuno che faccia per lui.
Chapter 7
p. 323
Antropologa: Ma invece [di] autismo [Lei] ha detto psicopedagogia?
Rispondente: Psicologia e psicopedagogia. Neuropsicologia, le neuroscienze.
Però voglio dire, mentre la psichiatria è più collegata con la malattia mentale, e
che voul dire proprio la persona matta, la persona folle, la persona psicopatica,
la persona sociopatica, la persona con disturbi di personalità, mm? La persona
con autismo non è necessariamente mentalmente patologica. Ci può essere, può
esserci una codiagnosi, no? Ma non si può dare per scontato [che] la persona
con autismo è una persona che va nel campo della psichiatria. Non
necessariamente.
A: E la psicologia e la psicopedagogia è considerata una parte della medicina o
un’altra cosa?
R: No no no no. storicamente in Italia nasce più dalla branca filosofica.
p. 325
Pensavamo che non fosse una cosa neurologica e invece negli studi ho capito che
è proprio una cosa di struttura del cervello, un problema fisico. Questo è quello
che ho capito, la causa è questa. Una parte del cervello lavora in un certo modo
Nelle fattispecie appunto di [figlio] [...] [alcune delle] caselle del suo cervello
che erano molto più avanti. No? Invece in certe altre cose anche molto semplici
come un giorno, per esempio, ho detto “[figlio], apri l’ombrello.” Lui non era
capace di aprire l’ombrello.
Lei socializza magari con i bambini piccoli. Non magari con i giovani. Perché
lei… è bambina! È grande come donna, perché già ormai… però è piccola come
cervello, purtroppo.
p. 326
[Vengono] fuori queste... macchie, queste zone, così, che però ha detto la
neurologa [che] non giustificavano il ritardo nel linguaggio di [figlio].
Giustificavano un ritardo, ma non un ritardo così avanzato come quello. Per
questo lei fece questa diagnosi che c’era un ritardo mentale, ma la sindrome
dell’autismo un po’ copriva questa cosa, cioè era più in quella direzione che la
difficoltà di comunicazione di [figlio] – sempre più evidente – era giustificabile
con l’autismo piuttosto che con questo ritardo mentale.
Ho provato a farle fare degli - ma ancora addesso - facciamo un po’ delle cose
431
per conto mio, tipo il brain gym, cioè quindi partendo da una stimolazione,
facendo dei movimenti corporei, c’è una maggior fluidità di pensiero.
p. 327
C’è una ricerca a cui ha partecipato anche l’Italia, che sta arrivando… non so
quanto [sia] lontana a dimostrarlo, l’origine autoimmune dell’autismo. Cioè,
secondo cui può essere un qualcosa che entra, potrebbe essere un virus, un
banale - e io aggiungo, perché non un virus attenuato come quello di un vaccino?
- in gravidanza o nei primi mesi di vita, che scatena una reazione autoimmune.
p. 328
È un gastroenterologo che per primo ha cominciato a curare bambini autistici
rendendosi conto che la maggior parte di essi aveva dei disturbi intestinali [....]
se viene a contatto con glutine con le sostanze... loro sono più intolleranti che
allergici, sono molto più, quindi è una forma meno grave, però il loro intestino è
compromesso, cioè, almeno al tempo, quando lui fece questa diagnosi, mi disse
che ormai l’intestino di (figlio) era dann[eggiat]o, che aveva dei problemi e
quindi che non erano stati capiti ed era ormai molto infiammato. In effetti lui
ancora oggi nonostante si curi, quotidianamente prende iuna pastiglia per questo
problema e stia a dieta quindi con assenza di glutine e beh, sostanzialmente con
assenza di glutine. Prima anche caseina, adesso la caseina l’abbiamo
reintrodotta. Cioè lui appunto ha dei periodi lunghissimi in cui è sempre molto
gonfio. E questo è strano perché non si riesce a fare una statistica. Prima questo
corrispondeva con i periodi di maggior rilassatezza da parte sua. Quindi metteva
su peso e si gonfiava. Ora non è così, ora è il contrario. Ora si gonfia quando è
agitato. Quindi si può pensare che sia anche un’ingestione continua di aria,
tanta.
Leggendo adesso tutte queste informazioni su questi vaccini che hanno creato
danni, ho letto che questi vaccini economici venivano fatti utilizzando il mercurio,
che adesso tutti noi sappiamo che il mercurio causa danni al cervello.
p. 330
Secondo me è opportuno inserire i ragazzi autistici in strutture specializzate.
Soprattutto perché gli educatori sono preparati specificamente per lavorare con
ragazzi autsitici. Le attività sono adeguate ai ragazzi autistici.
p. 331
Anche grazie ai miei genitori che hanno trovato questo sezione [cioè, il servizio]
perché loro mi hanno consigliato infatti erano anche al loro merito, se non sono
[sic] per loro, non sapevo con chi relazionarmi, non sapevo con cui parlavo.
432
Non ci ha spiegato nulla sull’autismo! Non ci ha detto nulla. Continuava a dire
che questo bambino aveva problemi [....] e lei non arrivava mai al dunque, non
arrivava mai al dunque, mi aveva un po’ innervosita la cosa, ecco. Alla fine, è
stato il papà a dire “ma Lei sta dicendo che nostro figlio è autistico?” E lei ha
detto “sì, è autistico.” Ecco perché lei non riusciva ad arrivare al dunque.
p. 334
Ho notato che non guardavo mai... a volte avevo paura di guardare in faccia le
persone, oppure quando le guardavo in faccia le guardavo in modo pronunciato
cioè le guardavo troppo, quasi le fissivo con ... le inquietavo. Quando guardavo
una persona non guardavo lei ma le guardavo attraverso, guardo un muro che
c’era dietro. La persona era come se fosse invisibile. Molto spesso, e poi,
sottovalutavo, sottovaluto tantissimo le persone [....] E poi ho fatto vari test di
intelligenza su internet e tutti mi portavano al risultato alto.
p. 336
[Era] bello perché io l’ho condiviso appieno – è che era una terapia che
coinvolgeva anche la famiglia, per cui nel periodo in cui [figlia] faceva la terapia
in contemporanea, la facevo anch’io. E quindi cioè questo [era] lavorare insieme
sicuramente era, cioè ci si preparava insieme per andare, si facevamo le cose
insieme.
Comunque credo il Risperdol gliel’ho dato. Ho provato a darglielo, non so quanti
giorni, se 15 giorni o cosa, ho visto un peggioramento e lì io ignorante gliel’ho
tolto di colpo, cosa che mi hanno detto che non avrei dovuto togliere di colpo,
avrei dovuto toglierlo piano piano. Io non lo sapevo. E un giorno ho detto
“Basta! Non glielo do più.” Però sono contenta. [....] Ma considerando tutti
viaggi e che [figlio] ormai da tempo mi diceva “Mamma sono stanco, non voglio
più andare e lo stesso psicologo dopo che io avevo portato [figlio], magari usciva
5 minuti prima e diceva “Siamo usciti perché tanto non voule lavorare.” Allora
ho detto: “[figlio] è stanco. Non ha più voglia di fare queste cose. È inutile che
perdiamo tutto questo tempo perché portando a terapia [figlio] non aveva più il
tempo per fare altre attività. Infatti non poteva fare musica, non poteva fare
sport.” E allora io a un certo punto ho detto: “Basta, io blocco tutto.” Ho detto:
“Basta, terapie.” E quel tempo l’ho dedicato alla musica e allo sport. E [figlio]
ha avuto dei miglioramenti incredibili. Ecco. È stata la scelta fortunata.
p. 337
[Spero] che la situazione migliori [nel servizio]. Che prendano qualche
educatore in più. E che diventino autonomi nella gestione di [figlio] perché io
adesso lo seguo molto, eh? [....] Insomma [spero] che si arrangino un po’ più da
soli.
433
Con i professionisti conosciuti sono stata molto in disaccordo con quel equipe di
Delacato. Proprio perché promuovono un programma standardizzato, non
personalizzato [...] [mi hanno detto che] ero io che non riuscivo a portare avanti
il programma. Quando secondo me era un programma che non era adattato su le
sue - sui soui bisogni o soprattutto sulle tolleranze.
p. 338
E invece è un metodo che funziona bene, perché solo un’istituzione possa dare al
ragazzo autistico la serenità, la disciplina, l’ordine di cui hanno tanto bisogno. In
famiglia non si riesce. Le famiglie che conosciamo noi non sono in grado di
seguire sempre una routine stabilita, studiata, in modo scientifico. Non siamo
capaci. Non noi, ma neanche le altre famiglie che conosciamo.
Invece sa dove ci hanno detto che c’è una presa in carico totale, un papà che
abbiamo conosciuto 8, 9 anni fa [....] Lui ha vissuto in Nuova Zelanda, per tanti
anni. In Nuova Zelanda ha detto che prendono in carico dal momento in cui
nasce il bambino, “io facevo solo il papà.”
p. 339
Sì sì, ero tutto io nel senso che poi dopo mi sono, diciamo che ho... se poi alla fine
li strumenti ho sempre preparati io, ma ho voluto tenere separati il ruolo di
medico rispetto al roulo di mamma. Quindi preparavo tutto il materiale e lo
passavo alla neuropsichiatra infantile e poi volevo che fosse lei ad interfacciarsi
con gli insegnanti. Questo per non fare, insomma, la mamma, tuttofare,
comunque ci sono dei ruoli secondo me, c’è un certo ordine da rispettare. Questo
non toglie, chiaramente abbiamo degli ottimi rapporti quindi gli insegnanti
sapevano perfettamente chi è dietro questa cosa, c’ero io e abbiamo un ottimo
rapporto ma comunque ritengo che sia importante, ecco che ci sia, io sono
convinta che è stato molto importante anche il rispetto dei ruoli in cui una
mamma comunque anche se medico faccia la mamma ci sia neuropsichiatra
competente – questa è un’altra cosa molto importante sopratutto in Italia dove la
competenza sull’autismo da parte della neuropsichiatria è vicina allo zero – e poi
una scuola recettiva con la voglia di imparare.
Sono stata in seconda elementare molto fortunata a un certo punto, aveva
un’insegnante molto capace, molto molto brava, che all’inizio mi ha detto: “Lei
stia fuori, sì, faccia la mamma.” (Ridendo.) Poi dopo un paio di settimane mi ha
detto: “[Figlia] è molto complicata. Vediamo di lavorarci insieme.” Ho detto:
“Benissimo, lavoriamo insieme.” Quindi abbiamo fatto delle cose insieme e lei è
stata bravissima. Devo dire che è stata bravissima.
p. 340
Non si poteva assolutamente immaginare qualche cosa che riguardasse un
434
problema mentale. Non so, anche litigando con sua sorella, se lei gli diceva, o io
magari: “Cos’hai nella testa?”. Assolutamente, una frase da evitare in tutti i
modi, perché era una cosa… [una] tragedia, si arrabbiava da morire. Oppure:
“Ma sei scemo?”, oppure… dirgli una cosa così, che riguardava le malattie
mentali, diciamo… o “sei fuori di testa”, fare riferimento alla testa o al cervello,
era una cosa assolutamente… lo mandava su tutte le furie, perché chiaramente.
p. 341
Secondo loro il fatto che noi abbiamo scoperto tardi, tra virgolette, che [figlio]
fosse autistico... è stata una cosa positiva nel senso che noi abbiamo sempre...
um... cercato di... uh... sbloccarlo nel linguaggio, nel fare le cose. Nella sua
indipendenza. Abbiamo sempre insistito. [....] Però loro dicevano, appunto la
dottoressa diceva che secondo lei il fatto comunque di ... che l’abbiamo stimolato
veramente tanto. È stato stimolato “over” proprio in modo molto... poi magari
l’avremmo fatto lo stesso anche se sapevamo che era autistico prima, però.
Sono stata molto fortunata perché nella... intanto appunto mi ha detto che... non
si poteva ancora (trarre) una diagnosi totale, diciamo, perché era troppo
piccolina. E che comunque lavorando molto, c’erano molti margini, molti
margini di recupero. Quindi non è stato dato un verdetto assolutamente. Mi è
stato detto [che] c’è molto da lavorare. E ci siamo messi a lavorare. Questo
aiutava tanto. Cioè mi ha, in qualche modo mi ha fatto sentire un po’ soggetto
ancora, non vittima di una patologia ma ancora soggetto in grado di poter fare
delle cose.
p. 342
Quando dovevo spiegare tipo alle persone a cui ero affezionato che ero Asperger
sinceramente non ho avuto modo, cioè non gliel’ho spiegato. Ho detto: “è come
sono io,” perché loro già... le persone che io ritengo amiche o comunque che
ritengo vicino a me comunque capiscono quello che c’è, sì, vedono che sono
diverso dagli altri nel modo di comportarmi, nel modo di pensare, ecc. E quindi
cioè io dico questa in teoria è la sindrome di Asperger che riunisce tutti questi
fattori ecc.
p. 344
Nella Bibbia non si sa il giorno o l’ora, però noi abbiamo la speranza [che] non
ci saranno più malattie, problemi di lavoro, crisi economici, morte. Un governo
nel quale non ci sono mai più quei problemi. Questo paradiso nel mondo. Prima
ho visto mia figlia come gli altri ragazzi. È vero che ha un problema di cervello, e
nessuno lo può cambiare, è solo Dio che può darle un cervello come tutti gli altri.
Quello è il mio futuro, vivere sulla terra senza più problemi, malattie [....] Potrò
vedere mia figlia in realtà come tutte le altre e non vivere questa vita cattiva, tra
virgolette.
435
p. 345
Ti dirò che col tempo mi dà sempre più fastidio il discorso del personaggio
famoso, perché è evidente che la realtà delle persone con la sindrome di Asperger
non sia quella eccellenza, ma sia una prospettiva più difficile [....] Se all’inizio se
vuoi ho un po’ la consolazione di pensare che quelle persone così famose
avessero avuto la sindrome di Asperger, [ma] una conoscenza sempre più
profonda di che cosa sia la sindrome di Asperger, conoscendo anche tante
persone, tanti ragazzi con la sindrome di Asperger, mi portano proprio a
disgustarmi di questa cosa o anzi, essere sempre più infastidita da chi anche ha
un orgoglio di essere Asperger.
p. 346
I comportamenti ossessivi fra virgolette “normali” per esempio quelli che
fumano. La gran parte di quelli che fumano si danno [da fare con] un intero
pacchetto di sigarette. Finché ci sono sigeratte nel pacchetto io continuo a
fumare però posso accettare che se finisco il pacchetto, però, mi basta. Quindi
c’era una forma di autodisciplina che fa parte alla psiche e che in questi ragazzi
autistici è particolarmente spostata, particolarmente scoperta.
p. 347
Tutti, credo, siamo molto convinti in questa famiglia che vale molto anche per le
persone cosidette normali. Sono persone normali magari molto intelligenti
paralizzati dell’ansia, del nervosismo, dell’incapacità di decidere. In uno
soggetto autistico, oh, si vede molto di più!
p. 348
Mi sembra che meriti di avere i soldi necessari dietro per poter fare la ricerca
necessaria per aiutare questa popolazione, perché ormai sta diventando una
popolazione di persone speciali. In modo da trovare una compatibilità maggiore
tra questo mondo e il loro.
C’era l’aspettativa di avere una figlia normale che si comportasse come tutti gli
altri. E per questo dico che è un po’ stata la chiave di volta è stato il rispetto. E
quindi avere comunque una diagnosi, una conoscenza del problema significa che
sta a noi genitori modificare, trovare un contesto giusto, migliore, per il ragazzo,
e non martellarlo perché a tutti conti, a tutti i costi entri in quel contesto
preconfezionante.
p. 351
Io pensavo solo che mi desse quel numero di telefono e mi dicesse solo cose che
436
devo fare. Invece abbiamo iniziato a parlare, mi ha detto che anche lei era
separata, che però ha trovato un altro compagno, ha detto che... i problemi un
po’ di suo figlio, che adesso ha 18 anni, [nome]. Uh, così siamo rimaste al
telefono due ore. (Ridendo). Abbiamo parlato di tutto in due ore. Infatti adesso ha
detto che ci... non questa domenica ma la prossima ci incontriamo perché, dico,
dai, incontriamoci, ci beviamo un caffè mentre loro sono al [servizio].
Anche un paio di anni fa ci si è rivolta una mamma con una ragazzina, ce l’hanno
indirizzata... un pochino... ha conosciuto [figlia], con gli stessi problemi di
[figlia], le stesse modalità, molto... ma lei era minorenne ancora. E hanno dovuto
proprio allontarla dalla mamma e lei mi chiamava, piangeva e io le dicevo:
“Guarda, è dura, però guarda che di esperienze nostre, quelle che poi alla fine
vedrai.” E adesso l’ho sentita dopo un po’ e mi ha detto: “Guarda, avevi
ragione.”
p. 352
Non c’è un gruppo. C’è l’associazione, però non c’è un gruppo. Una
telefonata…io sono stata un mese e mezzo ad andare avanti e indietro
dall’ospedale, io non ho ricevuto una telefonata. Né da parte dei genitori,
neanche degli insegnanti di scuola, qua, eh? Ecco, questo mi ha fatto un po’
dispiacere.
[Figlio] deve andare in una communità per autistici. Perché questo che ci voule
per lui. Oppure in una comunità comunque per disabili perché io non sto a dire
che loro devono vivere con gli autistici. Loro devono vivere con altre persone
della loro età. Possono vivere con qualsiasi tipo di handicap, secondo me. Non
hanno bisogno di stare tra loro perché non è che si apprezzino tanto, gli autistici
(uno all’altro), proprio non si vedono, ecco, però hanno bisogno di stare in un
posto dove ci sono persone della loro età.
p. 355
Ah, quello che in Italia è un po’ diverso, nel senso [che] non è su questa carta
qua dove si diceva “[figlio] è autistico” che ti dicono “hai diritto a, hai diritto
a,” qui ti devi arrangiare da te in Italia. Nel senso che quando sono andata che
[filgio] aveva 2 anni in quel centro, io ho scoperto da una mamma che [figlio]
poteva avere il diritto ad una pensioncina di 200€ che chiama l’indennità. Non
sapevo niente. Quando sono tornata mi sono informata. Quando [figlio] si è
ricavato autistico nessuno mi ha detto niente, cioè non c’era, OK, sì ti fanno
all’uscita dall’ospedale, mi hanno dato l’esenzione ticket. Della mutua. Ma
quando ho avuto questa carta, non ho saputo niente. 5, 6, anni fa quando sono
entrata in [servizio], l’associazione, ho visto bambini come [figlio] o anche meno
gravi di [figlio] che avevano la pensione diversa, avevano il pass sulla macchina.
Ho detto “ma [figlio] no” perché queste cose le sai quando sei in associazione,
quando sei tra amiche. Non ti viene, non c’è molta informazione in Italia. Non c’è
437
una carta che dice “ [figlio] è autistico, devi fare questo questo questo questo.”
Avevamo organizzato dei corsi di formazione per gli inseganti delle scuole [....]
Ed erano corsi sia teorici che pratici e non stavamo a guardare che fossero
solamente di formazione per il TEACCH. Perché secondo noi un’associazione
che rapprasentava i genitori non doveva essere settoriale cioè non doveva essere
l’assocazione a dire “funziona solo il programma TEACCH.” Perché alcuni
genitori per dire con la comunicazione faciltata avevano avuto dei buoni risultati
e ci credevano. E allora come assocazione secondo noi dovevamo mettere in atto
non solo quello che noi come assocazione ritenevamo giusto, ma sentire un po’
tutte le campane.
p. 356
E mettermi in lotta con i professionisti, secondo me non sarebbe mai stata una
cosa positiva. Al limite se non ero soddisfatta, piuttosto che dichiararlo, lasciavo
perdere e cercavo un’altra cosa.
Quindi quando abbiamo trovato gli insegnanti giusti è stato veramente un buon
lavoro, perché io poi ho sempre cercato di collaboare con la scuola. Anzi, se loro
mi dicevano “guardi domani,” per dire “meglio che lui esca prima perché
l’insegnante di sostegno non c’è,” io me lo portavo a casa. Non insistevo per
lasciarlo lì, che poi magari era lasciato in un angolino. Nonostante ne avesse tutti
diritti, ma io ho sempre privilegiato il benessere di [figlio] ai diritti che
comunque avrebbe avuto però, ecco.
p. 358
Le aspettative sono di riuscire, molto ambiziose sono in realtà. Di riuscire a
costruirle intorno a un mondo assolutamente rispettoso. Cioè io ho veramente
questa aspettativa. E quindi mi inventerò se non riuscirà a seguire il percorso
scolastico, mi inventerò una scuola su misura per lei. Le costruirò un’attività
lavorativa su misura in modo che possa avere uno stipendo ed essere però anche
molto contenta. Sembrano più sogni di una bambina ma io ci credo molto in
questo.
438
References
Abel, Emily K., and Margaret K. Nelson, eds.
1990 Circles of Care: Work and Identity in Women’s Lives. SUNY Series on Women
and Work. Albany, N.Y: State University of New York Press.
Aiginger, K., and T. Leoni
2008 Typologies of Social Models in Europe. WIFO.
http://www.socialpolitik.ovgu.de/sozialpolitik_media/papers/Aiginger_Karl_uid5
63_pid502.pdf, accessed January 2, 2013.
Ajana, Btihaj
2012 Biometric Citizenship. Citizenship Studies 16(7): 851–870.
Alcano, Matteo Carlo
2009 Living and Working in Spite of Antiretroviral Therapies: Strength in Chronicity.
Anthropology & Medicine 16(2):119–130.
American Psychiatric Association (APA)
2000 Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-IV-TR. Arlington,
VA: American Psychiatric Association.
2013 Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5. Arlington, VA:
American Psychiatric Association.
Amit-Talai, Vered, and Mark Liechty, eds.
1995 Youth Cultures: A Cross-Cultural Perspective. In Youth Cultures: A CrossCultural Perspective Pp. 166–201. Routledge.
Anderson, Benedict
2006 Imagined Communities: Reflections on the Origin and Spread of Nationalism.
Rev. ed. London ; New York: Verso.
Anderson-Fye, Eileen P.
2010 The Role of Subjective Motivation in Girls’ Secondary Schooling: The Case of
Avoidance of Abuse in Belize. Harvard Educational Review 80(2):174–203.
Arduino, Giuseppe Maurizio, and Lorenza Latoni
2009 Percorsi Assistenziali Nell’autismo E Nei Disturbi Pervasivi Dello Sviluppo:
Dalla Diagnosi Precoce Alla Continuità Della Presa in Carico Tra Età Adulta.
Regione Piemonte: Agenzia Regionale per i Servizi Sanitari.
http://www.aress.piemonte.it/Download/SlideGallery/Torino20090507/arduino_la
toni_relazione.pdf.
2010 Epidemiologia E Percorsi Assistenziali Nel Case Dei Disturbi Pervasivi Dello
Sviluppo in Età Adulta. Autismo E Disturbi Dello Sviluppo 8: 133–150.
Armstrong, Elizabeth M.
2003 Conceiving Risk, Bearing Responsibility: Fetal Alcohol Syndrome & the
Diagnosis of Moral Disorder. JHU Press.
Árnason, Vilhjálmur
2013 Scientific Citizenship in a Democratic Society. Public Understanding of Science
22(8):927–940.
Arnett, Jeffrey Jensen
2000 Emerging Adulthood: A Theory of Development from the Late Teens through the
Twenties. American Psychologist 55(5):469.
2004 Emerging Adulthood: The Winding Road from the Late Teensthrough the
439
Twenties. Oxford ; New York: Oxford University Press.
Asberg, Cecilia, and Ericka Johnson
2009 Viagra Selfhood: Pharmaceutical Advertising and the Visual Formation of
Swedish Masculinity. Health Care Analysis 17:144–157.
Asperger, Hans
1944 Die “Aunstisehen Psychopathen” Im Kindesalter. Achiv Fur Psychiatrie Und
Nervenkrankheiten 117:76–136.
Autism Research Institute
2010a Defeat Autism Now! U.S. Clinicians.
http://www.autism.com/pro_danlists_us.asp.
2010b Defeat Autism Now! – A Project of the Autism Research Institute.
http://www.autism.com/dan/index.htm.
Autismo Italia
n.d. Criteri Diagnostici Secondo Il DSM-IV-TR (A.P.A. 2001).
http://www.autismoitalia.org/cridiagno.asp.
Bach, Marie-Anne
1998 Building the French Muscular Dystrophy Association: The Role of Doctor/Patient
Interactions. Social History of Medicine 11(2):233–253.
Bagatell, Nancy
2007 Orchestrating Voices: Autism, Identity and the Power of Discourse. Disability &
Society 22:413–426.
2010 From Cure to Community: Transforming Notions of Autism. Special Issue. Ethos
38:33-55.
Baggott, R., and R. Forster
2008 Health Consumer and Patients’ Organizations in Europe: Towards a Comparative
Analysis. Health Expectations 11(1):85–94.
Baio, Jon
2012 Prevalence of Autism Spectrum Disorders: Autism and Developmental
Disabilities Monitoring Network, 14 Sites, United States, 2008. Morbidity and
Mortality Weekly Report. Surveillance Summaries. Volume 61, Number 3.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. http://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED530639,
accessed September 4, 2014.
Baker, Dana Lee
2006 Neurodiversity, Neurological Disability and the Public Sector: Notes on the
Autism Spectrum. Disability & Society 21(1):15–29.
2007 Defining Autism in Canada: Unfolding the Public Aspects of Neurological
Disability. The Social Science Journal 44:687–697.
Baker, Dana Lee, and Trudy Steuernagel
2009 Comparative Policy Entrepreneurship: The Case of Autism-Related Policy in
North America. Journal of Comparative Policy Analysis 11(2):233–248.
Baker, Dana Lee, and Shannon Stokes
2007 Brain Politics: Aspects of Administration in the Comparative Issue Definition of
Autism-Related Policy. Public Administration Review 67(4):757–767.
Barbeau, Elise B., Isabelle Soulières, Michelle Dawson, Thomas A. Zeffiro, and Laurent
Mottron
2012 The Level and Nature of Autistic Intelligence III: Inspection Time.
440
http://psycnet.apa.org/psycarticles/2012-28388-001.pdf, accessed June 9, 2013.
Baron-Cohen, Simon, Sally Wheelwright, Antony Cox, et al.
2000 Early Identification of Autism by the CHecklist for Autism in Toddlers (CHAT).
Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine 93(10):521.
Baron-Cohen, Simon, Sally Wheelwright, Richard Skinner, Joanne Martin, and Emma
Clubley
2001 The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger Syndrome/highFunctioning Autism, Malesand Females, Scientists and Mathematicians. Journal
of Autism and Developmental Disorders 31(1):5–17.
Bates, Donald G
1995 Knowledge and the Scholarly Medical Traditions. Cambridge ; New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Beaudevin, Claire
2013 Old Diseases and Contemporary Crisis. Inherited Blood Disorders in the Sultanate
of Oman. Anthropology & Medicine 20(2):175–189.
Beck, Stefan, and Jorg Niewohner
2009 Localising Genetic Testing and Screening in Cyprus and Germany:
Contingencies, Continuities, Ordering Effects and Bio-Cultural Intimacy. In
Handbook of Genetics and Society: Mapping the New Genomic Era. Paul
Atkinson, Peter E. Glasner, and Margaret M. Lock, eds. Pp. 76–93. New York:
Routledge.
Beckmann, N., and J. Bujra
2010 The “Politics of the Queue”: The Politicization of People Living with HIV/AIDS
in Tanzania. Development and Change 41(6):1041–1064.
Begeer, Sander, Saloua El Bouk, Wafaa Boussaid, Mark Meerum Terwogt, and Hans M.
Koot
2009 Underdiagnosis and Referral Bias of Autism in Ethnic Minorities. Journal of
Autism and Developmental Disorders 39(1):142–148.
Begeny, John C., and Brian K. Martens
2007 Inclusionary Education in Italy: A Literature Review and Call for More Empirical
Research. Remedial and Special Education 28:80–94.
Bettelheim, Bruno
1972 Empty Fortress. New York, NY: Free Press.
Bharadwaj, Aditya
2008 Biosociality and Biocrossings: Encounters with Assisted Conception and
Embryonic Stem Cells in India. In Biosocialities, Genetics and the Social
Sciences: Making Biologies and Identities. Sahra Gibbon and Carlos Novas, eds.
Pp. 98–116. London, UK: Routledge.
Biklen, Douglas, and Richard Attfield
2005 Autism and the Myth of the Person Alone. NYU Press.
Bilu, Yoram, and Yehuda C. Goodman
1997 What Does the Soul Say? Metaphysical Uses of Facilitated Communication in the
Jewish Ultraorthodox Community. Ethos 25:375–407.
Bleuler, Eugen
1950 Dementia Praecox; Or, The Group of Schizophrenias. International Universities
Press.
441
Bliss, Catherine
2011 Racial Taxonomy in Genomics. Social Science & Medicine 73(7):1019–1027.
Block, Pamela, and Fatima Cavalcante
2012 Autism in Brazil from Adovcacy and Self-Advocacy Perspectives: A Preliminary
Research Report. Autism Around the Globe.
http://www.autismaroundtheglobe.org/countries/Brazil.asp.
Block, Pamela, Eva L. Rodriguez, Marcia C. Milazzo, et al.
2011 Building Pediatric Multiple Sclerosis Community Using a Disability Studies
Framework of Empowerment. In Disability and Community. Allison C. Carey
and Richard K. Scotch, eds. Pp. 85–112. Research in Social Science and
Disability. Bingle: Emerald.
Blum, Richard H, and Eva Maria Blum
1965 Health and Healing in Rural Greece; a Study of Three Communities. Stanford,
Calif: Stanford University Press.
1970 The Dangerous Hour: The Lore of Crisis and Mystery in Rural Greece. Chatto &
Windus.
Bodewitz, J.H.W., Henk Buurma, and Gerard H. de Vries
1987 Regulatory Science and the Social Management of Trust in Medicine. In The
Social Construction of Technological Systems: New Directions in the Sociology
and History of Technology. Wiebe E Bijker, Thomas Parke Hughes, and T. J
Pinch, eds. Pp. 243–259. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press.
Bollini, P., M. Reich, and G. Muscettola
1988 Revision of the Italian Psychiatric Reform: North/South Differences and Future
Strategies. Social Science & Medicine 27(12):1327–1335.
Bonaccorso, Monica M. E
2009 Conceiving Kinship: Assisted Conception, Procreation and Family in Southern
Europe. New York: Berghahn Books.
Borghese, Massimo
2007 Autismo. Nuovi aspetti diagnostici e terapeutici. Torino: Omega.
Borkman, Thomasina
1976 Experiential Knowledge: A New Concept for the Analysis of Self-Help Groups.
Social Service Review 50(3):445–456.
Boundy, Kathryn
2008 “Are You Sure, Sweetheart, That You Want to Be Well”: An Exploration of the
Neurodiversity Movement. Radical Psychology: A Journal of Psychology,
Politics, and Radicalism 7.
Boutot, E. Amanda, and Diane P. Bryant
2005 Social Integration of Students with Autism in Inclusive Settings. Education and
Training in Developmental Disabilities 40(1):14–23.
Bracci, Silvia
2003 Sviluppo della neuropsichiatria in Italia ed Europa. Storia delle istituzioni
psichiatriche per l’infanzia. In L’Ospedale psichiatrico di Roma. Dal Manicomio
Provinciale alla Chiusura. Antonio Iaria, Tommaso Losavio, and Pompeo
Martelli, eds. Pp. 145–161. Bari: Dedalo.
Braun, Bruce
2007 Biopolitics and the Molecularization of Life. Cultural Geographies 14:6–28.
442
Breemer Ter Stege, C. P. C., and P. Van Heugten
1986 The Changing Structure of Dutch Mental Health Care. Social Science & Medicine
23(3):283–291.
Brekke, Ole Andreas, and Thorvald Sirnes
2011 Biosociality, Biocitizenship and the New Regime of Hope and Despair:
Interpreting “Portraits of Hope” and the “Mehmet Case.” New Genetics and
Society 30(4):347–374.
Briggs, Charles L., and Daniel C. Hallin
2007 The Neoliberal Subject and its Contradictions in News Coverage ff Health Issues.
Social Text 25(4):43–66.
Broderick, Alicia, and Ari Ne’eman
2008 Autism as Metaphor: Narrative and Counter-Narrative. International Journal of
Inclusive Education 12:469–476.
Bromberger, C.
2006 Towards an Anthropology of the Mediterranean. History and Anthropology 17(2):
91–107.
Brown, B. Bradford, and Reed W. Larson
2002 The Kaleidoscope of Adolescence: Experiences of the World’s Youth at the
Beginning of the 21st Century. In The World’s Youth: Adolescence in Eight
Regions of the Globe Pp. 1–20. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Brownlow, Charlotte
2010 Presenting the Self: Negotiating a Label of Autism. Journal of Intellectual and
Developmental Disability 35(1):14–21.
Brownlow, Charlotte, and O’Dell, Lindsay
2013 Autism as a Form of Biological Citizenship. In Worlds of Autism: Across the
Spectrum of Neurological Difference. Joyce Davidson and Michael Orsini, eds.
Pp. 97–114. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.
Buchman, Daniel Z., Judy Illes, and Peter B. Reiner
2011 The Paradox of Addiction Neuroscience. Neuroethics 4(2):65–77.
Bucholtz, Mary
1999 “Why Be Normal?”: Language and Identity Practices in a Community of
Nerd Girls. Language in Society 28(2): 203–223.
2001 The Whiteness of Nerds: Superstandard English and Racial Markedness. Journal
of Linguistic Anthropology 11(1): 84–100.
2002 Youth and Cultural Practice. Annual Review of Anthropology 31: 525–552.
Bucholtz, Mary, and Kira Hall
2006 Language and Identity. In A Companion to Linguistic Anthropology. Alessandro
Duranti, ed. Pp. 369–394. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.
Bumiller, Kristin
2008 Quirky Citizens: Autism, Gender and Reimagining Disability. Signs 33:967.
2009 The Geneticization of Autism: From New Reproductive Technologies to the
Conception of Genetic Normalcy. Signs 34(4):875–899.
2013 In Worlds of Autism: Across the Spectrum of Neurological Difference. Joyce
Davidson and Michael Orsini, eds. University of Minnesota Press.
Burbank, Victoria Katherine
1988 Aboriginal Adolescence: Maidenhood in an Australian Community. Adolescents
443
in a Changing World. New Brunswick: Rutgers University Press.
Burke, L.
2008 “The Country of My Disease”: Genes and Genealogy in Alzheimer’s LifeWriting. Journal of Literary & Cultural Disability Studies 2(1):63–74.
Burri, Regula Valerie
2007 Sociotechnical Anatomy: Technology, Space, and Body in the MRI Unit. In
Biomedicine as Culture: Instrumental Practices, Technoscientific Knowledge, and
New Modes of Life. 1st edition. Regula Valérie Burri and Joseph Dumit, eds. Pp.
109–121. Routledge.
Burti, Lorenzo
2001 Italian Psychiatric Reform 20 plus Years after. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica
104:41–46.
Burti, Lorenzo, and Paul R. Benson
1996 Psychiatric Reform in Italy: Developments Since 1978. International Journal of
Law and Psychiatry 19:373–390.
Cambrosio, Alberto, Allan Young, and Margaret Lock
2000 Introduction. In Living and Working with the New Medical Technologies:
Intersections of Inquiry. Margaret M Lock, Allan Young, and Alberto Cambrosio,
eds. Pp. 1–16. Cambridge Studies in Medical Anthropology, 8. Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Cameron, R J
1997 Early Intervention for Young Children with Developmental Delay: The Portage
Approach. Child: Care, Health and Development 23(1):11–27.
Canevaro, Andrea, and Lucia de Anna
2010 The Historical Evolution of School Integration in Italy: Some Witnesses and
Considerations. ALTER, European Journal of Disability Research 4:203–216.
Canguilhem, Georges
1978 On the Normal and the Pathological. Studies in the History of Modern Science, v.
3. Dordrecht, Holland ; Boston: D. Reidel Pub. Co.
Carpenter, L. M
2010 On Remedicalisation: Male Circumcision in the United States and Great Britain.
Sociology of Health & Illness 32(4): 613–630.
Carson, John
2004 The Science of Merit and the Merit of Science: Mental Order and Social Order in
Early 20th Century France and America. In States of Knowledge: The CoProduction of Science and the Social Order. Sheila Jasanoff, ed. Pp. 181–205.
New York, NY: Routledge.
Carter, Erik W., Lynn G. Sisco, Lissa Brown, Dana Brickham, and Zainab A. AlKhabbaz
2008 Peer Interactions and Academic Engagement of Youth with Developmental
Disabilities in Inclusive Middle and High School Classrooms. Journal Information
113(6).
Cascio, M. Ariel
2012 Neurodiversity: Autism Pride Among Mothers of Children with Autism Spectrum
Disorders. Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities 50(3):273–283.
2013 Is Asperger’s Syndrome a Transient Mental Illness? Encountering the DSM-5. In
444
Tarragona, Spain.
2014a New Directions in the Social Study of the Autism Spectrum: A Review Essay.
Culture, Medicine, and Psychiatry 38(2):306–311.
2014b “This Is the Italian Variant on TEACCH”: Italian Adaptation of a North Carolina
Autism Service Model. Paper presented at the Society for Applied Anthropology
Annual Meeting, Albuquerque, NM.
2015a Rigid Therapies, Rigid Minds: Italian Professionals’ Perspectives on Autism
Interventions. Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry.
2015b Creating Opportunities for Adult Roles for Italians with Autism. Paper to be
presented at the Society for Psychological Anthropology Biennial Meeting,
Boston, MA.
Cassidy, Rebecca
2010 Global Expectations and Local Practices: HIV Support Groups in the Gambia.
Aids Care-Psychological and Socio-Medical Aspects of Aids/HIV 22:1598–1605.
Castel, Françoise, Robert Castel, and Anne Lovell
1982 The Psychiatric Society. Arthur Goldhammer, tran. European Perspectives. New
York: Columbia University Press.
Cataldo, Fabian
2008 New Forms of Citizenship and Socio-Political Inclusion: Accessing Antiretroviral
Therapy in a Rio de Janeiro Favela. Sociology of Health and Illness 30: 900–912.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC)
2014 Facts About ASDs. CDC - Facts about Autism Spectrum Disorders - NCBDDD.
http://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/autism/facts.html, accessed May 5, 2014.
Chamak, Brigitte
2008 Autism and Social Movements: French Parents’ Associations and International
Autistic Individuals’ Organisations. Sociology of Health & Illness 30:76–96.
Chamak, Brigitte, B. Bonniau, L. Oudaya, and A. Ehrenberg
2010 The Autism Diagnostic Experiences of French Parents. Autism 15(1):83–97.
Chamak, Brigitte, and Beatrice Bonniau
2013 Autism and Social Movements in France: A Comparative Perspective. In Worlds
of Autism: Across the Spectrum of Neurological Difference. Joyce Davidson,
Michael Orsini, and Majia Holmer Nadesan, eds. University of Minnesota Press.
Charles, Nicole
2013 Mobilizing the Self-Governance of Pre-Damaged Bodies: Neoliberal Biological
Citizenship and HPV Vaccination Promotion in Canada. Citizenship Studies 17(67):770–784.
Chu, Arthur
2014a Your Princess Is in Another Castle: Misogyny, Entitlement, and Nerds. The Daily
Beast. http://www.thedailybeast.com/articles/2014/05/27/your-princess-is-inanother-castle-misogyny-entitlement-and-nerds.html, accessed January 29, 2015.
2014b I’m Not “that Creepy Guy from the Internet”: How Gamergate Gave the Geek
Community a Bad Name.
http://www.salon.com/2014/10/30/that_creepy_guy_from_the_internet_how_gam
ergate_shattered_faith_in_the_geek_community/, accessed January 29, 2015.
2015 Nerd Masculinity: “They Won’t Give It Up Without A Fight.” Ravishly.
http://www.ravishly.com/2014/12/10/arthur-chu-nerd-masculinity, accessed
445
January 29, 2015.
Cimino, Guido, and Giovanni Pietro Lombardo
2004 Sante de Sanctis: Tra Psicologia Generale E Psicologia Applicata. Milano, IT:
FrancoAngeli s.r.l.
Cola, Mirna
2012 Ragionevolmente Differenti. Una Riflessione Antropologica Sulla Sindrome Di
Asperger E Disturbo Dello Spettro Autistico. Bologna, Italy: I Libri di Emil.
Cola, Mirna, and Daniella Crocetti
2011 Negotiating Normality: Experiences from Three Italian Patient Support Groups.
Kroeber Anthropological Society Papers 99/100(1):214–236.
Cole, J.
2004 Fresh Contact in Tamatave, Madagascar: Sex, Money, and Intergenerational
Transformation. American Ethnologist 31(4):573–588.
Collier, Stephen J., and Aihwa Ong
2005 Global Assemblages, Anthropological Problems. In Global Assemblages:
Technology, Politics, and Ethics as Anthropological Problems. Aiwha Ong and
Stephen J. Collier, eds. Pp. 3–21. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.
Collins, Paul
2004 Not Even Wrong: Adventures in Autism. Fusion.
2005 Né giusto né sbagliato. Avventure nell’autismo. C. Borriello, tran. Milano:
Adelphi.
Comune di Milano
2012 Avviso Pubblico: Per L’attivazione di interventi anticrisi a sostegno del reddito e
delle imprese finalizzati all’inserimento lavorativo a seguito di tirocinio in borsa
lavoro di persone in situazione di svantaggio sociale ed economico. Milano:
Comune di Milano.
http://www.comune.milano.it/dseserver/webcity/garecontratti.nsf/51607b595b240
841c1256c4500569c90/38708b62449be14dc1257aa300366bf4/$FILE/Avviso%2
0Pubblico.PDF, accessed February 5, 2015.
Condon, Richard G.
1988 Inuit Youth: Growth and Change in the Canadian Arctic. Adolescents in a
Changing World, v. 1. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.
Cornoldi, Cesare, Alessandra Terreni, Thomas E. Scruggs, and Margo A. Mastropieri
1998 Teacher Attitudes in Italy after Twenty Years of Inclusion. Remedial and Special
Education 19(6): 350–356.
Corsaro, W. A
1996 Early Education, Children’s Lives, and the Transition from Home to School in
Italy and the United States. International Journal of Comparative Sociology, 37
1(2): 121–139.
Corsaro, W. A, and Luisa Molinari
2000 Entering and Observing in Children’s Worlds: Some Methodological Insights. In
Research with Children: Perspectives and Practices. Pia Monrad Christensen and
Allison James, eds. Pp. 179–200. London: Falmer Press.
Corsaro, William A., and Francesca Emiliani
2014 Child Care, Early Education, and Children’s Peer Culture in Italy. In Child Care
in Context: Cross-Cultural Perspectives. Michael E. Lamb, Kathleen J. Sternberg,
446
Carl-Philip Hwang, and Anders G. Broberg, eds. Pp. 81–115. Psychology Press.
Cosmacini, Giorgio
2011 L’evoluzione Della Sanità Dall’unità d’Italia Al Duemila. Epidemiologia &
Prevenzione 35(5-6 Supp. 2):10–13.
Crainer, Brandy
N.d. Celebrate Austistic Pride Day on June 18th | Sunrise Group.
http://www.sunrisegroup.org/celebrate-austistic-pride-day-on-june-18th/, accessed
February 15, 2015.
Crapanzano, Vincent
1973 The Ḥamadsha: A Study in Moroccan Ethnopsychiatry. Berkeley: University of
California Press.
Crepet, Paolo
1990 A Transition Period in Psychiatric Care in Italy Ten Years after the Reform. The
British Journal of Psychiatry 156(1):27–36.
Crepet, Paolo, and Giovanni De Plato
1983 Psychiatry without Asylums: Origins and Prospects in Italy. International Journal
of Health Services 13(1):119–129.
Crialesi, R., A. Battisti, L. Salvador-Carulla, R. Martínez-Leal, and A. Rodríguez
2007 Deinstitutionalisation and Community Living–outcomes and Costs: Report of a
European Study. Country Report: Italy. In Deinstitutionalisation and Community
Living–outcomes and Costs: Report of a European Study. Pp. 265–311.
Canterbury: Tizard Centre, University of Kent.
https://www.kent.ac.uk/tizard/research/DECL_network/documents/DECLOC_Co
untry_Reports.pdf#page=267, accessed January 2, 2013.
Crosland, Maurice P, ed.
1976 The Emergence of Science in Western Europe. New York: Science History
Publications.
Crowley-Matoka, Megan, and Margaret M. Lock
2006 Organ Transplantation in a Globalised World. Mortality 11(2):166–181.
Cuevas Valenzuela, Hernán, and Isabel Pérez Zamora
2011 The Governance of AIDS in Chile: Power/knowledge, Patient-User Organisation,
and the Formation of the Biological Citizen*. International Social Science Journal
62(205-206):377–389.
Curnutte, Margaret, and Giuseppe Testa
2012 Consuming Genomes: Scientific and Social Innovation in Direct-to-Consumer
Genetic Testing. New Genetics and Society 31(2):159–181.
D’Alessio, Simona
2008 “Made in Italy”: Integrazione Scolastica and the New Vision of Inclusive
Education. In Policy, Experience and Change: Cross-Cultural Reflections on
Inclusive Education Pp. 53–72. New York: Springer.
2011 Disabilities Studies in Italia: Primi Sviluppi. Italian Journal of Disability Studies Rivista Italiana Di Studi Sulla Disabilità 1:3–9.
Daley, Tamara C.
2002 The Need for Cross-Cultural Research on the Pervasive Developmental Disorders.
Transcultural Psychiatry 39: 531–550.
2004 From Symptom Recognition to Diagnosis: Children with Autism in Urban India.
447
Social Science & Medicine 58(7):1323–1335.
Daley, Tamara C., and Marian D. Sigman
2002 Diagnostic Conceptualization of Autism Among Indian Psychiatrists,
Psychologists, and Pediatricians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders
32: 13–23.
Daley, Tamara C., and Thomas S. Weisner
2003 “I Speak a Different Dialect”: Teen Explanatory Models of Difference and
Disability. Medical Anthropology Quarterly 17(1):25–48.
Danforth, Scot, and Srikala Naraian
2007 Use of the Machine Metaphor Within Autism Research. Journal of
Developmental and Physical Disabilities 19(3):273–290.
Davidson, Joyce
2008a Autistic Culture Online: Virtual Communication and Cultural Expression on the
Spectrum. Social & Cultural Geography 9(7): 791–806.
2008b “More Labels Than a Jam Jar”: The Gendered Dynamics of Diagnosis for Girls
and Women with Autism. In Contesting Illness: Processes and Practices. Pamela
Moss and Katherine Teghtsoonian, eds. Pp. 239–258. Toronto: University of
Toronto Press.
Davidson, Joyce, and Victoria L. Henderson
2010 “Coming out” on the Spectrum: Autism, Identity and Disclosure. Social &
Cultural Geography 11:155–170.
Davidson, Joyce, and Michael Orsini, eds.
2013 Worlds of Autism: Across the Spectrum of Neurological Difference. University of
Minnesota Press.
Davis, John, Nick Watson, and Sarah Cunningham-Burley
2000 Learning the Lives of Disabled Children: Developing a Reflexive Approach. In
Research with Children: Perspectives and Practices. Pia Monrad Christensen and
Allison James, eds. Pp. 201–224. London: Falmer Press.
Davis, Susan Schaefer
1999 Adolescence in a Moroccan Town. Rutgers University Press.
Davis-Floyd, Robbie, and Carolyn F. Sargent
1997 Childbirth and Authoritative Knowledge. Childbirth and Authoritative
Knowledge. http://content.ub.huberlin.de/monographs/toc/ethnologie/BV011542334.pdf, accessed December 29,
2014.
Dawson, Michelle, Isabelle Soulières, Morton Ann Gernsbacher, and Laurent Mottron
2007 The Level and Nature of Autistic Intelligence. Psychological Science 18(8):657–
662.
Devereux, George
1980 Basic Problems of Ethnopsychiatry. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
De Wolfe, Juliette
2014 Parents of Children with Autism: An Ethnography. Palgrave Macmillan.
De Sanctis, Sante
1925 Neuropsichiatria Infantile. Roma: Stock.
Di Furia, Paola, and Giusi Montanari
2003 In service dirigenti: indicazioni per l’organizzazione dei servizi. In Autismo:
448
modelli applicativi nei servizi. Realizzazione di un programma psicoeducativo
mediante la formazione in service. Tina Lomascolo, Ascanio G. Vaccaro, and
Susanna Villa, eds. Pp. 193–214. Vannini.
Dinishak, Janette, and Nameera Akhtar
2013 A Critical Examination of Mindblindness as a Metaphor for Autism. Child
Development Perspectives 7(2):110–114.
Dodier, Nicolas
1998 Clinical Practice and Procedures in Occupational Medicine: A Study of the
Framing of Individuals. In Differences in Medicine: Unraveling Practices,
Techniques, and Bodies. Marc Berg and Annemarie Mol, eds. Pp. 53–85. Body,
Commodity, Text. Durham: Duke University Press.
Donnelly, Michael
1992 The Politics of Mental Health in Italy. London ; New York: Routledge.
Ducournau, Pascal, Pierre-Antoine Gourraud, Emmanuelle Rial-Sebbag, Anne CambonThomsen, and Alexandre Bulle
2013 Direct-to-Consumer Health Genetic Testing Services: What Commercial
Strategies for Which Socio-Ethical Issues? Health Sociology Review 22(1):75–
87.
Dufault, Renee, Walter J. Lukiw, Raquel Crider, et al.
2012 A Macroepigenetic Approach to Identify Factors Responsible for the Autism
Epidemic in the United States. Clinical Epigenetics 4(1):6.
Dumit, Joseph
2000 When Explanations Rest: “Good-Enough” Brain Science and the New SocioMeidcal Disorders. In Living and Working with the New Medical Technologies:
Intersections of Inquiry. Margaret M Lock, Allan Young, and Alberto Cambrosio,
eds. Pp. 209–232. Cambridge Studies in Medical Anthropology, 8. Cambridge,
UK: Cambridge University Press.
2006 Illnesses You Have to Fight to Get: Facts as Forces in Uncertain, Emergent
Illnesses. Social Science & Medicine (1982) 62(3):577–590.
Ecks, D. S
2005 Pharmaceutical Citizenship: Antidepressant Marketing and the Promise of
Demarginalization in India. Anthropology & Medicine 12(3): 239–254.
Edgerton, Robert B
1971 The Cloak of Competence; Stigma in the Lives of the Mentally Retarded.
Berkeley: University of California Press.
, ed.
1984 Lives in Process: Mildly Retarded Adults in a Large City. Monographs of the
American Association on Mental Deficiency, no. 6. Washington, DC: American
Association on Mental Deficiency.
Eglash, Ron
2002 Race, Sex, and Nerds: From Black Geeks to Asian American Hipsters. Social
Text 20(2 71):49–64.
El-Haj, Nadia Abu
2007 The Genetic Reinscription of Race. Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 36:283–300.
Emerson, Robert M., Rachel I. Fretz, and Linda L. Shaw
1995 Writing Ethnographic Fieldnotes. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
449
Erikson, Erik H.
1968 The Life Cycle: Epigenesis of Identity. In Identity: Youth and Crisis Pp. 91–141.
New York, NY: W.W. Norton & Company, Inc.
1994 Identity: Youth and Crisis. W. W. Norton & Company.
Erikson, S. L
2003 Post-Diagnostic Abortion in Germany: Reproduction Gone Awry, Again? Social
Science & Medicine 56(9):1987–2001.
Ervas, Fulvio
2012 Se ti abbraccio non aver paura. Marcos y Marcos.
Erwin, Deborah Oates
1987 The Militarization of Cancer Treatment in American Society. In Encounters with
Biomedicine: Case Studies in Medical Anthropology. Hans A. Baer, ed. Pp. 201–
227. New York, NY: Gordon and Breach Science Publishers.
Estroff, Sue
1993 Identity, Disability, and Schizophrenia: The Problem of Chronicity. In
Knowledge, Power, and Practice: The Anthropology of Medicine and Everyday
Life. Shirley Lindenbaum and Margaret M. Lock, eds. Pp. 247–286. Berkeley,
CA: University of California Press.
Eyal, Gil, Des Fitzgerald, Eva Gillis-Buck, et al.
2014 New Modes of Understanding and Acting on Human Difference in Autism
Research, Advocacy and Care: Introduction to a Special Issue of BioSocieties.
BioSocieties 9(3):233–240.
Eyal, Gil, Brendan Hart, Emine Onculer, Neta Oren, and Natasha Rossi
2010 The Autism Matrix. Cambridge, UK: Polity Press.
Eyre, S. L., V. Hoffman, and S. G. Millstein
1998 The Gamesmanship of Sex: A Model Based on African American Adolescent
Accounts. Medical Anthropology Quarterly 12(4):467–489.
Fabrega, Jr., Horacio
1993 Biomedical Psychiatry as an Object for Critical Medical Anthropology. In
Knowledge, Power, and Practice: The Anthropology of Medicine and Everyday
Life. Shirley Lindenbaum and Margaret M. Lock, eds. Pp. 166–188. Berkeley,
CA: University of California Press.
Farci, Giuseppe
2005 Per Un’educazione Special Dell’alunno Con Disturbi Pervasive Dello Sviluppo:
Riflessioni Relative Al Contesto Scolastico Italiano. Autismo E Disturbi Dello
Sviluppo 3:9–22.
Feighner, J. P.
1989 The Advent of the Feighner Criteria: A Citation Classic Commentary on
“Diagnostic Criteria for Use in Psychiatric Research” by Feighner JP, Robins E,
Guze SB, Woodruff RA, Winokur G, and Munoz R". Current Contents: Social
and Behavioral Sciences 43:14.
Fein, Elizabeth
2011 Innocent Machines: Asperger’s Syndrome and the Neurostructural Self. In
Sociological Reflections on the Neurosciences. Martyn Pickersgill and Ira Van
Keulen, eds. Pp. 27–49. Advances in Medical Sociology. Bingley, United
Kingdom: Emerald Group Publishing Limited.
450
Feinstein, Adam
2010 A History of Autism: Conversations with the Pioneers. Malden, MA: WileyBlackwell.
Felt, Ulrike, and Ruth Müller
2011 Tentative (id) Entities: On Technopolitical Cultures and the Experiencing of
Genetic Testing. BioSocieties 6(3):342–363.
Fenton, Andrew, and Tim Krahn
2007 Autism, Neurodiversity, and Equality Beyond the “Normal”: A Special Theme
Issue. Journal of Ethics in Mental Health 2:1–6.
Fiorani, Matteo
2011 Giovanni Bollea, 1913-2011: Per Una Storia Della Neuropsichiatria Infantile in
Italia. Medicina & Storia 11(21/22):251–276.
Fitzgerald, Des
2014 The Trouble with Brain Imaging: Hope, Uncertainty and Ambivalence in the
Neuroscience of Autism. BioSocieties 9(3):241–261.
Fitzgerald, Ruth
2008 Biological Citizenship at the Periphery: Parenting Children with Genetic
Disorders. New Genetics and Society 27(3):251–266.
Fombonne, Eric
2003 Epidemiological Surveys of Autism and Other Pervasive Developmental
Disorders: An Update. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 33:365–
382.
2005 The Changing Epidemiology of Autism. Journal of Applied Research in
Intellectual Disabilities 18(4):281–294.
Fondazione Censis
2012 La Dimensione Nascosta Delle Disabilità, Terzo Rapporto Di Ricerca: La
Domanda Di Cura E Di Assistenza Delle Persone Affette Da Sclerosi Multipla,
Da Disturbi Dello Spettro Autistico E Delle Loro Famiglie. Rome, Italy.
Fong, Vanessa
2006 Only Hope: Coming of Age Under China’s One-Child Policy. Stanford University
Press.
Foucault, Michel
1988 Technologies of the Self: A Seminar with Michel Foucault. Luther H. Martin,
Huck Gutman, and Patrick H. Hutton, eds. Amherst: University of Massachusetts
Press.
1990 The History of Sexuality, Volume 1: An Introduction. New York: Vintage Books.
1994 The Birth of the Clinic: An Archaeology of Medical Perception. New York:
Vintage Books.
2006 History of Madness. London ; New York: Routledge.
Fraser, Suzanne
2010 Hepatitis C and the Limits of Medicalisation and Biological Citizenship for
People Who Inject Drugs. Addiction Research & Theory 18(5):544–556.
Frazier, Thomas W, Stelios Georgiades, Somer L Bishop, and Antonio Y Hardan
2014 Behavioral and Cognitive Characteristics of Females and Males with Autism in
the Simons Simplex Collection. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Adolescent Psychiatry 53(3):329–340.e1–3.
451
Frazier, Thomas W., Eric A. Youngstrom, Leslie Speer, et al.
2012 Validation of Proposed DSM-5 Criteria for Autism Spectrum Disorder. Journal of
the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 51(1):28–40.
Freeman, Derek
1983 Margaret Mead and Samoa: The Making and Unmaking of an Anthropological
Myth. Harvard University Press.
Friedner, Michele
2010 Biopower, Biosociality, and Community Formation: How Biopower Is
Constitutive of the Deaf Community. Sign Language Studies 10(3):336–347.
Friend, John M.
2014 The Biopolitics of Policy-Making: Medical Activism in the Century of the Brain.
New Political Science 36(1):32–51.
The Feuerstein Institute
2014 ICELP.info. ICELP.info. ICELP.info.
Fullwiley, Duana
2010 Revaluating Genetic Causation: Biology, Economy, and Kinship in Dakar,
Senegal. American Ethnologist 37(4):638–661.
Fussell, Elizabeth, and Margaret E. Greene
2002 Demographic Trends Affecting Youth Around the World. In The World’s Youth:
Adolescence in Eight Regions of the Globe. Bradford B. Brown, Reed W. Larson,
and T. S. Saraswathi, eds. Pp. 21–60. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University
Press.
Gaines, Atwood D.
1982 Cultural Definitions, Behavior and the Person in American Psychiatry. In Cultural
Conceptions of Mental Health and Therapy. Anthony Marsella and Geoffrey
White, eds. Pp. 167–192. Dordrecht, the Netherlands: D. Reidel Publishing
Company.
1991 Cultural Constructivism: Sickness Histories and the Understanding of
Ethnomedicines beyond Critical Medical Anthropologies. In Anthropologies of
Medicine: A Colloquium on West European and North American Perspectives.
Wiesbaden, Germany: Vieweg Und Sohn Verlag. B. Pleiderer and G. Bibeau, eds.
Pp. 221–258.
1992a Ethnopsychiatry: The Cultural Construction of Psychiatries. In Ethnopsychiatry:
The Cultural Construction of Professional and Folk Psychiatries. Atwood D.
Gaines, ed. Pp. 3–49. Albany: State University of New York Press.
, ed.
1992b Medical/Psychiatric Knowledge in France and the United States. Culture and
Sickness in History and Biology. In Ethnopsychiatry: The Cultural Construction
of Professional and Folk Psychiatries Pp. 171–201. SUNY Series in Medical
Anthropology. Albany, N.Y: State University of New York Press.
2005 Race: Local Biology and Culture in Mind. In A Companion to Psychological
Anthropology. Conerly Casey and Robert B Edgerton, eds. Pp. 274–297.
Gaines, Atwood D., and Robbie Davis-Floyd
2004 Biomedicine. In Encyclopedia of Medical Anthropology. Melvin Ember and
Carol Ember, eds. Pp. 95–109. Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Kluwer Academic
Publishers.
452
Gaines, Atwood D., and Paul Farmer
1986 Visible Saints: Social Cynosures and Dysphoria in the Mediterranean Tradition.
Culture, Medicine, and Psychiatry 10:295–330.
Galt, Anthony H
1974 Rethinking Patron-Client Relationships: The Real System and the Official System
in Southern Italy. Anthropological Quarterly:182–202.
Garfield, E.
1989 Essays of an Information Scientist: Creativity, Delayed Recognition, and Other
Essays. Current Contents 12:3–10.
Geertz, Clifford
2000 “From the Native”s Point of View’: On the Nature of Anthropological
Understanding. In Local Knowledge: Further Essays in Interpretive Anthropology
Pp. 55–70. New York: Basic Books.
Ghaziuddin, Mohammad, Neera Ghaziuddin, and John Greden
2002 Depression in Persons with Autism: Implications for Research and Clinical Care.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 32(4):299–306.
Gibbon, Sahra
2007 Breast Cancer Genes and the Gendering of Knowledge: Science and Citizenship
in the Cultural Context of the “New” Genetics. Basingstoke, UK: Palgrave
Macmillan.
2008 Charity, Breast Cancer Activism and the Iconic Figure of the BRCA Carrier. In
Biosocialities, Genetics and the Social Sciences: Making Biologies and Identities.
Sahra Gibbon and Carlos Novas, eds. Pp. 19–37. London, UK: Routledge.
Gifford, Sandra M
1986 The Meaning of Lumps: A Case Study of He Ambiguities of Risk. In
Anthropology and Epidemiology: Interdisciplinary Approaches to the Study of
Health and Disease. Craig R Janes, Ron Stall, and Sandra M Gifford, eds. Pp.
213–246. Culture, Illness, and Healing. Dordrecht ; Boston : Hingham, MA,
U.S.A: Reidel ; Sold and distributed in the U.S.A. and Canada by Kluwer
Academic Publishers.
Gillis-Buck, Eva M., and Sarah S. Richardson
2014 Autism as a Biomedical Platform for Sex Differences Research. BioSocieties
9(3):262–283.
Glannon, Walter
2007 Neurodiversity: A Neurodiversity Theme Article. Journal of Ethics in Mental
Health 2.
Goldade, Kathryn
2009 “Health Is Hard Here” or “Health for All”? Medical Anthropology Quarterly
23(4):483–503.
Good, Byron J.
1977 The Heart of What’s the Matter The Semantics of Illness in Iran. Culture,
Medicine and Psychiatry 1(1):25–58.
1994 Medicine, Rationality, and Experience: An Anthropological Perspective.
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Good, Byron J., and Mary-Jo DelVecchio Good
1982 Toward a Meaning-Centered Analysis of Popular Illness Categories: Fright Illness
453
and Heart Distress in Iran. In Cultural Conceptions of Mental Health and Therapy.
Anthony J. Marsella and Geoffrey M. White, eds. Pp. 141–166. Dordrecht, the
Netherlands: D. Reidel.
1985 The Interpretation of Iranian Depressive Ilness and Dysphoric Affect. In Culture
and Depression: Studies in the Anthropology and Cross-Cultural Psychiatry of
Affect and Disorder. Arthur Kleinman and Byron J. Good, eds. Pp. 369–428.
Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
Gordon, Deborah
1990 Embodying Illness, Embodying Cancer. Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry
14(2):275–297.
Gordon, Deborah, and Eugenio Paci
1997 Disclosure Practices and Cultural Narratives: Understanding Concealment and
Silence around Cancer in Tuscany, Italy. Social Science & Medicine
44(10):1433–1452.
Gray, Carol A., and Joy D. Garand
1993 Social Stories: Improving Responses of Students with Autism with Accurate
Social Information. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities
8(1):1–10.
Greenhough, Beth
2011 Citizenship, Care and Companionship: Approaching Geographies of Health and
Bioscience. Progress in Human Geography 35(2):153–171.
Greenspan Floortime Approach
2014 Greenspan Floortime Approach for Children with Developmental Delays.
www.stanleygreenspan.com.
Grinker, Roy Richard
2007 Unstrange Minds: Remapping the World of Autism. New York, NY: Basic
Books.
Grinker, Roy Richard, Nola Chambers, Nono Njongwe, et al.
2012 “Communities” in Community Engagement: Lessons Learned from Autism
Research in South Korea and South Africa. Autism Research 5(3):201–210.
Grinker, Roy Richard, and Kyungjin Cho
2013 Border Children: Interpreting Autism Spectrum Disorder in South Korea. Ethos
41(1):46–74.
Groleau, Danielle, Allan Young, and Laurence J. Kirmayer
2006 The McGill Illness Narrative Interview (MINI): An Interview Schedule to Elicit
Meanings and Modes of Reasoning Related to Illness Experience. Transcultural
Psychiatry 43(4):671–691.
Gruppo Asperger ONLUS
2013 La sindrome di Asperger. Gruppo Asperger.
http://www.asperger.it/?q=asperger_syndrome, accessed June 8, 2013.
Guell, Cornelia
2011 Candi(e)d Action: Biosocialities of Turkish Berliners Living with Diabetes.
Medical Anthropology Quarterly 25:377–394.
Hacking, Ian
1995 The Looping Effects of Human Kinds. In Causal Cognition: A Multidisciplinary
Debate. Dan Sperber, David Premack, and Ann J. Premack, eds. Pp. 351–394.
454
Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.
2006a Genetics, Biosocial Groups & the Future of Identity. Daedalus 135(4): 81–95.
2006b Making Up People. London Review of Books, August 17:23–26.
2007 Kinds of People: Moving Targets. In Proceedings of the British Academy Pp.
285–318. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
2009a How We Have Been Learning to Talk About Autism: A Role for Stories.
Metaphilosophy 40(3-4):499–516.
2009b Autistic Autobiography. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B:
Biological Sciences 364(1522):1467–1473.
2010 Autism Fiction: A Mirror of an Internet Decade? University of Toronto Quarterly
79(2):632–655.
Haldane, Hillary, and David Crawford
2010 What Lula Lacks: Grappling with the Discourse of Autism at Home and in the
Field (Respond to This Article at Http://www. Therai. Org. Uk/at/debate).
Anthropology Today 26(3):24–26.
Hall, G. Stanley
1904 Adolescence; Its Psychology and Its Relations to Physiology, Anthropology,
Sociology, Sex, Crime, Religion and Education. New York: D. Appleton and
Company.
Hallowell, A. Irving
1961 Ojibwaw World View and Disease. In Men’s Image in Medicine and
Anthropology. Iago Galdston, ed. Pp. 258–315. New York, NY: International
Universities Press, Inc.
2010 Culture and Experience. In Psychological Anthropology: A Reader on Self in
Culture Pp. 30–50. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.
Hart, Brendan
2014 Autism Parents & Neurodiversity: Radical Translation, Joint Embodiment and the
Prosthetic Environment. BioSocieties 9(3):284–303.
Heath, D.
1998 Locating Genetic Knowledge: Picturing Marfan Syndrome and Its Traveling
Constituencies. Science, Technology & Human Values 23(1):71–97.
Heath, Deborah, Rayna Rapp, and Karen-Sue Taussig
2004 Genetic Citizenship. In A Companion to the Anthropology of Politics. David
Nugent and Joan Vincent, eds. Pp. 152–167. Blackwell Companions to
Anthropology, 2. Malden, MA: Blackwell Pub.
Hebdige, Dick
1991 Subculture: The Meaning of Style. New Accents. London ; New York: Routledge.
Hedgecoe, Adam
2001 Schizophrenia and the Narrative of Enlightened Geneticization. Social Studies of
Science 31(6):875–911.
Heilker, Paul
2012 Autism, Rhetoric, and Whiteness. Disability Studies Quarterly 32(4). http://dsqsds.org/article/view/1756, accessed January 29, 2015.
Hendriks, Ruud
2012 Autistic Company. Rev. and updated English ed. At the interface/Probing the
Boundaries, v. 81. Amsterdam ; New York: Rodopi.
455
Henrich, Joseph, Steven J. Heine, and Ara Norenzayan
2010 The Weirdest People in the World? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33:61–135.
Henry, Jules
1971 Pathways to Madness. New York, NY: Random House.
Herzfeld, Michael
1984 The Horns of the Mediterraneanist Dilemma. American Ethnologist 11:439–454.
2005 Cultural Intimacy: Social Poetics In The Nation-State. Routledge.
Hirschauer, Stefan
1998 Performing Sexes and Genders in Medical Practices. In Differences in Medicine:
Unraveling Practices, Techniques, and Bodies. Marc Berg and Annemarie Mol,
eds. Pp. 13–27. Body, Commodity, Text. Durham: Duke University Press.
Hollos, Marida
1989 Becoming Nigerian in Ijo Society. Adolescents in a Changing World. New
Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.
Holmes, Lowell D.
1988 Quest for the Real Samoa: The Mead/Freeman Controversy and Beyond.
Greenwood Publishing Group.
Howard, Brooke, Ellen Cohn, and Gael I. Orsmond
2006 Understanding and Negotiating Friendships Perspectives from an Adolescent with
Asperger Syndrome. Autism 10(6):619–627.
Huff, Toby E
2003 The Rise of Early Modern Science: Islam, China, and the West. 2nd ed.
Cambridge, UK ; New York: Cambridge University Press.
Hughes, Bill
2009 Disability Activisms: Social Model Stalwarts and Biological Citizens. Disability
& Society 24(6):677–688.
Hughes, Charles
1968 Medical Care: Ethnomedicine. In International Encyclopedia of the Social
Sciences. David L. Sills, ed. Pp. 87–93. New York: Macmillan.
1996 Ethnopsychiatry. In Medical Anthropology: Contemporary Theory and Method.
Rev. ed. Carolyn Fishel Sargent and Thomas M Johnson, eds. Pp. 131–150.
Westport, Conn: Praeger.
Humphrey, Neil, and Sarah Lewis
2008 “Make Me Normal”: The Views and Experiences of Pupils on the Autistic
Spectrum in Mainstream Secondary Schools. Autism 12:23–46.
Humphrey, Neil, and Wendy Symes
2011 Peer Interaction Patterns among Adolescents with Autistic Spectrum Disorders
(ASDs) in Mainstream School Settings. Autism 15:397–419.
Huyard, Caroline
2009 Who Rules Rare Disease Associations? A Framework to Understand Their
Action. Sociology of Health & Illness 31(7):979–993.
Hwang, Se Kwang, and Helen Charnley
2010 Making the Familiar Strange and Making the Strange Familiar: Understanding
Korean Children’s Experiences of Living with an Autistic Sibling. Disability &
Society 25(5):579–592.
Hyman, Mia
456
2009 Standing at Sinai with Autism: A Young Man’s Bar Mitzvah Journey. Journal of
Positive Behavior Interventions 11:186–192.
Institutes for the Achievement of Human Potential
2014 Autism Overview. iahp.org/autism/.
Italian National Statistical Institute (ISTAT)
2013 Italy in Figures. Roma, Italy. http://www.istat.it/en/files/2011/06/Italy-in-figures2013.pdf.
2014 L’integrazione degli alunni con disabilità nelle scuole primarie e secondarie di
primo grado statali e non statali. ISTAT. http://www.istat.it/it/archivio/143466,
accessed January 18, 2015.
Jack, Jordynn
2014 Autism and Gender: From Refrigerator Mothers to Computer Geeks. University
of Illinois Press.
Jackson, Luke
2002 Freaks, Geeks and Asperger Syndrome: A User Guide to Adolescence. Jessica
Kingsley Publishers.
Jasanoff, Sheila
2004 The Idiom of Co-Production. In States of Knowledge: The Co-Production of
Science and the Social Order. Sheila Jasanoff, ed. Pp. 1–12. New York, NY:
Routledge.
Jenkins, Janis Hunter, and Robert J. Barrett, eds.
2004 Schizophrenia, Culture, and Subjectivity: The Edge of Experience. Cambridge
University Press.
Johnson, Andi
2013 The Athlete as Model Organism: The Everyday Practice of the Science of Human
Performance. Social Studies of Science 43(6):878–904.
Jones, J. L.
2012 Factors Associated With Self-Concept: Adolescents With Intellectual and
Development Disabilities Share Their Perspectives. Intellectual and
Developmental Disabilities 50(1):31–40.
Jones, K., and A. Poletti
1986 The’Italian Experience’reconsidered. The British Journal of Psychiatry
148(2):144–150.
Jordan, Brigitte
1993 Birth in Four Cultures: A Crosscultural Investigation Ofchildbirth in Yucatan,
Holland, Sweden, and the United States. 4th ed. Prospect Heights, Ill: Waveland
Press.
Kalofonos, Ippolytos Andreas
2010 “All I Eat Is ARVs.” Medical Anthropology Quarterly 24(3):363–380.
Kampf, Antje
2010 “The Risk of Age”? Early Detection Test, Prostate Cancer and the Practices of
Self. Journal of Aging Studies 24:325–334.
Kang-Yi, Christina D., Roy R. Grinker, and David S. Mandell
2013 Korean Culture and Autism Spectrum Disorders. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders 43(3):503–520.
Kanner, Leo
457
1943 Autistic Disturbances of Affective Contact. Nervous Child 2:217–50.
Kapp, Steven K, Kristen Gillespie-Lynch, Lauren E Sherman, and Ted Hutman
2012 Deficit, Difference, or Both? Autism and Neurodiversity. Developmental
Psychology. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22545843, accessed July 29,
2012.
Kaufman, Barry Neil, and Raun Kaufman
1995 Son Rise: The Miracle Continues. Tiburon, Calif.: HJ Kramer.
Kaufman, Sharon R.
2010 Regarding the Rise in Autism: Vaccine Safety Doubt, Conditions of Inquiry, and
the Shape of Freedom. Ethos 38(1):8–32.
Kay, Lily E
1993 The Molecular Vision of Life: Caltech, the Rockefeller Foundation, and the Rise
of the New Biology. Monographs on the History and Philosophy of Biology. New
York: Oxford University Press.
Keating, Peter
2003 Biomedical Platforms: Realigning the Normal and the Pathological in LateTwentieth-Century Medicine. Inside Technology. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press.
Keating, Peter, and Alberto Cambrosio
2000 Diagnosing Leukemias and Lymphomas. In Living and Working with the New
Medical Technologies: Intersections of Inquiry. Margaret M Lock, Allan Young,
and Alberto Cambrosio, eds. Pp. 103–134. Cambridge Studies in Medical
Anthropology, 8. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Keller, Evelyn Fox
2000 The Century of the Gene. Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press.
Kendall, Lori
2000 “Oh no! I’m a nerd!” Hegemonic Masculinity on an Online Forum. Gender &
Society 14(2):256–274.
2011 “White and Nerdy”: Computers, Race, and the Nerd Stereotype. The Journal of
Popular Culture 44(3):505–524.
Kendell, R. E., J. E. Cooper, A. J. Gourlay, et al.
1971 Diagnostic Criteria of American and British Psychiatrists. Archives of General
Psychiatry 25(2):123–130.
Kennard, Edward
1959 Major Patterns of the Mental Hospital - U.S.A. In Culture and Mental Health.
Marvin Kaufmann Opler, ed. Pp. 391–409. New York, NY: MacMillan.
Kim, Taewoo, Charlotte Haney, and Janis Faye Hutchinson
2012 Exposure and Exclusion: Disenfranchised Biological Citizenship among the FirstGeneration Korean Americans. Culture, Medicine, and Psychiatry 36(4): 621–
639.
Kinsella, Sharon
1995 Cuties in Japan. In Women, Media, and Consumption in Japan. Lise Skov and
Brian Moeran, eds. Pp. 220–254. University of Hawaii Press.
Kirmayer, L. J
1988 Mind and Body as Metaphors: Hidden Values in Biomedicine. In Biomedicine
Examined. Margaret Lock and Deborah Gordon, eds. Pp. 57–93. Dordrecht, the
Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
458
2006 Beyond the “new Cross-Cultural Psychiatry”: Cultural Biology, Discursive
Psychology and the Ironies of Globalization. Transcultural Psychiatry 43(1):126–
144.
Kleinman, Arthur
1980 Patients and Healers in the Context of Culture: An Exploration of the Borderland
between Anthropology, Medicine, and Psychiatry. Berkeley, CA: University of
California Press.
1982 Neurasthenia and Depression: A Study of Somatization and Culture in China.
Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry 6:117–190.
1987 Anthropology and Psychiatry: The Role of Culture in Cross-Cultural Research on
Illness. British Journal of Psychiatry 151:447–454.
1988 Rethinking Psychiatry: From Cultural Category to Personal Experience. New
York : London: Free Press ; Collier Macmillan.
Kleinman, Arthur, Leon Eisenberg, and Byron Good
1978 Culture, Illness, and Care: Clinical Lessons from Anthropologic and CrossCultural Research. Annals of Internal Medicine 88(2):251–258.
Kleinman, Arthur, and Byron J. Good
1985a Introduction: Culture and Depression. In Culture and Depression: Studies in the
Anthropology and Cross-Cultural Psychiatry of Affect and Disorder. Arthur
Kleinman and Byron J. Good, eds. Pp. 1–33. Berkeley, CA: University of
California Press.
, eds.
1985b Culture and Depression: Studies in the Anthropology and Cross-Cultural
Psychiatry of Affect and Disorder. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
Kleinman, Arthur, and Joan Kleinman
1985 Somatization: The Interconnections in Chinese Society among Culture,
Depressive Experiences, and the Meanings of Pain. In Culture and Depression:
Studies in the Anthropology and Cross-Cultural Psychiatry of Affect and
Disorder. Arthur Kleinman and Byron J. Good, eds. Pp. 429–490. Berkeley, CA:
University of California Press.
Kleinman, Arthur M.
1977 Depression, Somatization and the “new Cross-Cultural Psychiatry.” Social
Science & Medicine 11(1):3–10.
Koegel, Lynn Kern
2011 Pivotal Response Treatment®: Using Motivation as a Pivotal Response. Behavior
Management Student Organization.
Koller, Rebecca
2000 Sexuality and Adolescents with Autism. Sexuality and Disability 18:125–135.
Korbin, Jill E., and Eileen P. Anderson-Fye
2011 Adolescence Matters: Practice-and Policy-Relevant Research and Engagement in
Psychological Anthropology. Ethos 39(4):415–425.
Kremer-Sadlik, Tamar
2004 How Children with Autism and Asperger Syndrome Respond to Questions: A
“naturalistic”theory of Mind Task. Discourse Studies 6(2):185–206.
Krige, John
2006 American Hegemony and the Postwar Reconstruction of Science in Europe.
459
Transformations. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press.
Kurtz, Hilda, Amy Trauger, and Catarina Passidomo
2013 The Contested Terrain of Biological Citizenship in the Seizure of Raw Milk in
Athens, Georgia. Geoforum 48:136–144.
Lappé, Martine D.
2014 Taking Care: Anticipation, Extraction and the Politics of Temporality in Autism
Science. BioSocieties 9(3).
Latour, Bruno
1993 We Have Never Been Modern. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Lave, J., P. Duguid, N. Fernandez, and E. Axel
1992 Coming of Age in Birmingham: Cultural Studies and Conceptions of Subjectivity.
Annual Review of Anthropology 21:257–282.
Lave, Jean, and Etienne Wenger
1991 Situated Learning: Legitimate Peripheral Participation. Cambridge University
Press.
Lebra, William P, ed.
1976 Culture-Bound Syndromes, Ethnopsychiatry, and Alternate Therapies. Mental
Health Research in Asia and the Pacific, v. 4. Honolulu: University Press of
Hawaii.
LeCompte, Margaret Diane, and Jean J. Schensul
1999 Analysis from the Top Down: Applying Codes Deductively. In Analyzing and
Interpreting Ethnographic Data (Ethnographer’s Toolkit, Volume 5) Pp. 45–66.
Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield Publishers, Inc.
Lee, Sandra Soo-Jin, and LaVera Crawley
2009 Research 2.0: Social Networking and Direct-to-Consumer (DTC) Genomics. The
American Journal of Bioethics 9(6-7):35–44.
Leiter, Russell Graydon, and Stanley David Porteus
1936 The Leiter International Performance Scale. University of Hawaii Bulletin xv(7): 142.
Leonardi, Matilde
2008 Libro bianco sull’invalidità civile in Italia. Uno studio nelle regioni del Nord e del
Centro. FrancoAngeli.
Leone, Barbara
2010a Riforma Gelmini Della Scuola: I Nuovi Licei. Studenti.it.
http://www.studenti.it/superiori/scuola/nuovi_licei_riforma_gelmini.php,
accessed January 15, 2015.
2010b Riforma Gelmini Della Scuola: I Nuovi Istituti Tecnici. Studenti.it.
http://www.studenti.it/superiori/scuola/nuovi_istituti_tecnici_riforma_gelmini.ph
p, accessed January 15, 2015.
2010c Riforma Gelmini: I Nuovi Istituti Professionali. Studenti.it.
http://www.studenti.it/superiori/scuola/nuovi_istituti_professionali_riforma_gelmi
ni.php, accessed January 15, 2015.
Leslie, Charles M.
1976 Introduction. In Asian Medical Systems: A Comparative Study. Charles M Leslie,
ed. Pp. 1–12. Berkeley: University of California Press.
LeTendre, Gerald K.
460
2000 Learning to Be Adolescent: Growing up in U.S. and Japanese Middle Schools.
New Haven, Conn: Yale University Press.
Levi, Gabriel
2005 Linea Guida per L’autismo: Raccomandazioni Techniche - Operative per Is
Servizi Di Neuropsichiatria Dell’età Evolutiva. Milano, Italy: Società Italiana di
Neuropsichiatria dell’Infanzia e dell’Adolescenza.
Levi, Gabriel, and Paola Bernabei
1997 Italy. In Handbook of Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders. 2nd
edition. Donald J. Cohen and Fred R. Volkmar, eds. New York, NY: John Wiley
& Sons.
Levy, Alissa, and Adrienne Perry
2011 Outcomes in Adolescents and Adults with Autism: A Review of the Literature.
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 5:1271–1282.
Lippmann, A.
1991 The Geneticization of Health and Illness: Implications for Social Practice.
Endocrinologie 29(1-2): 85.
Liptak, Gregory S., Lauren B. Benzoni, Daniel W. Mruzek, et al.
2008 Disparities in Diagnosis and Access to Health Services for Children with Autism:
Data from the National Survey of Children’s Health: Journal of Developmental &
Behavioral Pediatrics 29(3):152–160.
Littlewood, Roland
2000 Psychiatry’s Culture. In Anthropological Approaches to Psychological Medicine.
Vieda Skultans and John Cox, eds. Pp. 66–93. London, UK: Jessica Kingsley
Publishers.
Llewellyn, Penny
2009 Supporting People with Intellectual Disabilities to Take Part in Focus Groups:
Reflections on a Research Project. Disability & Society 24:845–856.
Lloyd, S.
2008 Morals, Medicine and Change: Morality Brokers, Social Phobias, and French
Psychiatry. Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry 32(2):279–297.
Lo Scalzo, Alessandra, Andrea Donatini, Letizia Orzella, et al.
2009 Italy: Health System Review. Health Systems in Transition Vol. II No. 6.
Copenhagen, Denmark: World Health Organization on behalf of the European
Observatory on Health Systems and Policies.
Lock, Margaret
1982 Popular Conceptions of Mental Health in Japan. In Cultural Conceptions of
Mental Health and Therapy. Anthony J Marsella and Geoffrey M White, eds. Pp.
215–233. Culture, Illness, and Healing, v. 4. Dordrecht, the Netherlands: D.
Reidel.
1995 Encounters With Aging: Mythologies of Menopause in Japan and North America.
Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
2005 Eclipse of the Gene and the Return of Divination. Current Anthropology
46(S5):S47–S70.
2008 Biosociality and Susceptibility Genes: A Cautionary Tale. In Biosocialities,
Genetics and the Social Sciences Pp. 56–78. New York, NY: Routledge.
2013 The Alzheimer Conundrum: Entanglements of Dementia and Aging. Princeton
461
University Press.
Lock, Margaret, Stephanie Lloyd, and Janalyn Prest
2006 Genetic Susceptibility and Alzheimer’s Disease: The Penetrance and Uptake of
Genetic Knowledge. In Thinking About Dementia: Culture, Loss, and the
Anthropology of Senility Pp. 123–154. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University
Press.
Lock, Margaret M., and P. Kaufert
2001 Menopause, Local Biologies, and Cultures of Aging. American Journal of Human
Biology 13(4):494–504.
Lock, Margaret, and Vinh-Kim Nguyen
2011 An Anthropology of Biomedicine. John Wiley & Sons.
Lora-Wainwright, Anna
2009 Of Farming Chemicals and Cancer Deaths: The Politics of Health in
Contemporary Rural China. Social Anthropology 17(1):56–73.
Lord, Catherine, Susan Risi, Linda Lambrecht, et al.
2000 The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule—Generic: A Standard Measure of
Social and Communication Deficits Associated with the Spectrum of Autism.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 30(3):205–223.
Lord, Catherine, Michael Rutter, and Ann Le Couteur
1994 Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: A Revised Version of a Diagnostic
Interview for Caregivers of Individuals with Possible Pervasive Developmental
Disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 24(5):659–685.
Loudon, Irvine
1997 Western Medicine: An Illustrated History. Oxford ; New York: Oxford University
Press.
Luborsky, Mark R.
1994 The Cultural Adversity of Physical Disability: Erosion of Full Adult Personhood.
Journal of Aging Studies 8(3):239–253.
Macrì, Sandra Lupi
2010 Mio figlio, un alieno dal pianeta autismo. EMP.
Magaña, S., and M. J Smith
2006 Psychological Distress and Well–Being of Latina and Non–Latina White Mothers
of Youth and Adults with an Autism Spectrum Disorder: Cultural Attitudes
Towards Coresidence Status. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry 76(3):346–
357.
Maggiolini, Silvia, and Paola Molteni
2013 Presentazione Ricerca-Azione. Paper presented at the conference of Cascina San
Vicenzo, Vimercate, Italy: Associazione Cascina San Vincenzo.
Mandell, David S., Richard F. Ittenbach, Susan E. Levy, and Jennifer A. Pinto-Martin
2007 Disparities in Diagnoses Received Prior to a Diagnosis of Autism Spectrum
Disorder - Springer. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 37(9):1795–
1802.
Mandell, David S., Lisa D. Wiggins, Laura Arnstein Carpenter, et al.
2009 Racial/Ethnic Disparities in the Identification of Children With Autism Spectrum
Disorders. American Journal of Public Health 99(3):493–498.
Markus, Hazel Rose, and Shinobu Kitayama
462
1991 Culture and Self: Implications for Cognition, Emotion, and Motivation.
Psychological Review 98:224–253.
Marsella, Anthony J, and Geoffrey M White
1982 Introduction: Cultural Conceptions in Mental Health Research and Practice. In
Cultural Conceptions of Mental Health and Therapy. Anthony J Marsella and
Geoffrey M White, eds. Pp. 3–38. Culture, Illness, and Healing, v. 4. Dordrecht,
the Netherlands: D. Reidel.
Marsland, Rebecca
2012 (Bio) Sociality and HIV in Tanzania: Finding a Living to Support a Life. Medical
Anthropology Quarterly 26(4):470–485.
Martucci, Jessica
2010 Negotiating Exclusion MSM, Identity, and Blood Policy in the Age of AIDS.
Social Studies of Science 40(2):215–241.
McCandless, Jaquelyn
2009 Children with Starving Brains: A Medical Treatment Guide for Autism Spectrum
Disorder. USA: Bramble Books.
McDonnell, Jane Taylor
1997 On Being the “Bad” Mother of an Autistic Child. In “Bad” Mothers: The Politics
of Blame in Twentieth-Century America. Molly Ladd-Taylor and Lauri Umansky,
eds. Pp. 220–229. New York, NY: NYU Press.
McLaughlin, Hugh
2009 What’s in a Name: “Client”, “Patient”, “Customer”, “Consumer”, “Expert by
Experience”, “Service User”—What’s Next? British Journal of Social Work
39(6):1101–1117.
McPartland, James C., Brian Reichow, and Fred R. Volkmar
2012 Sensitivity and Specificity of Proposed DSM-5 Diagnostic Criteria for Autism
Spectrum Disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent
Psychiatry 51(4):368–383.
Mead, Margaret
2001 Coming of Age in Samoa: A Psychological Study of Primitive Youth for Western
Civilisation. HarperCollins.
Medeghini, Roberto, and Enrico Valtellina
2006 Quale Disabilità? Culture, Modelli E Processi D’inclusione. Milano, Italy:
FrancoAngeli s.r.l.
Mehrotra, N., and S. Vaidya
2008 Exploring Constructs of Intellectual Disability and Personhood in Haryana and
Delhi. Indian Journal of Gender Studies 15(2):317–340.
Melone, Mary B., and Amy L. Lettick
1983 Sex Education at Benhaven. In Autism in Adolescents and Adults Pp. 169–186.
Current Issues in Autism. New York, NY: Plenum Press.
Mendoza-Denton, N.
1996 “Muy Macha”: Gender and Ideology in Gang-Girls’ Discourse about Makeup.
Ethnos 61(1-2):47–63.
Meroni, Gabriella
2008 Autismo: In Aumento in Italia, Ma Sottovalutato. VITA.
http://www.vita.it/news/view/87454.
463
Mesibov, Gary B., and S. Handlan
1997 Adolescents and Adults with Autism. Handbook of Autism and Pervasive
Developmental Disorders:309–324.
Mesibov, Gary B.
2007 TTAP: TEACHH Transition Assessment Profile. Pro-ed.
Metzler, I.
2007 “Nationalizing Embryos”: The Politics of Human Embryonic Stem Cell Research
in Italy. BioSocieties 2(4):413–427.
Migone, Paolo
2014 Storia Della Neuropsichiatria Infantile (prima Parte). Il Ruolo Terapeutico
125:55–70.
Ministero dell’Istruzione, dell’Università e della Ricerca [MIUR]
2012 Alunni Con Disabilità. Www.istruzione.it.
http://www.istruzione.it/urp/alunni_disabili.shtml, accessed February 15, 2015.
Misra, Kavita
2006 Politico-Moral Transactions in Indian AIDS Service: Confidentiality, Rights and
New Modalities of Governance. Anthropological Quarterly 79(1):33–74.
Mitchell, Chris
2008 Asperger’s Syndrome and Mindfulness: Taking Refuge in the Buddha. 1 edition.
London ; Philadelphia: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
Mitchell, Robert
2012 US Biobanking Strategies and Biomedical Immaterial Labor. BioSocieties
7(3):224–244.
Moderato, Lucio
2008 Modello superability. Un approccio globale alle disabilità intellettive, ai disturbi
pervasivi dello sviluppo e all’autismo. Vannini.
Mol, Annemarie
1998 Missing Links, Making Links: The Performance of Some Atheroscleroses. In
Differences in Medicine: Unraveling Practices, Techniques, and Bodies. Marc
Berg and Annemarie Mol, eds. Pp. 144–165. Body, Commodity, Text. Durham:
Duke University Press.
2000 Pathology and the Clinic: An Ethnographic Presentation of Two Atheroscleroses.
In Living and Working with the New Medical Technologies: Intersections of
Inquiry. Margaret M Lock, Allan Young, and Alberto Cambrosio, eds. Pp. 82–
102. Cambridge Studies in Medical Anthropology, 8. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
University Press.
Mol, Annemarie, and Marc Berg
1998 Differences in Medicine: An Introduction. In Differences in Medicine: Unraveling
Practices, Techniques, and Bodies. Marc Berg and Annemarie Mol, eds. Pp. 1–12.
Body, Commodity, Text. Durham: Duke University Press.
Molteni, Paola
2011 Voci dal silenzio. Testimonianze e indicazioni a sostegno delle famiglie che
vivono l’autismo. FrancoAngeli.
Mordini, Emilio, and Corinna Ottolini
2007 Body Identification, Biometrics and Medicine: Ethical and Social Considerations.
ANNALI-ISTITUTO SUPERIORE DI SANITA 43(1):51.
464
Mortimer, Jeylan T., and Reed Larson
2002 Macrostructural Trends and the Reshaping of Adolescence. In The Changing
Adolescent Experience: Societal Trends and the Transition to Adulthood Pp. 1–
17. Cambridge ; New York: Cambridge University Press.
Murray, Stuart
2008 Representing Autism: Culture, Narrative, Fascination. Liverpool University Press.
2009 The Perils of Scientific Obedience: Bioethics under the Spectre of Biofascism. In
Critical Interventions in the Ethics of Healthcare: Challenging the Principle of
Autonomy in Bioethics. Stuart J. Murray and Dave Holmes, eds. Burlington, VT:
Ashgate Publishing Company.
Nadesan, Majia Holmer
2005 Constructing Autism: Unravelling the “Truth” and Understanding the Social. New
York, NY: Routledge.
2013 Autism and Genetics: Profit, Risk, and Bare Life. In Worlds of Autism: Across
the Spectrum of Neurological Difference. Joyce Davidson and Michael Orsini,
eds. University of Minnesota Press.
Nahman, Michael
2008 Synecdochic Richochets: Biosocialities in a Jerusalem IVF Clinic. In
Biosocialities, Genetics and the Social Sciences: Making Biologies and Identities.
Pp. 117–135. London, UK: Routledge.
Nardocci, Franco
2009 The Birth of Child and Adolescent Neuropsychiatry: From Rehabilitation and
Social Inclusion of the Mentally Handicapped, to the Care of Mental Health
during Development. Ann Ist Super Sanità 45:33–38.
Narzisi, Antonio, and Filippo Muratori
1981 Autismo: diversità percettiva e riconoscimento delle emozioni al confine di
contatto tra organismo e ambiente. In Psicoterapia della Gestalt e neuroscienze:
Dall’isomorfismo alla simulazione incarnata. Pietro Andrea Cavaleri, ed. Milano,
Italia: FrancoAngeli.
National Institute of Mental Health
N.d. Autism Spectrum Disorder. http://www.nimh.nih.gov/health/topics/autismspectrum-disorders-asd/index.shtml, accessed August 29, 2014.
2007 Autism Spectrum Disorders (Pervasive Developmental Disorders). With
Addendum January 2007 (NIH 5511). Department of Health and Human Services,
National Institutes of Health.
Navon, Daniel, and Gil Eyal
2014 The Trading Zone of Autism Genetics: Examining the Intersection of Genomic
and Psychiatric Classification. BioSocieties 9(3):329–352.
Nguyen, Vihn-Kim
2005 Antiretroviral Globalism, Biopolitics, and Therapeutic Citizenship. Aihwa Ong
and Stephen J. Collier, eds. Global Assemblages:124–44.
Nichols, Shana, and Audrey Blakeley-Smith
2010 “I’m Not Sure We’re Ready for This...”: Working with Families Toward
Facilitating Healthy Sexuality for Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorders.
Social Work in Mental Health 8:72–91.
Nicoletti, Gianluca
465
2013 Una notte ho sognato che parlavi. Edizioni Mondadori.
Niewöhner, Jörg
2011 Epigenetics: Embedded Bodies and the Molecularisation of Biography and
Milieu. BioSocieties 6(3):279–298.
Norton, A. T, and O. Zehner
2008 Which Half Is Mommy? Tetragametic Chimerism and Trans-Subjectivity.
Women’s Studies Quarterly 36(3/4):106–125.
Novas, Carlos, and Nikolas Rose
2000 Genetic Risk and the Birth of the Somatic Individual. Economy and Society
29:485–513.
Ochs, Elinor, Tamar Kremer-Sadlik, Karen Gainer Sirota, and Olga Solomon
2004 Autism and the Social World: An Anthropological Perspective. Discourse Studies
6:147.
Ochs, Elinor, Tamar Kremer-Sadlik, Olga Solomon, and Karen Gainer Sirota
2001 Inclusion as Social Practice: Views of Children with Autism. Social Development
10(3):399–419.
Ochs, Elinor, and Olga Solomon
2004 Introduction: Discourse and Autism. Discourse Studies 6(2):139–146.
Ohnuki-Tierney, Emiko
1984 Illness and Culture in Contemporary Japan: An Anthropological View.
Cambridge [Cambridgeshire]: Cambridge University Press.
Olney, Marjorie F., and Amanda Kim
2001 Beyond Adjustment: Integration of Cognitive Disability into Identity. Disability
& Society 16(4):563–583.
Orans, Martin
1996 Not Even Wrong: Margaret Mead, Derek Freeman, and the Samoans. Chandler &
Sharp Publishers.
Orchard, Vivienne
2013 The“ Rendez-Vous Manqués” of Francophone and Anglophone Disability
Studies: The Case of Autism in Cross-Cultural Context. Synergies Royaume Uni
et Irlande 6:53–73.
Orsini, Michael
2008 Hepatitis C and the Dawn of Biological Citizenship: Unravelling the Policy
Implications. In Contesting Illness: Processes and Practices. Pamela Moss and
Katherine Teghtsoonian, eds. Pp. 107–122. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.
2009 Contesting the Autistic Subject: Biological Citizenship and the Autism/Autistic
Movement. In Critical Interventions in the Ethics of Healthcare: Challenging the
Principle of Autonomy in Bioethics. Stuart J. Murray and Dave Holmes, eds. Pp.
115–130. Burlington, VT: Ashgate Publishing Company.
Orsmond, Gael I., Marty Wyngaarden Krauss, and Marsha Mailick Seltzer
2004 Peer Relationships and Social and Recreational Activities among Adolescents and
Adults with Autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 34(3):245–
256.
Orsmond, Gael I., Hsin-Yu Kuo, and Marsha Mailick Seltzer
2009 Siblings of Individuals with an Autism Spectrum Disorder: Sibling Relationships
and Welbeing in Adolescence and Adulthood. Autism 13:59–80.
466
Ortega, Francisco
2009 The Cerebral Subject and the Challenge of Neurodiversity. Biosocieties 4:425–
445.
Ortega, Francisco, and Suparna Choudhury
2011 “Wired up Differently”: Autism, Adolescence and the Politics of Neurological
Identities. Subjectivity 4:323–345.
Ortega, Francisco, and Fernando Vidal
2007 Mapping the Cerebral Subject in Contemporary Culture. RECIIS-Electronic
Journal of Communication Information & Innovation in Health 1(2):255–259.
Osteen, Mark
2008 Autism and Representation. Routledge.
Oudshoorn, Nelly, and Andre Somers
2007 Constructing the Digital Patient: Patient Organizations and the Development of
Health Websites. In Biomedicine as Culture: Instrumental Practices,
Technoscientific Knowledge, and New Modes of Life. 1st edition. Regula Valérie
Burri and Joseph Dumit, eds. Pp. 205–222. New York, NY: Routledge.
Owen-DeSchryver, Jamie S., Edward G. Carr, Sanja I. Cale, and Audrey Blakeley-Smith
2008 Promoting Social Interactions between Students with Autism Spectrum Disorders
and Their Peers in Inclusive School Settings. Focus on Autism and Other
Developmental Disabilities 23(1):15–28.
Panerai, Simonetta, Marinella Zingale, Grazia Trubia, et al.
2009 Special Education Versus Inclusive Education: The Role of the TEACCH
Program. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 39:874–882.
Panofsky, Aaron
2010 Generating Sociability to Drive Science: Patient Advocacy Organizations and
Genetics Research. Social Studies of Science 41:1–27.
Parikh, Shanti, and Vincanne Adams
2005 From Auntie to Disco: The Bifurcation of Risk and PLeasure in Sex Education in
Uganda. In Sex in Development Pp. 125–158. Raleigh, NC: Duke University
Press.
Parsons, Talcott
1951 The Social System. New York, NY: Free Press.
Paxson, Heather
2008 Post-Pasteurian Cultures: The Microbiopolitics of Raw-Milk Cheese in the United
States. Cultural Anthropology 23(1):15–47.
Petryna, Adriana
2002 Life Exposed: Biological Citizens after Chernobyl. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
University Press.
De Pina-Cabral, Joao
1989 The Mediterranean as a Category of Regional Comparison: A Critical View.
Current Anthropology 399–406.
Poon, K. K.
2011 The Activities and Participation of Adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorders
in Singapore: Findings from an ICF-Based Instrument. Journal of Intellectual
Disability Research 55:790–800.
Prince, Dawn Eddings
467
2010 An Exceptional Path: An Ethnographic Narrative Reflecting on Autistic
Parenthood from Evolutionary, Cultural, and Spiritual Perspectives. Ethos
38(1):56–68.
Prince Eddings, Dawn
2013 “All The Things I have Ever Been”: Autoethnographic Reflections on Academic
Writing and Autism. In Worlds of Autism: Across the Spectrum of Neurological
Difference. Joyce Davidson and Michael Orsini, eds. University of Minnesota
Press.
Probst, Paul
1998 Child Health-Related Cognitions of Parents with Autistic Children: A CrossNational Exploratory Study. In The Family and Family Therapy in International
Perspective Pp. 461–483. Trieste, Italy: Lint Editoriale.
Quail, Christine
2011 Nerds, Geeks, and the Hip/Square Dialectic in Contemporary Television.
Television & New Media 12(5):460–482.
Rabeharisoa, Vololona, and Michel Callon
2004 Patients and Scientists in French Muscular Dystrophy Research. In States of
Knowledge: The Co-Production of Science and the Social Order. Sheila Jasanoff,
ed. Pp. 142–160. New York, NY: Routledge.
Rabeharisoa, Vololona, Tiago Moreira, and Madeleine Akrich
2014 Evidence-Based Activism: Patients’, Users’ and Activists’ Groups in Knowledge
Society. BioSocieties 9(2):111–128.
Rabeharisoa, Vololona, Tiago Moreira, Madeleine Akrich, and others
2013 Evidence-Based Activism: Patients’ Organisations, Users’ and Activist’s Groups
in Knowledge Society. http://halshs.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00822864/, accessed
November 11, 2014.
Rabinow, Paul
1999 Artificiality and Enlightenment: From Sociobiology to Biosociality. In The
Science Studies Reader. Mario Biagioli, ed. Pp. 407–416. Princeton: Princeton
University Press.
Race, Kane
2012 “Frequent Sipping”: Bottled Water, the Will to Health and the Subject of
Hydration. Body & Society 18(3-4):72–98.
Ramachandran, Vilayanur S., and Lindsay M. Oberman
2006 Broken Mirrors: A Theory of Autism. Scientific American 295(5):62–69.
Raman, S., and R. Tutton
2010 Life, Science, and Biopower. Science, Technology & Human Values 35(5):711–
734.
Ramirez, Michelle, Andrea Altschuler, Carmit McMullen, et al.
2014 “I Didn’t Feel Like I Was a Person Anymore”: Realigning Full Adult Personhood
after Ostomy Surgery. Medical Anthropology Quarterly.
Rapp, Rayna
2000a Testing Women, Testing the Fetus: The Social Impact of Amniocentesis in
America. New York: Routledge.
2000b Extra Chromosomes and Blue Tulips: Medico-Familial Interpretations. In Living
and Working with the New Medical Technologies: Intersections of Inquiry.
468
Margaret M Lock, Allan Young, and Alberto Cambrosio, eds. Pp. 184–208.
Cambridge Studies in Medical Anthropology, 8. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
University Press.
2003 Cell Life and Death, Child Life and Death. In Remaking Life & Death: Toward an
Anthropology of the Biosciences. Sarah Franklin and Margaret M Lock, eds. Pp.
129–164. School of American Research Advanced Seminar Series. Santa Fe:
School of American Research Press.
Raspberry, K., and D. Skinner
2007 Experiencing the Genetic Body: Parents’ Encounters with Pediatric Clinical
Genetics. Medical Anthropology 26(4):355–391.
2010 Enacting Genetic Responsibility: Experiences of Mothers Who Carry the Fragile
X Gene. Sociology of Health & Illness 33(3):1–14.
Ravanelli, Alice
2013 L’autismo Nella Lettaratura Italiana Del Nuovo Millennio. Tesi, Università
Cattolica Sacre Cuore, Milano, Italia.
Raven, Jean
2003 Raven Progressive Matrices. In Handbook of Nonverbal Assessment Pp. 223–
237. Springer. http://link.springer.com/chapter/10.1007/978-1-4615-0153-4_11,
accessed May 5, 2014.
Reubi, David
2010 Blood Donors, Development and Modernisation: Configurations of Biological
Sociality and Citizenship in Post-Colonial Singapore. Citizenship Studies
14(5):473–493.
Rheinberger, Hans-Jorg
2000 Beyond Nature and Culture: Modes of Reasoning in the Age of Molecular
Biology and Medicine. In Living and Working with the New Medical
Technologies: Intersections of Inquiry. Margaret M Lock, Allan Young, and
Alberto Cambrosio, eds. Pp. 19–30. Cambridge Studies in Medical Anthropology,
8. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Rhodes, Tim, Magdalena Harris, and Anthea Martin
2013 Negotiating Access to Medical Treatment and the Making of Patient Citizenship:
The Case of Hepatitis C Treatment. Sociology of Health & Illness 35(7):1023–
1044.
Rios, Clarice, and Barbara Costa Andrade
in press
The Changing Face of Autism in Brazil. Culture, Medicine and
Psychiatry.
Rizzolatti, Giacomo, and Maddalena Fabbri-Destro
2010 Mirror Neurons: From Discovery to Autism. Experimental Brain Research 200(34):223–237.
Roberts, Celia
2007 Messengers of Sex: Hormones, Biomedicine and Feminism. Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Roberts, Elizabeth F. S.
2008 Biology, Sociality and Reproductive Modernity in Ecuadorian inVitro Fertilization: The Particulars of Place. In Biosocialities, Genetics and the
Social Sciences. Sahra Gibbon and Carlos Novas, eds. Pp. 79–97. London:
469
Routledge.
Robins, Steven
2006 From “Rights” to “Ritual”: AIDS Activism in South Africa. American
Anthropologist 108:312–323.
Rogers, Sally J., and Geraldine Dawson
2009 Early Start Denver Model for Young Children with Autism: Promoting Language,
Learning, and Engagement. 1 edition. New York: The Guilford Press.
Romain, Tiffany
2010 Extreme Life Extension: Investing in Cryonics for the Long, Long Term. Medical
Anthropology 29(2):194–215.
Romano, Maria Francesca, and Michela Natilli
2011 I Giovani in Toscana. Vite E Identità. Pacini Editore.
https://www.iris.sssup.it/handle/11382/310364, accessed February 5, 2015.
Rosaldo, Michelle Zimbalist, and Louise Lamphere, eds.
1974 Woman, Culture, and Society. Stanford University Press.
Rose, Nikolas
2003 Neurochemical Selves. Society 41: 46–59.
2007a The Politics of Life Itself. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
2007b Genomic Susceptibility as an Emergent Form of Life? Genetic Testing, Identity,
and the Remit of Medicine. In Biomedicine as Culture: Instrumental Practices,
Technoscientific Knowledge, and New Modes of Life. 1st edition. Regula Valérie
Burri and Joseph Dumit, eds. Pp. 141–150. Routledge.
Rose, Nikolas, and Carlos Novas
2005 Biological Citizenship. In Global Assemblages: Technology, Politics, and Ethics
as Anthropological Problems. Aiwha Ong and Stephen J. Collier, eds. Pp. 439–
463. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.
Rosenberg, Rebecca E., Amy M. Daniels, J. Kiely Law, Paul A. Law, and Walter E.
Kaufmann
2009 Trends in Autism Spectrum Disorder Diagnoses: 1994–2007. Journal of Autism
and Developmental Disorders 39(8):1099–1111.
Rubel, Arthur J., and Michael R. Hass
1996 Ethnomedicine. In Medical Anthropology: Contemporary Theory and Method.
Rev. ed. Carolyn Fishel Sargent and Thomas M Johnson, eds. Pp. 113–130.
Westport, Conn: Praeger.
Ruef, Michael B., and Ann P. Turnbull
2002 The Perspective of Individuals with Cognitive Disabilities And/or Autism on
Their Lives and Their Problem Behavior. Research & Practice for Persons with
Severe Disabilities 27:125–140.
Russo, Concetta, Michele Capararo, and Enrico Valtellina
2014 A sé e agli altri. Storia della manicomializzazione dell’autismo e delle altre
disabilità relazionali nelle cartelle cliniche di S. Servolo. 1. edizione. Milano etc.:
Mimesis.
Ryan, S., and K. R. Cole
2009 From Advocate to Activist? Mapping the Experiences of Mothers of Children on
the Autism Spectrum. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities
22(1):43–53.
470
Sacks, Oliver
1995 An Anthropologist on Mars: Seven Paradoxical Tales. Vintage Books.
1998 The Man Who Mistook His Wife For A Hat: And Other Clinical Tales. Simon
and Schuster.
Salvador-Carulla, L., G. Tibaldi, S. Johnson, et al.
2005 Patterns of Mental Health Service Utilisation in Italy and Spain. Social Psychiatry
and Psychiatric Epidemiology 40(2):149–159.
Santone, G., G. de Girolamo, I. Falloon, et al.
2005 The Process of Care in Residential Facilities. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric
Epidemiology 40(7):540–550.
Saracci, Rodolfo
2011 L’epidemiologia in Centocinquanta Anni Di Italia Unitaria: Appunti per Una
Storia Ancora Da Scrivere. Epidemiologia & Prevenzione 35(5-6 Supp. 2):14–17.
Saracci, Rodolfo, Benedetto Terracini, and Franco Merletti
2011 Lo Sviluppo Dell’epidemiologia Moderna in Italia. Epidemiologia & Prevenzione
35(5-6 Supp. 2):18–24.
Sargent, C., and S. Larchanche
2007 The Muslim Body and the Politics of Immigration in France: Popular and
Biomedical Representations of Malian Migrant Women. Body & Society
13(3):79–102.
Sarrett, Jennifer
2015 Custodial Homes, Therapeutic Homes, and Parental Acceptance: Parental
Experiences of Autism in Kerala, India and Atlanta, GA USA. Culture, Medicine
and Psychiatry. DOI: 10.1007/s11013-015-9441-z
Scheper-Hughes, Nancy, and Anne M. Lovell
1986 Breaking the Circuit of Social Control: Lessons in Public Psychiatry from Italy
and Franco Basaglia. Social Science & Medicine 23(2):159–178.
Scheper-Hughes, Nancy
2001 Saints, Scholars, and Schizophrenics: Mental Illness in Rural Ireland. Berkeley:
University of California Press.
Scheper-Hughes, Nancy, and Margaret Lock
1987 The Mindful Body: A Prolegomenon to Future Work in Medical Anthropology.
Medical Anthropology Quarterly 1(1):6–41.
Schlegel, Alice, and Herbert Barry III
1991 Adolescence: An Anthropological Inquiry. New York, NY: Free Press.
Schneider, Arnd
2000 Futures Lost: Nostalgia and Identity among Italian Immigrants in Argentina.
Oxford, UK: P. Lang.
Schopler, Eric, and Robert J. Reichler
1976 Psychoeducational Profile. Chapel Hill, NC: Child Development Products,
Division TEACCH, Dept. of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Health Affairs,
University of North Carolina.
Schopler, Eric, Robert Jay Reichler, and Barbara Rochen Renner
1986 The Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS): For Diagnostic Screening and
Classification of Autism. Irvington New York.
http://www.getcited.org/pub/102448134, accessed May 5, 2014.
471
Schreibman, Laura
2005 The Science and Fiction of Autism. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Seltzer, Marsha Mailick, Leonard Abbeduto, Marty Wyngaarden Krauss, Jan Greenberg,
and April Swe
2004 Comparison Groups in Autism Family Research: Down Syndrome, Fragile X
Syndrome, and Schizophrenia. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders
34(1):41–48.
Senghas, R. J, and L. Monaghan
2002 Signs of Their Times: Deaf Communities and the Culture of Language. Annual
Review of Anthropology: 69–97.
Servizi per Disabili
N.d. COMUNE DI MILANO.
http://www.comune.milano.it/portale/wps/portal/searchresultdetail?WCM_GLOB
AL_CONTEXT=/wps/wcm/connect/ContentLibrary/ho+bisogno+di/ho+bisogno+
di/sostegno+e+assistenza_per+disabili_servizi+per+disabili, accessed January 17,
2015.
Shattuck, Paul T.
2006 The Contribution of Diagnostic Substitution to the Growing Administrative
Prevalence of Autism in US Special Education. Pediatrics 117:1028–1037.
Shearer, M. S., and D. E. Shearer
1972 The Portage Project: A Model for Early Childhood Education. Exceptional
Children 39(3):210–217.
Shorter, Edward
1997 A History of Psychiatry: From the Era of the Asylum to the Age of Prozac. New
York: John Wiley & Sons.
Shtayermman, Oren
2009 An Exploratory Study of the Stigma Associated With a Diagnosis of Asperger’s
Syndrome: The Mental Health Impact on the Adolescents and Young Adults
Diagnosed with a Disability with a Social Nature. Journal of Human Behavior in
the Social Environment 19:293–313.
Silberman, S.
2001 The Geek Syndrome. Wired: 9, 12. The Condé Nast Publications Inc. http://www.
wired. com/wired/archive/9.12/aspergers. html.
Silverman, Chloe
2008a Fieldwork on Another Planet: Social Science Perspectives on the Autism
Spectrum. BioSocieties 3:325–341.
2008b Brains, Pedigrees, and Promises: Lessons from the Politics of Autism Genetics. In
Biosocialities, Genetics and the Social Sciences: Making Biologies and Identities.
Sahra Gibbon and Carlos Novas, eds. Pp. 38–55. London: Routledge.
2012 Understanding Autism: Parents, Doctors, and the History of a Disorder. Princeton,
NJ: Princeton University Press.
Simon, Jonathan
2008 War on! Why A’war on Cancer’should Replace Our’war on Crime’(and Terror).
European Journal of Cultural Studies 11(3):351–369.
Sinclair, Jim
1993 Don’t Mourn for Us. http://www.autreat.com/dont_mourn.html.
472
Sirota, Karen Gainer
2004 Positive Politeness as Discourse Process: Politeness Practices of HighFunctioning Children with Autism and Asperger Syndrome. Discourse Studies
6(2):229–251.
2010 Narratives of Distinction: Personal Life Narrative as a Technology of the Self in
the Everyday Lives and Relational Worlds of Children with Autism. Ethos
38(1):93–115.
Sistema Nazionale per le Linee Guida-Istituto Superiore di Sanità
2011 Il trattamento dei disturbi dello spettro autistico nei bambini e negli adolescenti.
Linea Guida 21. Roma, Italy: Istituto Superiore di Sanità.
2012 Autismo: Quale Trattamento per Bambini E Adolescenti? http://www.snlgiss.it/cms/files/scheda_autismo_14-03.pdf.
Sleeboom-Faulkner, M. E
2011 Genetic Testing, Governance, and the Family in the People’s Republic of China.
Social Science & Medicine 72(11):1802–1809.
Smith, Phil, and Christine Routel
2009 Transition Failure: The Cultural Bias of Self-Determination and the Journey to
Adulthood for People with Disabilities. Disability Studies Quarterly 30(1).
http://dsq-sds.org/article/view/1012/1224, accessed December 26, 2014.
Società Italian di Neuropsichiatria dell’Infanzia e dell’Adolescenza (SINPIA)
N.d. Chi Siamo. www.sinpia.edu. http://www.sinpia.eu/index/index/atom/1, accessed
January 19, 2015.
Sökefeld, Martin
1999 Debating Self, Identity, and Culture in Anthropology. Current Anthropology
40:417–448.
Solomon, Olga
2004 Narrative Introductions: Discourse Competence of Children with Autistic
Spectrum Disorders. Discourse Studies 6(2):253–276.
2008 Language, Autism, and Childhood: An Ethnographic Perspective. Annual Review
of Applied Linguistics 28(1):150–169.
2010 Sense and the Senses: Anthropology and the Study of Autism. Annual Review of
Anthropology 39:241–259.
Solomon, Olga, and Nancy Bagatell
2010 Introduction: Autism: Rethinking the Possibilities. Special Issue: Rethinking
Autism, Rethinking Anthropology. Ethos 38:1–7.
Sommer, Marianne
2010 DNA and Cultures of Remembrance: Anthropological Genetics, Biohistories and
Biosocialities. BioSocieties 5(3):366–390.
Sotgiu, I., D. Galati, M. Manzano, et al.
2011 Parental Attitudes, Attachment Styles, Social Networks, and Psychological
Processes in Autism Spectrum Disorders: A Cross-Cultural Perspective. The
Journal of Genetic Psychology 172(4):353–375.
Soulières, Isabelle, Michelle Dawson, Morton Ann Gernsbacher, and Laurent Mottron
2011 The Level and Nature of Autistic Intelligence II: What about Asperger
Syndrome? PloS One 6(9):e25372.
Sparrow, Sara S., David A. Balla, and Domenic V. Cicchetti
473
1984 Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales: Interview Edition, Survey Form Manual.
American Guidance Service Circle Pines, MN.
http://www.getcited.org/pub/102417475, accessed May 5, 2014.
Spitzer, Robert L.
1997 From Feighner to RDC, DSM-III, DSM-III-R, DSM-IV, and ICD-10. In The
Feighner Criteria: Their Role in Psychiatric Progress: A 25th Anniversary
Review. George Winokur, ed. Pp. 11–13. Secaucus, NJ: Churchill
Communications North America.
Sterponi, Laura
2004 Construction of Rules, Accountability and Moral Identity by High-Functioning
Children with Autism. Discourse Studies 6(2):207–228.
Sterponi, Laura, and Alessandra Fasulo
2010 “How to Go On”: Intersubjectivity and Progressivity in the Communication of a
Child with Autism. Ethos 38(1):116–142.
Storia » Società Italiana Di Psichiatria
N.d. http://www.psichiatria.it/chi-siamo/storia, accessed January 19, 2015.
Suárez-Navaz, Liliana
2005 Rebordering the Mediterranean: Boundaries and Citizenship in Southern Europe.
Berghahn Books.
Sundberg, M L, and J Michael
2001 The Benefits of Skinner’s Analysis of Verbal Behavior for Children with Autism.
Behavior Modification 25(5):698–724.
Symes, Wendy, and Neil Humphrey
2010 Peer-Group Indicators of Social Inclusion among Pupils with Autistic Spectrum
Disorders (ASD) in Mainstream Secondary Schools: A Comparative Study.
School Psychology International 31(5):478–494.
Tanassi, Lucia M
2004 Compliance as Strategy: The Importance of Personalised Relations in Obstetric
Practice. Social Science and Medicine 59(10):2053–2069.
Taussig, Karen-Sue, Rayna Rapp, and Deborah Heath
2003 Flexible Eugenics: Technologies of the Self in the Age of Genetics. In Genetic
Nature/culture: Anthropology and Science Beyond the Two-Culture Divide. Alan
H. Goodman, Deborah Heath, and M. Susan Lindee, eds. University of California
Press.
Thompson, James Richard
2004 Supports Intensity Scale: Users Manual. American Association on Mental
Retardation.
Thompson, Travis
2007 Making Sense of Autism. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing.
Tissot, Catherine
2009 Establishing a Sexual Identity: Case Studies of Learners with Autism and
Learning Difficulties. Autism 13: 551–566.
Townsend, John Marshall
1978 Cultural Conceptions and Mental Illness: A Comparison of Germany and
America. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Treccani, l’Enciclopedia Italiana.
474
N.d.
Scuola Media. http://www.treccani.it/enciclopedia/scuola-media/, accessed
February 15, 2015.
Twachtman-Cullen, Diane, and Jennifer Twachtman-Reilly
2007 Communication and Language Issues in Less Able School-Age Children with
Autism. In Growing Up with Autism: Working with School-Age Children and
Adolescents Pp. 73–94. New York: NY: The Guilford Press.
UNESCO-IBE
2010 Italy. World Data on Education, VII Ed. Paris, France: UNESCO-IBE.
University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill School of Medicine
2014 TEACCH.com. teacch.com.
Valentine, Kylie
2010 A Consideration of Medicalisation: Choice, Engagement and Other
Responsibilities of Parents of Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Social
Science & Medicine 71(5):950–957.
Valtellina, Enrico
2011 Metodologie Emancipative Nella Ricerca Sulla Neurodiversità. Note Sul
Fieldwork Su Marte. Italian Journal of Disability Studies - Rivista Italiana Di
Studi Sulla Disabilità 1:31–38.
2015 Tipi umani particolarmente strani: La sindrome di Asperger come oggetto
culturale. Università Ca’Foscari Venezia.
Van Der Geest, S., and K. Finkler
2004 Hospital Ethnography: Introduction. Social Science & Medicine 59(10):1995–
2001.
Van Moffaert, M., and A. Vereecken
1989 Somatization of Psychiatric Illness in Mediterranean Migrants in Belgium.
Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry 13(3): 297–313.
Verhoeff, Berend
2012 What Is This Thing Called Autism? A Critical Analysis of the Tenacious Search
for Autism’s Essence. BioSocieties 7(4):410–432.
Vermeulen, Eric
2004 Dealing with Doubt: Making Decisions in a Neonatal Ward in The Netherlands.
Social Science and Medicine 59(10):2071–2085.
Vestel, V.
2009 Limits of Hybridity versus Limits of Tradition?: A Semiotics of Cultural
Reproduction, Creativity, and Ambivalence among Multicultural Youth in
Rudenga, East Side Oslo. Ethos 37(4):466–488.
Vogel, Gila, and Shunit Reiter
2003 Spiritual Dimensions of Bar/Bat Mitzvah Ceremonies for Jewish Children with
Developmental Disabilities. Education and Training in Developmental
Disabilities 38:314–322.
2004 Significance of the Bar/Bat Mitzvah Ceremony for Parents of Jewish Children
with Developmental Disabilities. Mental Retardation 42:294–303.
Vogel, Morris J, and Charles E Rosenberg, eds.
1979 The Therapeutic Revolution; Essays in the Social History of American Medicine.
Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press.
Vrecko, Scott
475
2008 Capital Ventures into Biology: Biosocial Dynamics in the Industry and Science of
Gambling. Economy and Society 37(1):50–67.
Waltz, Mitzi
2003 Metaphors of Autism, and Autism as Metaphor: An Exploration of
Representation. In Second Global Conference. Inter-Disciplinary. Net.
https://www.inter-disciplinary.net/ptb/mso/hid/hid2/waltz%20paper.pdf, accessed
August 29, 2014.
Wazana, Ashley, Michaeline Bresnahan, and Jennie Kline
2007 The Autism Epidemic: Fact or Artifact? Journal of the American Academy of
Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 46:721–730.
Wechsler, David
2008 Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Fourth Edition (WAIS–IV). San Antonio, TX:
NCS Pearson. http://www.statisticssolutions.com/academicsolutions/resources/directory-of-survey-instruments/wechsler-adult-intelligencescale-fourth-edition-wais-iv/, accessed May 5, 2014.
Wehling, P.
2011 The “technoscientization” of Medicine and Its Limits: Technoscientific Identities,
Biosocialities, and Rare Disease Patient Organizations. Poiesis & Praxis:
International Journal of Technology Assessment and Ethics of Science: 1–16.
Weisner, T. S., and E. D. Lowe
2005 Globalization, Childhood, and Psychological Anthropology. In A Companion to
Psychological Anthropology Pp. 315–336.
Weiss, Robert
1995 Learning from Strangers: The Art and Method of Qualitative Interview Studies.
New York: The Free Press.
Whiting, John, V. K. Burbank, and M. S. Ratner
1994 The Duration of Maidenhood Across Cultures. In Culture and Human
Development: The Selected Papers of John Whiting Pp. 282–305. Cambridge,
UK: Cambridge University Press.
Whitington, Jerome
2013 Fingerprint, Bellwether, Model Event: Climate Change as Speculative
Anthropology. Anthropological Theory 13(4):308–328.
Whyte, Susan Reynolds
2009 Health Identities and Subjectivities: The Ethnographic Challenge. Medical
Anthropology Quarterly 32:6–15.
Wilhelm-Solomon, Matthew
2013 The Priest’s Soldiers: HIV Therapies, Health Identities, and Forced Encampment
in Northern Uganda. Medical Anthropology 32(3):227–246.
Willems, Dick
1998 Inhaling Drugs and Making Worlds: A Proliferation of Lungs and Asthmas. In
Differences in Medicine: Unraveling Practices, Techniques, and Bodies. Marc
Berg and Annemarie Mol, eds. Pp. 105–118. Body, Commodity, Text. Durham:
Duke University Press.
Wing, Lorna
1981 Asperger’s Syndrome: A Clinical Account. Psychological Medicine 11(1):115–
129.
476
Winokur, George, ed.
1997 The Feighner Criteria: Their Role in Psychiatric Progress: A 25th Anniversary
Review. Secaucus, NJ: Churchill Communications North America.
World Health Organization
2008 International Classification of Diseases (ICD). World Health Organization.
http://apps.who.int/classifications/icd10/browse/2010/en.
World Health Organization Working Group on Changing Patterns in Mental Health
1980 Changing Patterns in Mental Health Care: Report on a WHO Working Group,
Cologne, 27 November-1 December 1978. Regional Office for Europe, World
Health Organization.
Wright, Scott D., Denise E. Brooks, Valerie DAstous, and Temple Grandin
2013 The Challenge and Promise of Autism Spectrum Disorders in Adulthood and
Aging: A Systematic Review of the Literature (1990-2013). Autism Insights(5).
Yamaguchi, S.
2004 Nursing Culture of an Operating Theater in Italy. Nursing & Health Sciences
6(4):261–269.
Yan, Y.
1999 Rural Youth and Youth Culture in North China. Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry
23(1):75–97.
Young, Allan
1995 The Harmony of Illusions: Inventing Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder. Princeton,
NJ: Princeton University Press.
Fly UP