Comments
Description
Transcript
Towards an explication of
Towards an explication of the role ofchild language in linguistic change STEPHEN SAMPLE Swarthmore College. Swarthmore. Pennsylvania 19081 ssam pIe [email protected] Thesis. Linguistics 180 Abstract The acquisition of language is discussed in general. with particular consideration to the acquisition of morphology in English. Child word creation is described. and the acquisition of various word creation mechanisms is described. Parallels are drawn between historical changes in English and changes and processes seen in child language. in an attempt to clarify the oft-assumed connection between child language and language change. Introduction The study of linguistics is in some sense necessarily the study of how children acquire language. Why may not be immediately obvious: there is no clear connection between. say. metrical stress theory and language acquisition; however. the linguist and the child are confronted with essemially the same problem: to idemify and describe a linguistic system from scratch. when that system could in principle be anything (Wanner & Gleitman 1982b. 4). The connection between the two statemems of the problem is not always clear. however. In some cases psychological motivation has been found for the connection, but for many areas of linguistics the acquisition question has been simply allowed to stand. In the case of diachronic linguistics, it has been assumed that language change is motivated by children, or at least that the mechanisms by which children build and remodel their internal lexica and grammars are also used to remodel the collective lexicon and grammar of the linguistic community. Many plausible arguments have been put forward on the spur of the moment for this assumption, but the relationship between child language and changes in the adult language has never been investigated in great detail. I am more interested in lexical change than in usage change (or perhaps I just imagine a clearer connection with child language for such changes). so I propose to look at changes in vocabulary and word Towards an explication ofthe rOle ofchild language in language change Page 1 usage over time. and draw connections between those changes and processes of acquisition and word generation by children. To that end. I will be attempting to characterize child coinages morphologically. so that I can associate specific instances of language change with specific (approximate) developmental stages. I should note. however. that I am entirely ignoring any possible relations between child phonok~ and historico-phonological change, in part because child phonology has already been investigated quite thoroughly. and I thus forsee fewer opportunities for new insight, and in part because phonology really doesn't interest me all that much. Theory Overview of language acquisition The question of how children acquire language is logically prior to the question of how their acquisition and production processes stimulate language change in the adult community. so we need to establish at least a primitive understanding of those processes before we can continue. We also need to have at least a primitive understanding of diachronic linguistics, but we can't discuss everything at once. Accordingly. I'll start out with the general theory of language acquisition , and work from there toward an explanation of the detailed processes by which children build and alter their lexicon and grammar. The acquisition of syntax With this eventual goal in mind, we can ask: how do children acquire and generate language? They clearly do so. and with minimal feedback, but the early explanations of how they do so all fell short. Accordingly, in the venerable scientific tradition of ignoring a problem which you can't solve in favor of a simpler (and perhaps less interesting) one which you can, developmental psycholinguistics during the 'sixties switched to characterizing child language rather than describing the processes by which a child attains adult language proficiency (Pinker 1984, 2). Unfortunately, the computational algorithms proposed proved too powerful and flexible for this more restricted task, and a new problem surfaced: all the theoretical cognitive processes for inducing a language overgenerated fairly drastically (Pinker 1984. 1-4). However, indeterminacy problems are familiar in science: if we know anything about the object of interest which the theory does not directly address (in this case, child language"as an intermediate stage of language acquisition), we may be able to come up with some external constraints which will enable us to reach a unique solution (there are lots of nifty mathematical techniques for doing this formally, but none of them are of immediate interest here). If we rerurn to the question oflanguage acquisition rather than child language, we find just such a constraint. since child language must be an intermediate stage of the acquisition mechanism that eventually gets us to adult language (Pinker 1984, 5). The problem with this constraint is that it ignores all the other differences between children and adults: attention span, memory. cognitive abilities, perceptivity, etc. (Pinker 1984,6). By Occam's razor, the best theory is that which· Towards an explication ofthe role ofchild language in language change Page 2 invokes the fewest computational mechanisms, and thus the fewest qualitative developmental changes in any of these mechanisms (Pinker 1984, 7). This is not to say that acquisition of rules and data cannot feed the language acquisition mechanisms, but only that the mechanisms themselves are time-invariant. There are several possible alternatives to this assumption: neural maturation could cause qualitative changes in the cognitive mechanisms of language, or, more poetically, the child could simultaneously learn abour the world and build the mind with which the world is comprehended. However, the assumption that the set of acquisition mechanisms is fixed is more compact and thus preferable in the absence of contradictory data. One of the solutions to the question of how children acquire language has traditionally been mat they come preprogrammed with a fully developed Universal Grammar (UG) and only add words and set parameters of the panicular language they are learning (Carroll 1986, 307). This assumption can lead to some logical problems. in that it implies that, f.g, holophrastic utterances have the same syntax as adult sentences (Carroll 1986, 327). This leaves us unable to explain the initially fixed word-order of Slavicspeaking children (Carroll 1986, 333) or the syntacto-semantic organization of early one- and two-word sentences. In the case of two-word sentences, the assumption that the two words embed all of the syntactic relations of the adult paraphrase suggests that the child's sentence is in tact more complex than the adult sentence, since the two-word sentence needs additional rules to keep most of the words out of the phonology. However, one could also assume that children do not come preprogrammed with a UG, but that they are equipped to learn something comparably general and complex. I I will presume a grammar as an acquisition subject because it obviates the need for the child to acquire different mechanisms for interpretation, production, and judgment (the mechanisms for these three processes, at least under me computational models I have seen, each (redundantly) specifY the structure of grammatical sentences), and, as such, reduces computational overhead. Grammars also have me advantage of allowing a clearer distinction between those data and operations which must be acquired from the input and those which need not be (Pinker 1984, 22). At this point, however, a clear distinction should be made between the restriction proposed earlier that there be few changes in the child's language-processing mechanisms (although some may become more widely used with time as more resources become available to them: improvements in the child's accessible corpus and processing resources can feed the acquisition mechanisms) and having few changes in the child's grammar. Proposing a staric set of acquisition resources which are used as needed and proposing a static grammar are not equivalent, and indeed I agree with me former (if only as a necessary simplification for the model) and not with the latter (which seems to be in disagreement with much of the available data). But what is this input from which children deduce the rules oflanguage? Towards an fxplication ofthf role ofchild language in language change Page 3 Children undoubtedly learn a language at least in pan by hearing sentences in that language from their parents and peers. but the available evidence suggests that not all sentences. or not all the pans of a sentence. are used as input. Children seem to be most likely to encode those sentences in which they understand most of the words. and those pans of sentences which are stressed or near the edges (Pinker 1984. 28). The processing of those segments which are encoded is not necessarily either straightforward or fully justified. however. "Young children are inclined to draw grand inductive generalizations even over noisy data.» (Wanner & Gleitman 1982b. 7) This sort of overgeneralization is common. both lexically (which will be discussed later) and grammatically; as an example. children use both strong and weak past tense forms at 2 and 3. but by around 4 they only use weak ones (Wanner & Gleitman 1982b, 7). How do children make judgements about which sentences are ungrammatical? They cannot do so based on positive evidence because they will not be presented with examples of ungrammatical sentences by their caretakers; they do not do so based on negative evidence because such evidence is invariably ignored by children even when present (Pinker 1984. 28). This ignoring of parental correction is advisable because such corrections are. when available, contingent upon neither ungrammaticality nor syntactic illformedness (Pinker 1984. 29), but on any of several criteria which are distributed widely across the fields of adult linguistics.2 However. when generalizing from the input corpus. children consistently draw the right generalizations. or nearly the right ones. from the language corpus before them (Wanner & Gleitman 1982b. 6-7) If we suppose that the inpur for a child's language-processing mechanisms is a string of words (or, at early stages. a string of phonemes). then we run into some problems. such as explaining how. even presuming children to be preprogrammed with a fully developed UG, mappings from the surface (SS) to the deep structure (DS) are established, given that such mappings are neither constant nor predictable functions of the SS. The solurion to this problem is to propose that the input includes far more than simple word strings, that utterance meaning is a part of the raw material for the acquisition (Pinker 1984, 29). This more complex input. of course. requires that children be able to infer meaning of adult utterances from physical and discourse contexts. That children are able to do so is supported by characteristics ofMotherese (Carroll 1986. 308. 328-29. 337-38), although adult speech to children is not uniform in this respect. and thus semantic bootstrapping must still be possible (albeit presumably more difficult) with little or no adult cooperation. Semantic bootstrapping refers to the fact that children apparently acquire syntactic relations as semantic ones (organizing sentences in terms of a-roles rather than GFs) (Carroll 1986. 332, 350). and then induce the syntax at a later point. That is. rather than interpreting a simple sentence as. e.g. NVN. children under the age of four will interpret it as Agent Action Patient (Carroll 1986. 350). Questions of how children map the surface structure of received sentences to the deep structure can thus be put off. and Towards an explication ofthe role ofchild language in language change Page 4 statements that children speak deep structure (MacNeill 1966b) can be avoided entirely, along with the embarrassment that they no doubt cause. The advantages to having children acquire meanings along with word strings are quite impressive: it allows us to avoid proposing that that children have adult grammars at birth, when many of their words and sentences, while consistently (and thus, presumably correctly) formed, are not well-formed by adult standards; it allows us to explain the acquisition and modification of word meanings, which will be discussed in more detail later; and it allows us to explain the apparent semantic bootstrapping of the grammar by young children. The acquisition of morphemes and lexemes While the acquisition of grammar is not eighty bound to the acquisition of morphology or lexical items,3 it presents many of the same problems, and has been investigated in much more detail. If we are to explain word creation from a developmental standpoint, however. we need to explain both lexical organization and morphology. Lexical organization The theory I am assuming is that lexemes are stored initially as unanalyzed units, and that space constraints prompt reanalysis (see later discussion) of complex lexemes as a set of morphemes, which are stored independently, and the initiallexemes subsequently reproduced by morphological rules (Bybee 1985. 114). Irregular forms, of course. retain their independent lexical entries (Bybee 1985, 113). The organization of the lexicon is being modeled by two parameters. lexical strength and lexical connections (Bybee 1985, 116). such that lexical strength is proportional to the frequency of access (Bybee 1985. 116), and lexical connections exist between words with morphological similarity (Bybee 1985. 118). that is. with both semantic and phonological similarity. There are interactions between the two: lexically strong words can form more distant connections than weaker words can (Bybee 1985, 118). and large numbers of connections can strengthen lexical entries (Bybee 1985, 134). There are useful similarities between the lexical and morphological acquisition theories: lexical strength models rote learning (Bybee 1985. 119), and can account for, e.g.• why wend and wentwere separated but spendand spent. or sendand sent. were not (Bybee 1985. 122). Lexical strength can result in limited productivity. as in the case of the strung schema for past tense in English. which is attracting members form other paradigms (Bybee 1985, 130). This productivity is related to type frequency rather than token frequency, however (Bybee 1985, 133), so the lower-strength words within a paradigm thus contribute most to its productivity (Bybee 1985. 134). This lexical organization theory is primarily morphological. but it corresponds well with both the psycholinguistic and acquisition theories. One of the popular models from language acquisition suggests Towards an explication ofthe role ofchild language in language change Page 5 that words are acquired and stored as bundles of semantic features (Carroll 1986. chapter 5. passim). although there certainly questions which this-approach does not address. The basic thrust of the theory makes sense. however: children cannot be expected to acquire full adult definitions upon a single hearing. and. indeed. adults frequently differ in their definitions for uncommon terms depending on the context which they learned them. Children also are frequently observed to use words much more broadly than adults do. which in itself is not an argument for featural theories. except that such theories are often more successful in explainingthe ways in which children overextend their words. The nature of the features on which early word meanings are based is rather uncle-ar. or at least controversial. Clark has suggested that early words are based upon their perceptual features. and that in the case of overextensions. the word has simply been based upon too few of the features of the prototype. and that the salient features for extension are physical. t.g. shape and colour (Carroll 1986. 324). Nelson has claimed that early words are based upon the functional characteristics of their prototypes (Carroll 1986. 324). at least among children from 1;0 to 1;3. Gentner [1978] found that 2;6-5;0 children and adults responded to objects based upon physical attributes rather than functional ones, while intermediate-aged children did the reverse (Carroll 1986. 325). As usual. the solution to this controversy is somewhere in the middle. and multiple attribute categories (e.g. perceptual, functional. and affective similariry. and contextual association features) all playa role in a child's early understanding of words (Carroll 1986. 325). Morphological acquisition theory The question of how and when children acquire morphological structures and operations does not seem to have been investigated with the level of attention which semantic and syntactic development have received. The two main theoretical studies (as opposed to studies of children's acquisition of specific grammatical morphemes) which I have made use of are MacWhinney (1978) and Roeper (1981). Since both studies attempt a generalal description. they do not go into as much language-specific detail as I might wish. but they provide a good framework for talking about the acquisition of word creation strategies in English. MacWhinney (1978) proposes that acquisition of morphology makes use of only three mechanisms: rote. combination. and analogy. and characterizes both the correct and erroneous function of those mechanisms. Rote is ultimately a nonanalytic process which preserves any and all irregularities in the input (MacWhinney 1978, 1). Rote is the earliest stage of morphological processing (if indeed it can be called processing), and usually begins to fade out in English by around the age of 3. Errors in rote morphology are randomly distributed. because it applies before the child has recognized any patterns in the input Towards an txplication oftht rolt ofchild languagt in languagt change Pagt6 corpus. and there are no d priori grounds for supposing' any particular form for a given morpheme (since. e.g. if the plural morpheme is not known. banana would seem as likely a plural of foot as are fiet or foots). Combination shows up rather later in the acquisition process: when the input corpus and the child's suite of computational resources have grown large enough. the child is able to consider how units (words) are built from smaller units (morphemes)(MacWhinney 1978, 3). This is partly driven by storage restrictions on the lexicon and partly by the greater efficiency of computations using more general rules. The extent of the role for combination depends heavily on the theoretical model of the grammar and the acquisition strategy: generative models focus heavily on combination. and performance-based models much less so (MacWhinney 1978, 3). It is however somewhat unclear how allomorphs are picked and identified under combination in MacWhinney's theory. In the working theory of Derwing & Baker (1979) allomorphs would be coidentified based on phonetic and semantic similarity. It is suggested. however, that children can coidentify morphemes based on a single point of similarity. which I would expect to cause problems for a computational model of combination. Analogy is proposed as the third mechanism of morphology acquisition because errors in acrual child speech are not randomly distributed: forms based upon regular patterns are seldom produced incorrectly. but forms based upon irregular patterns often are (MacWhinney 1978. 1-2). The data from studies of specific languages (OK. I only have hard data on English) suggest further that the reliability of production is. directly proportional to the productivity of the paradigm (Derwing & Baker 1979. passim). Analogy is strongly emphasized by linguistic structuralists. without any plausible mechanism by which it should operate (MacWhinney 1978. 2). It is also proposed as a full-fledged word creation mechanism in the historical-linguistic literature (Hock 1986. ???). although I have rejected it as a mechanism for other reasons (see also the word creation section for further discussion). Brian MacWhinney's model is. of course. niore complicated than a list of three mechanisms. but. since he is trying to be general. not by much. He presents a list of claims for the acquisition process which. he maintains. account fully for the acquisition of morphophonology. 1. The three mechanisms are used in order: combination is only used if rote fails; analogy is only used if both rote and combination fail (MacWhinney 1978, 6). 2. In combination and analogy, words or roots are retrieved, and any necessary affixes are added. If the word or root appears to contain the affix already. the affix is blocked from applying (MacWhinney 1978, 7). There is a self-checking process on the phonology of the input. the output. and the lexical form which flags utterances for correction in the case .of misapplication of any morphological rules. 3. Children don't learn in the short term from overt corrections by adults (MacWhinney 1978. 8) 4. Frequency of irregular forms is inversely related ro the likelihood of their overregularization (MacWhinney 1978,9), and they are more likely to be overgeneralized when used out of context (MacWhinney 1978,20) 5. Word acquisition is driven by lexical gaps (MacWhinney 1978, 10). 6. Early forms match up with input units which are separately packaged by intonation (MacWhinney 1978,10). Towards an explication ofthe rOle ofchild language in language change Pagel 7. The first productive uses and overgeneralizations of a root will use the uninflected (citation) formi if the citation form is unavailable. the first overgeneralizations will be of the most common allomorph (MacWhinney 1978. 11) 8. AffIxes are first acquired as applying to only a small lexical domain (MacWhinney 1978. 20). 9. Children avoid acquiring discontinuous morphemes. Such morphemes will be acquired in chunks. and the fir. chunk acquired will be that with the most predictable allomorphs (MacWhinney 1978.20). German children first use the auxilliary to express the progressive. English children the -ing suffIx (MacWhinney 1978. 82) 10. The first productive uses and first overgeneralizations of an affix will make use of the most frequent form (MacWhinney 1978.20). 11. The productivity of a rule is directly related to the number of 'disequilibrated pairs' (conflicts between a derived form and the corresponding rote form) which led to its generalization. The first rules used will be the most productive (MacWhinney 1978, 20). 12. Productive rules which only occasionally lead to productive errors will be retained but weakened wich each successive error (MacWhinney 1978.20). 13. Productive rules that have many exceptions will only be generalized if they are seen (MacWhinney 1978,20). to apply in the input 14. Children who can use affix informacion to judge the class membership of a root will also be able to use information not directly afflXed (e.g. the form of DET. etc.) (MacWhinney 1978, 20) 1S. When an allomorphic variation cannot be accounted for by a productive rule. children first overgeneralize the strongest allomorph. and later, the weaker allomorphs (MacWhinney 1978.20). e.g. use of German past tense in the presesnc, and use of~om for -oy in Russian (MacWhinney 1978. 83)? 16. Morphophonological patters which correspond with natural phonological predispositions will be acquired early and will seldom lead to errors (MacWhinney 1978. 20). The one weakness of MacWhinney's theory from my viewpoint is its very generality: because it is trying to describe the acquisition of morphophonology for any language, it lacks sufficient specific..~ for any, either in terms of the acquisition of particular morphemes or mechanisms. or in terms of ages of acquisition. Fortunately, however. others have found most of those details for English, and so I can use their results. Thomas Roeper's study of deductive acquisition of morphology proceeds along rather different lines: he looks at the subcategorization of complex verbs, and considers how it can be induced from positive evidence only (1981, 129-130). His simplest suggestion, which he quickly dismantles. is that a derived word has the same subcategorization frame as its parent (Roeper 1981. 131). He later suggests the following contstraints on the production of morphologically complex verb: they are only produced by productive morphological processes. and have valence of two or less (Roeper 1981, 135), only optional subcategorizations are inheirited (Roeper 1981, 139-40), and all subcategorizations are assumed to be obligatory unless proved otherwise (Roeper 1981. 140). Since Roeper's study is more focused on the syntax of what morphological operations are allowed and when than on the operations themselves, it is not Towards an explication ofthe role ofchild language in language change Page 8 immediately useful for a study of the acquisition of word creation. although it might well prove useful for a followup in which the environments for word creation were investigated in more detail. Morphological acquisition experiments On a more concrete and immediate level. many studies have been done investigating the order of acquisition for specific grammatical morphemes in English. and attempting co relate that order to various presumed measures of morphological complexity. The measures of complexity are The seminal experiment in the acquisition of morphology of English was Jean Berko {later Jean Berko Gleason)'s experiment with grade school children. wherein they were asked to apply derivational and inflectional morphemes to nonsense srems (the most famous being wug. one of which is picrured below). The children's responses not only showed the completeness which the morphemes had been acquired, but also showed the acquisition of allomorphy rules (e.g. Isl->[z]/[-affricate. +voice]--.1). Gleason's studies have since been modified and expanded to include more morphemes and additional bells and whistles. e.g. by Innes (Derwing & Baker 1979. 211-2), and the later studies will be the ones cited. for compactness. Derwing & Baker attempted to describe the complexity of morphological operations such as pluralization and aspectual changes by r~lating it to the proponion of correctly formed tokens using that operation and the age at which the operation is acquired. Their results for 112 children between 3 and 9 are (Derwing & Baker 1979. 212). morpheme correct roken percentage progressive 83.3 plural 78.0 past tense 76.6 possessive 75.2 present tensel 3d person sg 71.5 When class conversions were investigated. Derwing & Baker found the following rates of correct modification of nonsense stems (Derwing & Baker 1979. 215). construction Preschool Early Middle Late Adult agentive -er 7 63 80 86 96 Towards an explication ofthe role ofchild language in language change Page 9 N+N compound 47 50 65 79 70 adjectival -y 0 30 55 86 100 intrumental -er 7 35 4S 64 59 adverbial -Iy 0 13 20 79 81 diminutive -y 7 5 10 14 33 They suggest that the late acquisition of instrumental -er may be due in part to competition with nullconversion. which is highly productive in the instrumental. This hypothesis is supported by the fact that almost all error responses to the instrumental presentations were of the bare stem. Similarly. N+N compounding, which is very successful in preschool but never becomes any more so. was apparently suffering among the older respondants from competition with the possessive (N's N) construction. which they admit was a proper response to their input data. The researchers assume responsibility for the low success rate of the diminutive. due to procedural errors in the elicitation. (Derwing & Baker 1979. 215-6). The final test by Derwing and Baker was to construct a list of 50 word pairs which they categorized by semantic and phonological similarity. and ask whether the pair was morphologicaJly related (specifically. the question was about etymology) and whether the relation had ever occurred to them before (217-8). Clearly. questions of the development of morphological awareness are important for the development of reanalyrical processes such as backformation (see the discussion of word creation for a description) as well as for simpler operations such as agentive -tr affixation. In the study, adults only gave strong positive responses for word pairs with a semantic similarity of ~ or greater and a phonological similarity of 1.75 or greater on a scale of 0-4 (Derwing & Baker 1979. 218). Children. by contrast. gave strong positive responses when tithtr dimension showed a similarity greater than 3.5, as well as when both dimensions showed somewhat lower similarities (Derwing & Baker 1979. 221). A few examples ran afoul of specific etymological knowledge (as witnessed by jumps of 1 or greater in the mean confidence rating between one group and the next. interpreted as the result of specific instruction about the etymology of the pair (e.g. that holiday comes from holy) (Derwing & Baker 1979. 221). Further work in the acquisition and complexity of grammatical morphemes was done by de Villiers & de Villiers (1978). They worked with more (14) morphemes. and attempted to relate their data about the acquisition order to the morphological complexity of the morphemes. Order of acquisition of 14 grammatical morphemes (de Villiers 1978, 78) morpheme Average rank-ordering Rank-ordering for the for the children in Brown children in de Villiers method 1 Towards an txp/ication oftht ro/t ofchild languagt in language changt method 2 Pagt 10 present progressive 2 4 on 2.5 2 2 10 2.5 4 1 plural 4 2 3 past irregular 5 ) 5 possessive 6 7 11 uncontraaible copula 7 12 10 articles 8 6 8 past regular 9 10.5 7 3rd person regular 10 10.5 12 3rd person irregular 11 8.5 6 uncontractible auxilliary 12 14 14 contractible copula 13 8.5 9 contractible auxilliary 14 13 13 method 1 involved ranking the morphemes according co the lowest MLU sample in which the morpheme occurred in 90+% of the obligatory contexts (de Villiers 1978, 76), and method 2 involved summing the percentages across all the children and averaging, and ranking the morphemes by the average accurate-use percentage. ordering of 13 of the morphemes according to the number of transformations in the derivation of each morpheme4 (de Villiers 1978. 80) morpheme Transformations complexity ranking present progressive Progressive affix 3 10 Preposition segment 3 on preposition segment 3 articles Article 3 past irregular verbal agreement 3 past regular verbal agreement. verb suffix 7 plural noun suffix, article or nominal agreement 7 3rd person irregular aux agreement, verbal agreement 7 3rd person regular aux agreement, verbal agreement. verb suffix 10 contraaible copula copula, aux agreement. aux incorporation 10 uncontraaible copula copula. aux agreement, aux incorporation 10 contractible auxill iary progressive affix, progressive segment Towards an explication of the role ofchild language in language change 12.5 Page 11 uncontractible auxilliary aux agreement, aux incorporation 12.5 13 morphemes from child speech, ordered according to number of unitary meanings expressed or presupposed (de Vtlliers 1978. 82) major meanings complexity ranking temporary duration 4 in containment 4 on suppon 4 plural number 4 past, irregular earliness 4 possession 4 specific-nonspecific 4 3rd person. irregular or regular number. earliness 8.5 copula, contractible or uncontractible number. earliness 8.5 temporary duration, number, earliness 10 morpheme present progressive possessive articles auxilliary. contractible or uncontractible Again, the morphological operations under consideration here are not of direct importance to my study, but the data on their acquisition should be very helpful in fleshing out a picture of the acquisition ot English morphology. Word Creation The area which most interests me. both in terms of acquisition and of adult production. is rather more messy than normal derivational and inflectional morphology: word creation is a fairly complicated process, and is made more so by the fact that noone agrees on the vocabulary with which to talk about it. This is, of course. highly appropriate. Nonetheless. historical linguists and developmental psycholinguists have developed completely discrete methods of discussing word creation. which provides the student attempting to relate the two approaches with some interesting problems. The major difference seems to consist of a disagreement about what exactly constitutes a mechanism of word creation: the historical linguists use two distinct levels of mechanisms: a fundamental or systematic level with mechanisms such as analogy (called fourpan analogy, but essentially no different from ordinary analogy) and leveling (Hock 1988. 167-89), and a sporadic level with much more specific mechanisms such as blending, contamination. recomposition. folk etymology, and backformation (Hock 1988. 189-209). By contrast. the psycholinguists and Towards an explication ofthe role ofchild language in language change Page 12 morphologists who talk about word creation have emphasized what the historical linguists would call the sporadic level, in large part because the systematic mechanisms operate on time scales far too gradual for the changes to be visible on the time scales in which they are interested. The word formation processes (WFPs) I will be discussing are compounding, blending, clipping, and rebracketing. There are other mechanisms available, e.g. acronymy and arbitrary coinage, but they are not immediately relevant. Acronymy produces new words, but has a strong orthographic component, and as such is not active in the acquisition process, and arbitrary coinage, by virtue of the fact that the innovations have no antecedents, is not conducive to drawing insightful comparisons between child and adult forms. Mechanisms Compounding Compounding is the earliest-acquired (or perhaps only the earliest-productive) word creation strategy, which is interesting given the morphological complexity of adult compounds. The fact that words which are morphologically complex for adults are not so for children (Bowerman 1982, 323), however, suggests that children's compounds may be the results of linear, concatenative processes rather than morphological operations. In adult speech, compounding is by far the freest and most productive morpheme combination strategy (Bybee 1985, 108). For adults, it is a highly productive mechanism for generating new meanings within a category (formations similar to space+ boat (Brown 1973 data, adam33.cha) or mail-person (Clark 1982 data, shem06.cha) are commonly produced by adults as well), but it is also used derivationally (e.g. for V->N conversion) by children. Derivational compounding appears before the generally more productive mechanisms of derivational morphology (Clark 1981, 313), and remains remarkably productive as a derivational strategy until 3;06 or so. The facts that compounding is used for V->N conversion before derivational morphology, that the second word in agent compounds is strongly constrained (almost always thing or man (Clark 1981, 316», and that (related to the constraint on the second word) that the meaning of the compound is simply predictable from the meaning of its components, suggests to me that compounding is not morphologically complex for small children. Presumably, then, the simple concatenative approach to compounding is discarded in favour of morphological processes as the child's morphological competence increases. 5 Similarly, clipping appears to go through an initial and purely linear phase which will be discussed at greater length later. If we look at three-year-olds' deverbal nouns, we see that compounding is of comparable productivity with -er affixation for agent and instrument forms 3-year-olds' agent nominals (Clark 1981, 316) % conversion mechanism Towards an explication ofthe role ofchild language in language change 50 N. 50 N+N Nl+er. Clark gives no examples. but lawn" and lawn-man are examples of the correct types 3-~ar-olds' instrument nominals (Clark 1981. 317) idiomatic innovative % mechanism % mechanism 31 V+er 2S V+er 33 N+N 3S N+N 27 {V+N, N+V} 25 V Again. Clark gives no examples. bue throw", throwing-thing, throw-thing. and throw are of the correct types (V+er. N+N. V+N, and V, respectively). Percent representation of WCMs for agent forms by age (Clark 1981. 321) Age V +er compounding suppletive derivation example 3;0-3;08 55 21 3 3;09-4;05 90 5 2 4;06-5;02 76 5 10 5;03-6;0 91 2 3 Suppletive examples are, e.g., the use of knift or saw rather than CUff" or cut{ting)-thing when prompted with "a thing that you cut with" Percent representation ofWCMs for instrument forms by age (Clark 1981, 322) Age V +er compounding suppletive derivation example 3;0-3;08 42 6 28 3;09-4;05 71 2 8 4;06-5;02 70 3 10 5;03-6;0 72 10 6 The discussion of the morphological status of compounding is funher complicated by the possibility that compounding morphology may be acquired before derivational morphology. Donna Jo Napoli (p.c.) has suggested that since, according to Lexical Phonology, the layer on which compounding takes place is closer to the semantics than the (typically viewed as 'earlier') derivational layers, it may in fact be acquired first. This seems quite plausible to me, but I have not investigated the possibility in any great detail. The suggestion that the morphological layers are acquired from the top down would have very Towards an explication ofthe role ofchild language in language change Page 14 intriguing implications for the theory. and should certainly be investigated further. The semantic appearance of compounds still suggests that compounding goes through an initial concatenative phase. but if the compounding layer is acquirede derivational layers. compounding may become morphological much earlier than we would otherwise expect. Reanalysis Reanalysis should really be considered a supercategory. in that it includes several normally distinguished WFPs, such as recomposition. folk erymology, and backformation. All of the abovementioned processes can be interpreted as the result of inserting brackets into an existing word. although the details are slightly different in each case. In the case of folk etymology, the reanalysis matches a phoneme sequence with that of a word, as in som"sau/t -> tumb/fsau/t or [emf/Ie -> [emale. In that ofbackformation, with that of an affix. as in backformation -> backform+ation. And in that of recomposition. with JUSt about anything. as in bracket -> bra+ket. It is often convenient to refer to the three processes separately. 6 bur it appears to me that by claiming them to be distinct mechanisms. rather than variants of a single mechanism. linguists are mising a valuable generalization. The folk etymology aspect of reanalysis begins to be attested for children much earlier than either of the other two. This is unsurprising. given that the inflectional affixation paradigms necessary for backformation are not acquired until 3-4 years of age, and (obvious) recomposirion requires a level of morphological sophistication that should not be 'present until fairly late in the acquisition process. Recomposition is a less-discussed word creation submechanism than either backformation or folk etymology, so it probably should be described. in case I discuss any examples of it at some later point. Recomposition is similar to clipping in that it produces new morphemes based upon (possible) syllable boundaries rather than morpheme boundaries. but it seems to be much more analogical in its application and idiomatic in terms of created meanings. and is thus like backformation. I t works by dividing a donor word into multiple (usually 2) morphemes which can then participate in other morphological processes. Recomposition can produce either free or bound morphemes. and has no clear restrictions except that any morphs. it produces have to be syllabic. Rather incidentally. recomposition provides pretty strong evidence for bracket erasure in that it divides lexical words into morphemes. which division is usually diachronically inaccurate. The best-known examples of splitting are only of peripheral interest in this paper because they are known to have been produced by adults. but they are interesting because of the high level of productivity that they currently exhibit. The most productive example involves alcohol which is actually quite an interesting word because it has participated in splitting at at least two different points. one leading to -oho/ and one to -oho/ie. The twO cases of splitting are represented as unconnected for two reasons: a) the creation of the morphemes -oho/ and -oho/ic occurred at drastically different times, and b) their meanings are almost totally unconnected. X-oho/ meaning 'X such that X has an aldehyde Towards an explication ofthe role ofchild language in language change Page 15 group' and X-oholic 'someone addicted to X' (there is also an X-oholic which means containing X-ohol but it was produced by derivational morphology and not by splitting.) Reanalysis is generally far more productive among children than it is among adulrs. This is not initially obvious: very few examples are attested. and certainly not enough to justify rhe claim. Howeve ·given that children initially acquire words as unanalyzed wholes (Bowerman 1982, 321). all morphologically complex words which were acquired before ... 4;0 are examples of reanalysis, but are not generally noticed as such because they coincide with the adult forms (indeed. that is how we get the adult forms) and such forms generally go undetected (Clark 1981, 302). Similarly, inflectional paradigms are simplified down to a single allomorph, and later expanded (although there may be intermediary allomorph switching) (Bowerman 1982, 322), and a distributionally contrastive set of rules will be simplified to a single rule which will be applied to the whole domain of the set, and subsequently the rule paradigm will be reorganized (Bowerman 1982, 322). Blending Blending seems to be mostly an adult process, but there are plenty of examples of blending by children (e.g. jack+a+weasel (Brown 1973 data, adam 19.cha), kersplode (Kuczaj 1977 data, abe 113.cha), and raisin+branios (MacWhinney ChiLDES data, boys81.cha)). It is generally listed as being a discrete word creation mechanism, bur it could, if there were theoretical or computational reasons for doing so, alternately be analyzed in terms of clipping or ~ecomposition followed by morphological compounding (Bauer 1983, 236). By analogy with the purely linear-concatenative compounding and MLU-prompted clipping, I would suggest that there is an aspect of blending which is in fact purely phonological, but that the process is a morphological one. Also, given the strong evidence that judgements about morphology are based on phonological and semantic similarity (Derwing & Baker 1979, 217-21) I would expect blending to lead to the formation of new morphemes. Clipping Clipping (or hypocoristics) consists of taking a syllable or possible syllable from a word, usually from the one of the ends. and using it to represent the entire word. It therefore seems to violate the Principle of Contrast, in that there is little or no initial semantic differentiation between the 'parent' word and the clipped form, at least at first. Words such as diss (originally disrespecty) which persist in both clipped and unclipped forms may have those forms undergo divergent semantic evolution, possibly driven by Contrast. Young children appear to have a specialized form of clipping, which may in fact avoid the violation of the Principle of Contrast: clipping seems to be a strategy for allowing longer utterances in the face of MLU restrictions, and as such may not constitute creation of a new lexical form, bur essentially be a phonemectomy on words thac grow too long. I propose chis for Towards an fXplication ofthe role ofchild language in language change twO reasons: first, is the analogy with chii Page 16 compounding, which appears to be purely concatenative in its early form; second, there is a distinction between the syllable retained in child clipping and that retained in 'normal' adult clipping. The syllable that is kept in child clipping appears to correspond closely with the 'most salient' syllable of the word. the one on which children focus in acquisition (generally, this is the most metrically or phonologically prominent syllable, as in ); dipping among adults needn't retain the most salient syllable: examples such as za for pizza or rents for parents retain the least prominent syllable. and are still perfectly comprehensible. Discussion Several studies have been conducted on the acquisition of morphology, bm all of the language-specific studies (e.g. Kuczaj 1977, Gleason 1978. Derwing & Baker 1979) have focused on very specific mechanisms, e.g. the morphemes for past, or plural, or progressive aspect, or agentive -er, or adverbial -/y. Studies on word creation have looked at compounding in some detail (e.g. Clark 1986, 1988), bur apparently not at any of my other mechanisms. I am attempting to redress this o~ission, at least in a programmatic way. Characterizing the development of the productivity of word creation by children of course requires a large amount of data, gathered over a fairly significant period, and is therefore entirely unsuited to the time constraints on an undergraduate thesis. Fonunately, however, there is no requirement that a researcher gather her own data, and the ChiLDES database has more than enough for any reasonable research programme. Data analysis I worked with the data from a number of researchers (see Appendix A for a list, and a table of all the neologisms I found in the data), using the CLAN program combo to search for neologisms. and the mit program to get word counts for normalization purposes. Word and utterance counts from ChiLDES, grouped by age range age utterances turns words w/u 0;6 - 1;0 3 3 13 4.33 1;0 - 1;06 976 494 1473 1.51 1;06 - 2;0 13068 6611 21831 1.67 2;0 - 2;06 20916 11115 47233 2.26 2;06 - 3;0 76102 49475 237242 3.12 3;0 - 3;06 63236 39934 242552 3.84 3;06 -4;0 35172 23175 161058 4.58 Towards an explication ofthe role ofchild language in language change Page 17 4;0 - 4;06 30611 20038 137003 4.48 4;06 - 5;0 26816 15722 121271 4.52 5;0 - 5;06 15907 10993 72754 4.57 5;06 - 6:0 10837 8275 52485 4.84 6;0 - 6;06 4702 3478 22313 4.75 6;06 -7;0 5025 3251 21171 4.21 7:0 -7;06 9042 6188 37303 4.12 7;06 - 8;0 1727 1266 7260 4.20 8;0 - 8;06 522 398 2373 4.55 Since the data were formatted in CHAT, I searched for the patterns '*+ '* (which matches any word which has been transcribed with the + morpheme boundary) and *@* (matches any neologisms identified by the transcriber). The resulting data are rather skewed, in that some ages had more data than others. I therefore used the word counts from mIt to scale the word creation counts. The number of occurrances of neologisms, soned by age and mechanism, and normalized to the overall word counts in each age range, are included in the table below. There are cenain to be examples missing. as I have eliminated neologisms produced by word creation mechanisms studied by other researchers from this table (e.g. examples of agentive -er affixation and past tense regularization. while present in the corpus (Appendix ' are not included in my tables or statistics. Neologism counts from ChiLDES. normalized to the number of words Age range blending tokens % clipping compounding nonce creation tokens % tokens % tokens % 1;0 - 1;06 0.002 0.20 0.005 0.01 0.001 0.02 1:6 - 2:0 0.018 1.51 0.018 0.26 2:0 - 2;6 0.159 13.05 2.789 4.95 0.199 0.400 32.78 18.614 33.05 0.409 33.52 21.895 reanalysis tokens % 2.82 0.040 1.946 2.402 33.94 0.600 29.32 38.87 3.683 52.05 1.023 49.95 4.620 8.20 0.408 5.76 0.136 6.63 3.583 6.36 0.116 1.63 2.865 5.09 0.205 2.89 0.205 9.99 5;6 - 6;0 1.505 2.67 0.044 0.63 0.044 2.16 6:0 - 6:6 0.264 0.47 2:6 - 3;0 0.200 16.59 3:0 - 3;6 3;6 - 4;0 0.543 45.06 4:0 - 4;6 0.462 38.33 4;6 - 5;0 0.231 18.93 5:0 - 5;6 Towards an ~plication ofth( rol( ofchild languag( in languagf changf 6;6 -7;0 0.054 0.10 7;0 -7;6 0.125 0.22 7;6 - 8;0 0.006 0.01 Since not all researchers use the + morpheme boundary marker or the @ innovation marker in their transcriptions. and those who use it do not do so with perfect consistency. this list is almost certainly incomplete. However, there are several clear patterns in the data, and some of them are even genuine. The most obvious pattern is that compounding is a vastly more productive method of word creation than any other. This is very likely true, but if so, my data don't indicate it: my inspection of several of the data files suggests that the use of the + marker in the transcriptions is far more reliable than that of @. I found very few compounds that were unlabeled, and many more neologisms that were produced by other means. I am therefore hesitant to accept as real the ratios of the productivities suggested by these data. Obviously, in an attempt to categorize the productivity and acquisition of word creation mechanisms, it would be nice to be able to relate the relative productivities as well as the changes in a particular mechanism's productiviry with age. Since I can't do it by automated search, I could (obviously) do it manually: read all of the transcriptions on ChiLDES and note down all of the neologisms. If I had opted to do this, you would be first reading this thesis around the year 2000. Instead, I have conducted a similar investigation of the data in another, much smaller, set of transcripts (Parkhurst & Gottman 1986). In these. I have compared the relative but not the absolute productivity of several mechanisms. assuming that my reading is not biased toward detecting any particular one. Instances of word creation, sorted by age (Parkhurst& Gottman 1986 data) age range mechanism tokens lexemes 2;06 - 3;0 clipping 3 3 2;06 - 3;0 compounding 2 2 2;06 - 3;0 compounding (L) 1 2;06 - 3;0 null conversion 1 3;0 - 3;06 compounding (R) 4;0 - 4;06 adjectival -y affixation 4;0 - 4;06 backformation 4;0 - 4;06 compounding 4 4 Towards an explication ofthe role ofchild language in language change Page 19 4;06 - 5;0 blending 1 4;06 - 5;0 blending 1 4;06 - 5;0 clipping 1 4;06 - 5;0 compounding 10 10 4;06 - 5;0 null conversion 2 2 5;0 - 5;06 adjectival -y affixation 2 2 5;0 - 5;06 clipping 3 3 5;0 - 5;06 compounding 13 13 5;0 - 5;06 nonce creation 1 5;0 - 5;06 agentive -er affIXion 5;0 - 5;06 null conversion 5;0 - 5;06 reanalysis 6;06 - 7;0 clipping 1 1 6;06 - 7;0 compounding (R) In most of these cases, compounding was the most productive mechanism, but by much less than in the ChiLDES data. Unfortunately, the overlap between the ChiLDES and Parkhurst & Gottman data was small enough that I am leery of trying to use the latter to scale the relative productiviry of the mechanisms in the former in any but the roughest way (although they do seem to support my impression that I am missing a significant number of non-compounding neologisms in the ChiLDES data. The identification of word creation mechanisms should be simple: certainly the morphological processes involved are fairly distinctive, and there should be little or no risk of confusing, e.g., instances of compounding with blending, or reanalysis with clipping. There certainly are some fuzzy cases, but I feel fairly confident about most of my classifications. Conclusions Possible links between language acquisition and language change The original impetus for this study was an interest in how an understanding of child word creation and acquisition, and any morphological or statistical signals it might leave, could be used to identify the instigators of linguistic change. Historical linguists have argued even more strongly for child motivation of language change than have the psycholinguists. although the arguments have typically been very general. Towards an explication ofthe rOle ofchild language in language change Page 20 The major argument for children's driving language change is based on the fact that the grammars of adults do not undergo reorganizarion, although they may undergo other, simpler modifications (King 1969, 65). Evidence for this is seen in the inability of adults to acquire additional languages (although they may learn them) (King 1969,66) and in the existence of hypercorrection (King 1969,69·71). The argument is that children's grammars are oprimizing: computational constraints and smaller gramatical base make simplification of the grammar both possible and necessary (King 1969, 65). The status of this claim is rather unclear, however: certainly, children fed the output of an adult rule that, e.g., always replaces one phoneme with another (lhwl -> [w], or, more formally, [.voc, .cons] -> 0 I_ [.voc, ·cons, +backD have no reason to learn the adult underlying form (lhwl) when their input corpus doesn't contain it (King 1969,81); in situations where two different rule systems will produce the same output, then, the child would be expected to adopt the simpler form in her grammar. This picture is clearly over· simple, although it avoids the claim that language acquisition should drive grammars universally to maximal simplicity. Of course, it only avoids the claim because the optimal form of the grammar is usually much less clear than in the example above: due to morpho phonological stem alternations in Modern English (e.g. between goose and gosling, keep and kept, etc.), either the underlying vowels must be left in their pre.Great.Vowel.Shift state and rules (presumably similar to those involved in the vowel Shift itself) added to provide for the vowel contrast (King 1969, 83) or each (morphologically related) form must be listed seperately in the lexicon. The cost of a great many extra lexical entries may well outweigh the cost of an additional phonological rule, but as either would produce the same output, I would want to see some data from speech error or lexical access experiments before pronouncing on which form was used. On a syntactic level, King argues for reorganization (and thus for child agency) in cases such as the rule sequence Case marking -> Wh·movement -> Whom·replacement (replaced by Wh·movement -> Case marking) (King 1969, 142·9) These examples of restructuring, of course, are not what Melissa Bowerman (1982) means by the term, as they are all cases where the child could have learned the simple form directly, the more complex form being present only in the previous generation. Bowerman uses reorganization to refer to internal processes whereby the child's syntactic or morphological rules are restructured internally. While her version of grammatical restructuring is much more interesting than King's, it is less useful for determiningachronic work, since the high time·resolution data on individual children are simply not present historically. A more interesting problem would be to find specific historical changes which were identifiably caused by children in a reasonably narrow age range. I have not found any examples that are definitely in this category, but there are some suggestive possibilities. Towards an explication ofthe role ofchild language in language change Page 21 It has been proposed (Shipley 1991) mat the use of thtt in both nominative and objective case among Quakers (rather than the historically more venerable use of thte only for objective case) originated among the children in the community. This hypothesis is based on her observations of differences between chile! and parental use of the Plain Speech (Shipley 1991, 179 and passim) rather than simply on the common assumption that all language change begins among children. The case she draws seems quite plausible, but requires some history first. When the Quakers adopted the thoulthttlthylthine paradigm (henceforth referred to as T for convenience) for use wirh everyone, all of the paradigm was still in use in the general English-speaking community, as well as the pronouns yt. you. you. your. and yours (collectively referred to as v). By me late 16th century. yt and you were used interchangeably, and by the early 18th century. yt had disappeared altogether (Shipley 1991, 179). During the 17th century, T dropped out of common English usage, in part because of distaste for its usage by Quakers (Shipley 1991, 172). Gradually, thou disappeared from the Plain Speech over the 18th, 19th. and early 20th centuries (179). Shipley proposed that this disappearance of thou occurred as a result of the use ofv by Quaker children in heterogenous communities. If children noticed that where non-Quakers used you, Quakers sometimes used thtt and sometimes thou (note rhar rhe disappearance of thou significantly posrdares the disappearance of ye). the distinction might well seem arbitrary. in which case rhe simplest solution would be due to use one of the Quaker forms exclusively when speaking to other Quakers (Shipley 1991. 179). Thee would be the obvious choice of the two, because of its similarities to he. she. we. and mt. Shipley proposes that such changes were stimulated by corrections by parents with a strong commitment to the Plain Speech. but. given the apparent unwillingness by children to consider parental correction as evidence in language acquisition. it seems more likely that the change would have occurred in communities with a large Quaker minority, in which case extrafamilial positive evidence would present the child with both paradigms. and thee would be marked as the preferred form by the rime rhe child's greater undersranding of social relationships made her use ofT predominant in speech to other Quakers. I should note here that Shipley's assertion that the disappearance of thou was due to parental correction is not incompatible with the fact that children do not immediately respond to parental corrections (e.g, MacWhinney 1978. 8 (rule 3» because the modification in child useage is not assumed to take place immediately bur rather at some point in adolescence when rhe child. who presumably has noted thar T is an adult pronominal form. begins to use it as a social device (if I speak like an adult. I will be treated like one). The child language data raise some interesting questions about mulrilingualism and multidialectalism. The word creation data suggests that words are added to the general lexicon via subgroup (dialect) vocabularies. Since there is a strong opposition to having multiple forms with the same meaning in a given language (there is a weaker opposition to having multiple meanings with the same form. but that's not immediately relevant). and opposition to having multiple forms with the same Towards an explication of the role ofchild language in language change Page 22 meaning among biligual children before the age of three. even when those forms are in different languages. Giects are typically assigned an intermediate status between ideolects and languages. it would be interesting to observe the acceptance rate for created words as a function of age (I have only considered production) Given the stronger form of the Principle of Contrast (allow no two forms with the same meaning) among young children. the borrowing of French animal names as English meat names immediately after the Norman Conquest appears promising as a possible child-driven change. My scenario would be that the initial change in meaning in going from boeu/to bee/may have been the result of overextension by an English child (probably in the household of a noble. or she would never have heard French) in an effort to avoid conflict between the new term and the existing term she knew it contrasted with. Proposals for further study In order to establish a clear hierarchy of morphological complexity. productivity, and acquisition order for the word creation mechanisms I have been investigating, some specific pieces of data are necessary: a more accurate search mechanism for clipping, blending, and reanalysis would do admirably. but a somewhat larger corpus for manual search (such as are available on ChiLDES) would do as well. as would an elicitation study for words to fill known lexical gaps. Such a study would need to keep several methodological questions in mind: in eliciting neologisms from a broad age range. either lexical gaps would need to be found which are not only gaps for both adults and children. but for which a need would be perceived by both groups. or equal numbers would need to be found which were illegitimate for each group; since it appears that linguistic elicitation cues have a significant effect on the form of created words (Clark 1981. 322). a set of prompts needs to be found which is either nonlinguistic or which has enough variety in the prompts for the effects of the phrasing to cancel out. I have compiled a partial list of lexical gaps (with most of the obviously age-specific examples eliminated) to this end. but the list would clearly need to be expanded for such a study A panial list of lexical gaps for a future elicitation study of child word creation a small, annoying dog the sudden irrational desire [0 do something completely foolish in a formal situation the superstitious practice of asserting something the fear that you will spontaneously and dire about the future in hopes that this will somehow involuntarily do something stupid and dangerous in any situation keep it from taking place the bunch of keys hiding in the back of a drawer that the amount by which a price in the store is less than a sensible amount you don't know what they're for those sticks for stirring paint the little dance you do when you really have to pee and can't Towards an ~plication o/the role o/child language in language change Page 23 the cwo liccle lines that come down from your nose che amount by which the heighc of a wave exceeds the height to which you have rolled up your panes the safe place where you put things where you then any cechnical or "childproof' device which can't find them grownups can'c make work buc any child can the place behind the cushions where all the small things you lose from your pockets end up a drawing made by a child for which no adult can identify the subject anything which you drag along a fence or wall to make a noise (esp. a stick) to go through the motions of an action while speaking. to remind you of che word for it the peace that passeth all understanding nonsexual love becween friends something which juscifies having a really good cry a person who pulls a wagon things which are broken and irreparable vs. things which are broken and either still usable or fixable to give someone a present chac they don' t want but you do the hideous gifts your relatives give you which you the drawer, usually in people's kitchens, where are expected to admire and use when chey are visiting random small objeccs like cwisties. paper clips. botcle lids, string and so on are stored. in no parcicular order (cf. whatnot) Someone who hogs the swings your mocher's best friend the feel of cold wacer sloshing over the coes of your rubber boocs a bully who hurts someone anJ precends he's playing so he doesn'c gec in crouble Such funher studies, together with the data I have gathered thus far, should make it possible to accurately characterize the acquisition of word creation mechanisms in English, and rhus to tighten and clarify the link between diachronic and developmental psycholinguistics. 1Pinker assumes thac children are sec up to learn Bresnan & Kaplan's Lexical Functional Grammar, (Pinker 1984, 14, and see 14-21 for an overview of LFG) but the specific UG acquired is not important for my concerns. 2 It has been argued that the distinction between phonology, syncax, morphology, semandcs, and pragmadcs is nonexistent for children, whatever its status for adults. 3The acquisition of grammar is quite clearly bound to the acquisition of grammatical morphemes such as tense markers or agenrive and instrumental -~r, but it does not seem to be connected to word creation processes such as clipping or blending in any way. Hence. not tightly bound. 4de Villiers & de Villiers use Jacobs & Rosenbaum's transformational grammar in their analyses for the sake of compatibility with Brown (1973). 'I would expect to see, given enough data, a slight dip in the productivity of compounding (and other creation mechanisms as well) at the point when it becomes morphologically complex rather than concatenative. Towards an explication of th( rol( ofchild languagf in languagf change 6And, indeed, I will probably do so, my ranting against overspecificity notwithstanding. Towards an explication oftht rolt ofchild Ianguagt in language changt Pagt25 Appendix A: Child neologism data From the ChiLDES database In this list of child coinages, line numbers have been omitted for space reasons, since a search of a single file under combo takes at most a few seconds on a Macintosh, and presumably even less on a UN*X workstation. Also for space reasons, the name of the principle researcher (actually, the person after whom the ChiLDES subdirectory was named) has been used in identifying the files and utterances used. Data are to be attributed as follows: Researcher Citation Bohannon Bohannon and Marquis 1977, Stine and Bohannon 1983 Brown Brown 1973 Clark Clark 1978a, Clark 1978b, Clark 1979, Clark 1982 Evans no citation Fletcher Fletcher & Garman 1988, Johnson 1986 Garvey Garvey & Hogan 1973 Gathercole Gathercole 1980 Gleason Bellinger & Gleason 1982, Masur & Gleason 1980 Ku~j KUC2j 1976, KUC2j 1977 MacWhinney no citation Sachs Sachs 1983 Snow no citation Warren Warren·Leubecker & Bohannon 1984, Warren·Leubecker 1982 Wells Wells 1981, Wells 1986 All data from the ChiLDES database are courtesy of Brian MacWhinney and Catherine Snow (MacWhinney & Snow 1985, MacWhinney & Snow 1990) researcher file speaker age word gloss # Wells ben;am04.cha Benjamin 1;01.30 mo"ow romorrow 1 Brown adam06.cha Adam 1;05.12 rumble +been [Nj ?? 3 Wells ben;am02.cha Benjamin 1;05.21 lone ?? The role ofchild language in linguistic change Page 26 researcher file speaker age word gloss # Wells france02.cha Frances 1:06.1 Pttp+bo ?? L Wclls benjam03.cha Bcn;amin 1:08.27 blips slippers 3 Wclls ben;am03.cha Bcnjamin 1:08.27 morning a train 10 Sachs nl1.cha Naomi 1;10.10 btt/Hating is eating beef Sachs nl1.cha Naomi 1:10.10 home+home to go home Sachs n33.cha Naomi 1:11.29 drop+arop perhaps a medicine-dropper 1 1 ?? Sachs n23.cha Naomi 1;11.6 drop+drops the contents of a drop-drop 3 Sachs n45.cha Naomi 2:01.7 mg+ling+ling ?? 1 Clark shem02.cha Shem 2:02.23 bandaid+paper cither the envelope or the adhesive backing of a bandaid 1 Clark shem02.cha Shem 2:02.23 uuider+cover (NJ ?? 1 Clark shem02.cha Shem 2:02.23 /adder+m4n (N] ?? 1 Clark shem02.cha Shem 2;02.23 /awn +mower a person who mows the lawn 2 Clark shem02.cha Shem 2;02.23 mow+/awner a person who mows the lawn Wells ;ack05.cha Jack 2;02.25 police car +bike a police motorcycle 1 Clark' shem04.cha Shem 2;03.16 boco+man (N] ?? 2 Clark shem04.cha Shem 2;03.16 fire+light a flashlight 3 Clark shem04.cha Shem 2;03.16 jar+cookies cookies that are kept in a jar Brown adam02.cha Adam 2:03.18 choo+ch{1() track a train track '(this suggests nonce compounding since train track should be referenrially opaque) Brown adam02.cha Adam 2;03.18 gree(n)+jea(m) green (blue) jeans 2 Brown adam02.cha Adam 2;03.18 gree(n)+jea(ns} marching bellr a marching bear which wears green+jeans 1 Brown adam02.cha Adam 2;03.18 suitcllse water (N] ?? 1 Brown adam02.cha. -03.cha. -OS.cha Adam 2;03.182;04.30 Ttxllco+Star the star from the Texaco logo ?? 6 Brown sarah003.cha Sarah 2:03.19 fish wa(ter) water that fish swim in ?? 1 Clark shem03.cha Shem 2;03.2 bunny+rabbit record a record with a song about a 1 bunny-rabbit ?? Towards an txplication oftht rolt ofchild language in language change Page 27 researcher file speaker age word gloss Wells beuy05.cha Betty 2;03.2 cockle+doo baby [N] ?? Clark shem03.cha Shem 2;03.2 play+dough [0 Clark shem03.cha Shem 2;03.2 snowpen+man ? [Nj ?? Clark shem03.cha Shem 2;03.2 space+tkle [N] ?? Clark shem06.cha Shem 2;03.28 mail+pmon a mail carrier Clark shem06.cha Shem 2;03.28 person +mail a mail carrier 2 Clark shem06.cha Shem 2;03.28 skate+car a game 2 Brown adamOI.cha Adam 2;03.4 hunny+rahhit walk a walk like that of a bunnyrahhit Brown adamOI.cha Adam 2;03.4 /adder fire truck a hook-and-Iadder truck Wells garyOS.cha Gary 2;03.4 Pasting+board a board for pasting things # play with play-dough 2 2 I on Brown adamOI.cha Adam 2;03.4 wu+wu+home the game this little piggy... 3 Brown adam01.cha. -02.cha Adam 2;03.42;03.18 too+man a tattooed man?? 21 Brown sarah002.cha Sarah 2;03.7 teddy +bear any stuffed (or cute and I harmless) animal (concluded from the use of teddy-bear bear to mean teddy-bear) Brown sarah007.cha. -009.cha. -OIO.cha. -013.cha. -019.cha. -020.cha. -024.cha. -030.cha. -044.cha. -OSS.cha. -071.cha. -092.cha Sarah 2;04.104;01.18 Chatty+baby [name of doll] 61 Brown sarahOlO.cha Sarah 2;04.19 Poor+Pony [name of horse] 13 Clark shem08.cha Shem 2;04.20 daddy+whul [N] ?? 1 Clark shem08.cha Shem 2;04.20 motor +thing a thing with a motor; a 1 machine Clark shem09.cha Shem 2;04.25 book+one a book Clark shem09.cha Shem 2;04.25 letter +man a mail carrier Towards an explication ofthe role ofchild language in language change 2 Page 28 researcher file speaker age word gloss # Clark shem09.cha Shem 2:04.25 pictur~+door [N] ?? 2 Clark shem09.cha Shem 2:04.25 pr~smt+on~ a present ,; Clark shem09.cha Shem 2:04.25 thi~f+man a thief 1 Clark shcm07.cha Shem 2:04.4 batur+man ?? 1 Wells gavin05.cha Gavin 2:04.4 Bib +boss ?? 1 Wells gavin05.cha Gavin 2:04.4 bib+on~ a bib 2 Wells gavin05.cha Gavin 2:04.4 plis+Tnan [N] ?? 1 Wells gavinOS.cha Gavin 2;04.4 pliss+on~ [N] ?? 1 Clark shem07.cha Shem 2:04.4 r~ading+th~+books+the+m the man reading the books 1 an Wells gavin05.cha Gavin 2:04.4 Sold+i" policmum a policeman who is a soldier (an MP ??) 1 Sachs n58.cha Naomi 2:04.5 Friar+Tuck [N] ?? 3 Clark sheml2.cha Shem 2:05.16 butterfly +shirt [N] ?? 1 Clark sheml2.cha Shem 2:05.16 garag~+door the door to the garage 3 Clark shem12.cha Shem 2;05.16 houu+door the door to a house 3 Clark shem12.cha Shem 2:05.16 key+C/ock [N] ?? 1 Snow nathOlb.cha Nathan 2;05.18 go~d+zo ?? 8 Snow nathOI b.cha. -Olc.cha Nathan 2;05.18 gunky [Nj ?? 7 Snow nathOlb.cha. -06.cha. -05.cha. -16.cha. Nathan 2;05.18 slupy+suit pyjamas 13 Clark shem1 O.cha Shem 2;05.2 fry+pancakes fried pancakes 2 Clark sheml0.cha Shem 2;05.2 store +man a storekeeper Kuczaj abe007.cha Abe 2;05.20 zig+zag zig+zag ! 6 Clark shem13.cha Shem 2;05.23 bup+thing a thing that goes beep beep 1 Clark shem13.cha Shem 2;05.23 bug+tiance a dance which bugs do Kuczaj abe009.cha Abe 2;05.23 cock +a +doodle +doo a rooster ?? Clark shem13.cha Shem 2;05.23 dog+party a party for dogs 2 Clark sheml3.cha Shem 2;05.23 e/~phan(t)+trunk the trunk of an elephant 1 Clark sheml3.cha Shem 2;05.23 flod+bowl a bowl to put food in 1 Clark sheml3.cha Shem 2:05.23 rabbit+garden a garden for rabbits ?? 2 Clark shem13.cha Shem 2;05.23 smoke +box a box with smoke coming oue Towards an txp/ication oftht raft ofchild language in languagt change Page 29 researcher file speaker age word gloss # Clark shem13.cha Shem 2;05.23 trnnis+playing+bal/ a tennis ball 1 Wells benjam06.cha Benjamin 2;05.28 wohhly+woo ?? 1 Sachs n61.cha Naomi 2:05.3 y~sur+ morning yesterday morning 2 Clark shem1 4.cha Shem 2;05.30 rngin~+one [N] ?? Clark shem14.cha Shem 2:05.30 garage+hose the hose in the garage Clark shem14.cha Shem 2;05.30 garag~+thing a thing that belongs in the garage Gleason dinner: Patricia 2:05.30 pickk fork a fork for eating pickles with 1 patricia.cha 1 Sachs n62.cha Naomi 2;05.8 Donkry +Benjamin [name] ?? 5 Sachs n62.cha Naomi 2;05.8 nappy+tim~ the time for changing 1 diapers Clark shemll.cha Shem 2:05.9 hahy+ftoggie a tadpole Clark shemll.cha Shem 2;05.9 mommy+ftoggje a frog who is a mommy 2 Clark sheml1.cha Shem 2;05.9 pancak~+book the book about pancakes 10 Clark sheml1.cha Shem 2;05.9 smile Hhirt ?? 2 Clark shemll.cha Shem 2;05.9 snajl+foot a snail's foO[ 1 Clark shemll.cha Shem 2;05.9 stirring+bowl a bowl for stirring things in (cf. mixing bowl) Clark shemll.cha Shem 2;05.9 wheel+wattr a water-wheel Snow nath02.cha Nathan 2:06.0 sing/~+duk" Wells darren06.cha Darren 2;06.1 ban+an a bandage Brown adam08.cha Adam 2;06.17 road paptr [N] ?? Brown adam08.cha Adam 2;06.17 tape recorder roll of film 1 Snow nath08.cha Nathan 2;06.25 bibble+wibblt ?? 2 Snow nath08.cha, Nathan 2;06.25 3;04.18 SUCkYHuck perhaps a pacifier ?? 5 1 -25.cha bus 2 single+decker bus Clark sheml6.cha Shem 2;06.27 drum +thing a thing you can drum on Clark shem16.cha Shem 2:06.27 drummtr+thing a thing you can drum on Clark sheml6.cha Shem 2;06.27 drummingHhing a thing for drumming with 2 Brown adam07.cha Adam 2;06.3 Popty~ a walker for Popeye 3 walk" (cf. baby walker) Brown sarahO 16.cha, -030.cha Towards an ~xplication ofth~ Sarah ro/~ 2;06.42;09.20 ofchild language in Tickle+Baby languag~ chang~ [proper name] ?? 2 Page 30 researcher file speaker age word gloss Clark sheml5.cha Shem 2;06.6 bo(at) +car either a car that pulls a boat 3 or one thar can float like one. Clark sheml5.cha Shem 2;06.6 jireungine+car a car associated with fire engines (e.g. [he fire chiefs car) 2 Clark sheml5.cha Shem 2;06.6 sign +truck a truck with a sign on it 1 Clark sheml7.cha Shem 2;07.10 . floor +thing Clark sheml7.cha Shem 2;07.10 flooring+thing [N) ?? Clark sheml7.cha Shem 2;07.10 head+board [N) ?? Clark sheml7.cha Shem 2;07.10 mi/l+part a millstone ?? Clark sheml7.cha Shem 2;07.10 outside+book a book you can take outside Clark sheml7.cha Shem 2;07.10 outside+door a door to the outside Clark sheml7.cha Shem 2;07.10 town +thing [N) ?? Brown sarah021.cha Sarah 2;07.12 boo +boo hand ?? Brown sarah021.cha Sarah 2;07.12 Booby +Bear [proper name) 1 Kuczaj abe024.cha Abe 2;07.15 right oh diliry+oh perhaps hi-ho-the-derry-o 3 Sachs n68.cha Naomi 2;07.16 potty+pan a bedpan ?? 2 Clark sheml8.cha Shem 2;07.18 bon thing a box Clark sheml8.cha Shem 2;07.18 door+home [N] ?? Clark sheml8.cha Shem 2;07.18 exercius+ book an exercise-book 3 Clark sheml8.cha Shem 2;07.18 mark+covrr the cap of a marker pen ?? 2 Clark sheml8.cha Shem 2;07.18 part+table [NJ ?? Clark sheml8.cha Shem 2;07.18 water+name the name of some water Clark sheml8.cha Shem 2;07.18 water+tail a paimbrush Clark shem19.cha Shem 2;07.26 ball+ehair ?? 2 Clark sheml9.cha Shem 2;07.26 minky+brd a house 1 Clark sheml9.cha Shem 2;07.26 mommy+deer ? a deer who is a mommy 2 Clark shem19.cha Shem 2;07.26 open+game a kind of game Clark sheml9.cha Shem 2;07.26 s(h)oe+part ? ?? Brown sarah020.cha Sarah 2;07.5 Tony+Pony fingrrs [N) ?? Sachs n69.cha Naomi 2;08.14 al/+gony characterized by being all gone Clark shem21 b.cha Shem 2;08.15 boy+onr a boy Towards an (Xp/icalion ofthe role ofchild language in language change # [N) ?? 3 1 2 1 Page 31 word gloss # pancake+one the one about pancakes (same referent as pancakebook) 1 2:08.15 xxx +horn [N] ?? 1 Shem 2:08.15 2;11.10 cry+one someone who cries 3 shem22b.cha Shem 2;08.20 baby+pig.r pigs which are babies 1 Clark shem22a.cha Shem 2;08.20 baby+song a song for babies (or about babies ?) 1 Clark shem22b.cha Shem 2;08.20 baseball +pad ?? Clark shem22a.cha Shem 2:08.20 grown+up plale a plate for grownups Clark shem22a.cha Shem 2;08.20 living+room chair the chair in the living room Clark shem22b.cha Shem 2:08.20 man +nightmare [N] ?? Clark shem22b.cha Shem 2:08.20 tantrum+book ?? Clark shem22a.cha Shem 2:08.20 tortilla +momler a monster who loves tortillas (cf. cookie monster) Wells martin07.cha Martin 2;08.24 mo+mo a pedal car Clark shem23.cha Shem 2;08.29 animal+movie a movie about animals 1 Clark shem23.cha Shem 2;08.29 mommy+ ra bbit a rabbit who is a mommy 1 Clark shem20.cha Shem 2;08.3 bake+one ? a baker ?? Clark shem20.cha Shem 2;08.3 duck+one a duck Clark shem20.cha Shem 2;08.3 egg+thing an egg 4 Clark shem20.cha Shem 2;08.3 foucet+wheel a faucet handle 4 Clark shem20.cha Shem 2;08.3 flot+book [N] ?? Clark shem20.cha Shem 2;08.3 grown+up spoon a spoon for grownups 2 Clark shem20.cha Shem 2;08.3 outsitU+thing a thing for using au (Side 1 Clark shem20.cha Shem 2:08.3 present +book a book brought as a present Clark shem20.cha Shem 2;08.3 snake+one a snake or a thing like a snake Clark shem20.cha Shem 2;08.3 sun +thing a parasol ?? Gathercole Ol.cha Jeff 2;09 grow ups grows up MacWhin ney ross25.cha Ross 2;09.0 car +street a street that cars go on Wells benjam07.cha Benjamin 2;09.1 brum to sweep ?? 3 Wells benjam07.cha Benjamin 2:09.1 finzy tiny 2 researcher file speaker Clark shem21 a.cha Shem ·2:08.15 Clark shem21 a.cha Shem Clark shem21 b.cha. -32.cha Clark age Towards an explication ofthe role ofchild language in language change 2 1 1 Page 32 researcher file speaker age word gloss Clark shem24.cha Shem 2i09.1 m~ss+room [N] ?? Clark shem26.cha Shem 2i09.19 s/~dHhing a sled ~ Clark shem27b.cha Shem 2;09.27 clichthing a ching which clicks 2 Clark shem27b.cha Shem 2;09.27 spinning+thinf/ tops 1 Kuczaj abe043.cha Abe 2:09.30 potter ?? 1 Kuczaj abe043.cha Abe 2:09.30 potter+ potter [occupation] ?? 1 Clark shem28b.cha Shem 2;10.2 baby Hru a baby trce 1 Clark shem28a.cha Shem 2;10.2 b~d+man someone who can change seats into beds 1 Clark shem28b.cha Shem 2:10.2 daddy+on~ a daddy Clark shem28a.cha Shem 2:10.2 fir~+chief car the cae belonging to che fire chief Clark shem28a.cha Shem 2;10.2 fir~+mgine on~ a fire engine Clark shem28a.cha Shem 2;10.2 fir~+r~scue a rescue from a fire Clark shem28a.cha Shem 2:10.2 go+light a green craffic lighc Clark shem28a.cha Shem 2:10.2 scratch Hhing.r claws Clark shem28a.cha Shem 2:10.2 smoke +mask a smoke fileration mask Clark shem28b.cha Shem 2:10.2 tru+branch ehe branch of a tree ", Kuczaj abe050.cha Abe 2:10.22 willy +willy a ching chac sometimes you squeeze and someeimes you don'c 5 Brown ad~m17.cha Adam 2il0.30 airplan~ [N] ?? Brown adaml7.cha Adam 2:10.30 Buddy+Shaddy [proper name] ?? 2 Kuczaj abe052.cha. -115.cha Abe 2:10.30 corn +butter [N] ?? 4 Brown adam 17.cha. -18.cha. -22.cha. -23.cha. -25.cha. -26.cha. -28.cha. -29.cha Adam 2ilO.303:04.18 ShaJow+Gay [proper name] 40 Clark shem31 b.cha Shem 2; 11.1 ey~s+brow eyebrows 1 Clark shem31 a.cha Shem 2;11.1 jumping+trick a crick involving jumping 1 Clark shem31 a.cha Shem 2:11.1 snow+ball+man a snowman 3 Clark shem32b.cha Shem 2:11.10 (pits)Hap~ ?? 1 Clark shem32a.cha Shem 2;11.10 barbuue +thing a barbecue grill Clark shem32a.cha Shem 2;11.10 cello [0 shooter Towards an explication ofthe role ofchild. language in language change # play the cello Page 33 researcher file speaker age word gloss # Clark shem32b.cha Shem 2:11.10 mist+har~ Mister 1 Clark shem32b.cha Shem 2:11.10 nUT1+tr~ss~s ?? 1 Clark shem32b.cha Shem 2;11.10 strut+walk a crosswalk ?? Brown adaml8.cha Adam 2;11.13 tig" monkrys striped monkeys ?? Kuc:za; abe053.cha Abe 2;11.2 daddy+doggy+goos~ [N] ?? Kuczaj abe053.cha Abe 2;11.2 pint +corn [N] ?? 1 Kuczaj abe058.cha Abe 2;11.21 bong [N] ?? 5 Kuc:zaj abe058.cha Abe 2:11.21 bong+kick a game like bong-marble where you kick instead 2 Kucza; abe058.cha Abe 2;11.21 bong+marb/~ a game played with a marble 5 Kucza; abe058.cha Abe 2;11.21 bunny+bing [N] ?? 1 Kuc:zaj abe059.cha Abe 2;11.25 lonry upset Clark shem33.cha Shem 2:11.28 (garag~)+on~ something that belongs in a garage Clark shem33.cha Shem 2;11.28 baby+one a baby Clark shem33.cha Shem 2;11.28 browni~+{shut) a cookie sheet 1 Wells france08.cha Frances 2:11.28 bum+bum ?? 2 Clark shem33.cha Shem 2;11.28 carrot jce+cr~am carrot-flavoured ice cream Clark shem33.cha Shem 2;11.28 dinn"+thin!,1 dinner dishes Clark shem33.cha Shem 2;11.28 fosun~r+n~t a net to hang things up with 2 Clark shem33.cha Shem 2;11.28 hiding+on~s [N] ?? 2 Brown adaml9.cha Adam 2;11.28 Jack+a+w~asel probably a Jack-in-the-box. (a blend from Jack-in-th~box and pop go~s th~ wease/?) 3 Clark shem33.cha Shem 2:11.28 mask+fou+{thing) a ma."k 1 Clark shem33.cha Shem 2:11.28 moth~r+froggj~ a frog who is a mother 2 Clark shem33.cha Shem 2:11.28 muffin +(thing) a muffin pan Brown adam19.cha Adam 2;11.28 spac~ Brown adaml9.cha Adam 2:11.28 spau+chart a space-chart Wells france08.cha Frances 2:11.28 Up+a+doo ?? Wells ben;am08.cha Benjamin 2:11.29 [rinzy bob [Nj ?? ? scann" Towards an explication of the role ofchild language in language change a thing that scans space 1 1 1 2 Page 34 -researcher file speaker age word gloss # Kucza; abeo60.cha Abe 2;11.30 ca"ot+h~ad a toy with a carrot for a head (cf. Mr. Potato H~ad) 3 Kucza; abe054.cha Abe 2;11.6 (king+)burg~r+hat a hat from Burger-King 1 Brown adam20.cha Adam 3;0.11 hous~+bird a bird that lives in or around houses ?? 2 Brown adam20.cha Adam 3;0.11 monkey train a train that carries monkeys 1 ?? Clark shem35.cha Shem 3;0.13 (b~rry) +thing a berry Clark shem35.cha Shem 3:0.13 broom if a verb. [0 sweep. if not. ignore th is word 3 Clark shem35.cha Shem 3;0.13 catch+go ?? 2 Clark shem35.cha Shem 3;0.13 dumping +place a place for dumping things 1 Clark shem35.cha Shem 3;0.13 j~t+mak~r [N] ?? 1 Clark shem35.cha Shem 3;0.13 m~at+cak~ a kind of cake made with meat Clark shem35.cha Shem 3:0.13 pia+stic plastic ?? Snow nathl5.cha Nathan 3;0.19 Michigro [A] ?? Clark shem36.cha Shem 3;0.20 babbing+place [N] ?? Clark shem36.cha Shem 3;0.20 bas~bal/+things baseball equipment Clark shem36.cha Shem 3;0.20 chi/drm+bed a bed where children sleep Clark shem36.cha Shem 3;0.20 dishwaur+stujf dish detergent Clark shem36.cha Shem 3;0.20 flmi/y+house the game house Clark shem36.cha Shem 3;0.20 flmi/y+house a house where a family lives Clark shem36.cha Shem 3;0.20 flag+place [N] ?? 1 Clark shem36.cha Shem 3;0.20 flag+store a store where you buy flags 1 Clark shem36.cha Shem 3;0.20 opm c!ran c/osing+thing a thing which opens and closes Clark shem36.cha Shem 3;0.20 play+dough ones things made of play-dough Clark shem36.cha Shem 3;0.20 schoolHruck school bus ?? Clark shem36.cha Shem 3;0.20 tenniHthingr tennL~ Fletcher 3:flkir.cha Kirsty 3;0.23 drming+up Kuczaj abe063.cha Abe 3;0.25 anagandzanrkrs [N] ?? Brown adam21.cha Adam 3;0.25 punch +ball a ball for punching Towards an explication ofthe role ofchild language in language change c/oth~s equipment clothes for dressing up in 1 2 1 1 Page 35 researcher file speaker age word gloss Brown adam21.cha Adam, 3;0.25 ro/lingHteam ? steamroller ?? Brown adam21.cha. -25.cha Adam 3;0.253;02.21 Do+Bu [proper name] ?? 4 Clark shem34.cha Shem 3;0.5 bin/ry+maker [N] ?? 2 Clark shem34.cha Shem 3:0.5 cookie +bread cake ?? 1 Clark shem34.cha Shem 3;0.5 doaor +things medical gear Clark shern34.cha Shem 3;0.5 ear +things things that go in your ear Clark shem34.cha Shem 3;0.5 mouth+thing a thing that goes in your mouth Clark shem34.cha Shem 3:0.5 p/anet+one a book about a planet 1 Clark shem34.cha Shem 3:0.5 point+things either pointed things or things to point with 1 Clark shem34.cha Shem 3;0.5 record+thing the speaker cable from the record player ?? 1 Kuczaj abe061.cha Abe 3:0.7 grumb/e+gud [N] ?? Kuczaj abe061.cha Abe 3;0.7 grumb/e+head a person who grumbles a lot Wells betty08.cha Betty 3:0.9 Mom+mom a grandmother 2 Kuczaj abe065.cha Abe 3:01.1 tinka+tonka ?? 2 Kuczaj abe068.cha Abe 3;01.11 moozer a thing that stays right in the moon but is not green 2 Kuczaj abe068.cha Abe 3:01.11 moozers they looked like two moozers two moozers that they looked like Clark shem38.cha Shem 3;01.13 finger+print finger-paint Clark shem38.cha Shem 3:01.13 re+eat eat again ?? Brown sarah045.cha Sarah 3:01.17 shika+burds ?? Kuczaj abe071.cha Abe 3:01.22 ding/e+turkry nickname Brown adam23.cha Adam 3:01.26 Harvard+Square bus the bus going to Harvard Square ?? Clark shem39.cha Shem 3:01.27 choco/au +medicine a medicine made from chocolate ?? Clark shem39.cha Shem 3:01.27 ugo+box a box you keep legos in Clark shem39.cha Shem 3:01.27 mun +sisur +... [N] ?? Clark shem39.cha Shem 3:01.27 poking+thing an unidentified tool Clark shem39.cha Shem 3:01.27 tinker+box a box you keep tinker-toys # 1 7 2 5 2 m Towards an explication ofthe role ofchild language in language change Page 36 researcher file speaker age word gloss # Clark shem37.cha Shem 3;01.5 bear+monster a monster that is like a bear 1 Clark shem37.cha Shem 3;01.5 card+board [N] ?? ... Clark shem37.cha Shem 3;01.5 hitting+tree [N] ?? 1 Clark shem37.cha Shem 3;01.5 marJut+guse a thing (a goose) that shows you how [0 get some geese 1 Clark shem37.cha Shem 3;01.5 pet+doggie a dog that is someone's pet 2 Clark shem37.cha Shem 3;01.5 pool+geese geese that are in a pool Clark shem37.cha Shem 3;01.5 pounds heavy (in pounds) 1 Clark shem37.cha Shem 3;01.5 simng+place a place where you sit 2 Clark shem37.cha Shem 3;01.5 starfish +book a book about starfish 1 Clark shem37.cha Shem 3;01.5 toy+geese either small geese or pretend ones Clark shem37.cha Shem 3;01.5 toy +house a dollhouse ?? Clark shem37.cha Shem 3;01.5 tree+pants ?? Clark shem37.cha Shem 3:01.5 whistling+part the part (of a song ?) where you whistle 1 Brown sarah048.cha Sarah 3;02.10 roller [n which can be bicycled upon] 1 Clark shem40.cha Shem 3;02.2 car+gate the gate cars go through ' MacWhinney ross35.cha Ross 3;02.2 car+Santa ' [N1 ?? Clark shem40.cha Shem 3;02.2 easter+bunny+rabbit the easter bunny Clark shem40.cha Shem 3;02.2 flod+lady a lady who gives you food ?? Clark shem40.cha Shem 3:02.2 fri!bu+time [N1 ?? MacWhinney ross35.cha Ross 3;02.2 one+one+money [N1 ?? Clark shem40.cha Shem 3:02.2 people+mouth a human mouth Clark shem40.cha Shem 3:02.2 stick+things either sticks or things that you stick with MacWhinney ross36.cha Ross 3:02.23 bottom top the lowest top part MacWhinney ross36.cha Ross 3:02.23 up +top the uppermost top parr Brown sarah050.cha, -052.cha Sarah 3;02.233;03.7 Loop +loops [N1 ?? 5 Kuczaj abe080.cha Abe 3;02.26 !hlom [N1 ?? 2 Snow nath18.cha Nathan 3;02.27 konka medicine [N] ?? Snow narhl8.cha Nathan 3;02.27 stumpa ?? Towards an explication ofthe role ofchild language in language change 2 1 1 1 1 Page 37 # researcher file speaker age word gloss Wells jonath09.cha Jonathan 3;02.28 nig+nog ?? Wells jonath09.cha Jonathan 3:02.28 nig+1UJg biscuit a biscuit that is either made 1 with nig-nogs[N] or is nignog(A) Kuczaj abe081.cha Abe 3;02.29 firing burning Kuczaj abe081.cha Abe 3;02.29 fist to punch Kuczaj abe081.cha Abe 3;02.29 poonk [V] ?? 2 Kuczaj abe081.cha Abe 3;02.29 s/ipp(~r)yd slippery 1 Kuczaj abe081.cha Abe 3:02.29 unsttd happy. as when you don't 5 1 have to go to school Brown adam24.cha Adam 3;02.9 string master [N] ?? 2 Brown adam24.cha Adam 3;02.9 strong master ? [N] ?? 4 Garvey pegron.cha Ron 3;03 Batbi/~ Batmobile 2 Gathercole Il.cha Erin 3;03 fishy+food fish-food 1 Garvey kimada.cha Ada 3;03 no+nam~ mayb~ ?? 1 Kuczaj abe082.cha Abe 3;03.1 catt/~+fish a fish chat is like cows ?? 1 (a play on catfish) Kucza; abe082.cha Abe 3;03.1 corUr+cat [N] ?? 2 Kuczaj abe082.cha Abe 3;03.1 dog+fish possibly a shark, but more 2 likely a play on catfish Kuczaj abe082.cha Abe 3;03.1 monsku~ an animal that likes to slide on mooSes b Kucza; abe082.cha. Abe 3;03.1 mooS~b an animal that eats trolls 5 -087.cha Kucza; abe082.cha Abe 3;03.1 padd/~+fish a fish that paddles ?? (a play on catfish) 1 Kuczaj abe085.cha Abe 3:03.11 A/ou [name of a river] 3 Kuczaj abe085.cha Abe 3;03.11 malou a kind of turtle that live... in [he river Alou 1 Kuczaj abe085.cha Abe 3;03.11 moonchin [animal] ?? Kuczaj abe088.cha Abe 3:03.25 yalla+candy a kind of candy like marshmallows that is soft and comes on sticks 3 Sachs n83.cha Naomi 3:03.27 mak~+b~/iro~ fish~s make-believe fishes 1 Brown adam26.cha Adam 3;03.4 a football 4 Brown adam26.cha Adam 3;03.4 a thing for measuring with 2 floti~+ ba//i~ m~asur~r Towards an explication ofthf rolf ofchild languagf in languagf changf Pagf38 researcher file speaker age word gloss Kucza; abe083.cha Abe 3:03.4 shoo+fly a fly Brown sarah052.cha Sarah 3:03.7 pu+pot either a bedpan or a child's training toilet ?? Kuczaj abe084.cha Abe 3:03.8 crisS+C7'oss cookie a cookie wich a crosshatched pattern on top (perhaps peanut butter ?) 1 Kuczaj abe084.cha Abe 3:03.8 fishmay a person who is where mean crocodiles bite you and who tells you how to get them 3 Gathercole 13.cha Erin 3:04 bonk to Kuczaj abe090.cha Abe 3:04.1 ra.ss a jail ?? Brown adam28.cha Adam 3:04.1 cake flrk+knift [N] ?? Brown adam28.cha Adam 3:04.1 larger a ching that makes things larger Brown adam28.cha Adam 3:04.1 mIXer rowash Brown adam28.cha Adam 3;04.1 mixer cakes [N] ?? Brown adam28.cha Adam 3:04.1 nothing monster either a monster with no distinguishing characteristics or one which is norhing 1 Brown adam28.cha Adam 3;04.1 opener a ching for opening things wich 1 Brown sarahO 56.cha Sarah 3:04.1 pincher something that pinches 1 Kuczaj abe090.cha Abe 3;04.1 rass [N] ?? 1 Brown adam28.cha Adam 3:04.1 water hydrant a fire hydrant Brown adam28.cha Adam 3:04.1 waur+Ua either fake tea-party tea (ie water) or tea made from wacer 5 Snow nath23.cha Nathan 3:04.10 dinah a dinosaur 4 Snow nath23.cha Nathan 3;04.10 !IIur a dinosaur 3 Kuczaj abe094.cha Abe 3;04.15 noiu+maker a thing that makes a lot of noise 1 Brown adam29.cha Adam 3;04.18 bucket roller ?? 3 Brown adam29.cha Adam 3;04.18 dark +time a time when it's dark, 1 # sit suddenly 2 1 probably night Brown adam29.cha Adam 3;04.18 stuloscope Towards an explication ofthe role ofchild language in language change [N] ?? Page 39 researcher file speaker age word gloss # Brown adam29.cha. -34.cha Adam 3;04.183;07.7 poppfr a person who pops (things ??) 2 Snow nath27.cha Nathan 3;04.21 handy +dandy handy-d.andy 1 Kuczaj abe097.cha Abe 3;04.26 god+blm+yous kleenex 3 Kuczaj abe097.cha Abe 3;04.26 tmnis to hit with a tennis racket 2 Brown sarah059.cha Sarah 3;04.26 wat" Slfd a sled that goes on the water 1 ?? Kuczaj abe091.cha Abe 3;04.4 hawk+trap a trap for catching hawks (cf. mousetrap) 4 Kuczaj abe091.cha Abe 3;04.4 micf +traps mousetraps 2 Kuczaj abe091.cha Abe 3;04.4 stink+hfat/ [insult-name] Brown adam30.cha Adam 3;05.0 buckft loader a thing which loads buckets ?? 1 Brown adam30.cha Adam 3;05.0 mix" truck a cement mixer 3 MacWhinney ross40.cha Ross 3;05.11 aspirin +chitis ?? 1 MacWhinney ross40.cha Ross 3;05.11 good+chitis [N] ?? 1 MacWhinney ross40.cha Ross 3;05.11 good+chitis [N] ?? MacWhinney ross40.cha Ross 3;05.11 nfw+chitis [N] ?? MacWhinney ross40.cha Ross 3;05.11 right+chitis [N] ?? 1 MacWhinney ross40.cha Ross 3;05.11 wrong+chitis [N] ?? 2 MacWhinney ross41.cha. Ross 3;05.11 3;07.4 COkf+a+co/a Coca-cola 2 -43.cha Brown sarah062.cha Sarah 3;05.13 blowfr something that blows Brown adam31.cha Adam 3;05.15 tick+tock [N] ?? Brown adam31.cha Adam 3;05.15 tick+tock ?? Kuczaj abel02.cha Abe 3;05.17 (a)risty+cat aristocrat ?? 3 Brown sarah063.cha Sarah 3;05.20 trapper [N] ?? 1 Wells nevilll0.cha Nevill 3;05.27 tow+jo [N] ?? Brown adam33.cha Adam 3;05.29 fish catchfr a thing which catches fish 1 Brown adam33.cha Adam 3;05.29 spacf+boat a boat that goes in space 1 Kucza; abel00.cha Abe 3;05.6 not+bladf either a thing that is not a blade or the place where a blade isn'r Brown sarah061.cha Sarah 3;05.7 Baby +Bright [name of a doll] ?? 13rown sarah061.cha Sarah 3;05.7 cufffr one a ching that cuts Towards an explication ofthe role ofchild language in fIlnguage change 1 Page 40 researcher file speaker age word gloss # Sachs n89.cha Naomi 3:05.7 sir+bu shirts [shin] ?? 1 Kucza; abell1.cha Abe 3:06.19 stringablob [N] ?? 1 Kucza; abel13.cha Abe 3:06.22 k~rsplod~ to explode without huning anyone 6 Kucza; abe113.cha Abe 3:06.22 y~surnight yesterday night 1 Brown sarah066.cha Sarah 3:06.23 roast cutUr either a thing for cutting roasts or a cutter that has been roasted 2 Kucza; abel07.cha Abe 3;06.4 n~ws+guy the newscaster 1 Kucza; abel19.cha Abe 3:07.22 Army+Joe GI Joe 1 MacWhinney boys61.cha Mark 3;07.22 bare+footing bare-footedness or bare feet 2 MacWhinney boys61.cha Mark 3;07.22 cUl+off amputated 1 MacWhinney boys61.cha Mark 3:07.22 fut+man a superhero (probably with bare feet) and any game where you play him 7 Kuczaj abe119.cha Abe 3;07.22 karate to hit using karate 3 MacWhinney boys61.cha Mark 3:07.22 pig+guard a pig who is a guard 1 Kuczaj abel19.cha Abe 3:07.22 plAntdilly [teasing name] 4 Brown sarah070.cha. -lOS.cha Sarah 3;07.234;04.18 Crazy+Foam [N] ?? 6 Kuczaj abel I S.cha Abe 3;07.4 b~aur to beat Brown adam34.cha Adam 3;07.7 wiggk+worm a worm Brown adam3S.cha Adam 3;08.0 axe hammer a hammer thac is an axe (a roofing hatchet ??) 1 Brown adam3S.cha Adam 3;08.0 cam" puzzle a puzzle that carries people 1 Brown adam3S.cha Adam 3;08.0 chopper a thing for chopping things down 1 Brown adam3S.cha Adam 3;08.0 cutUr a thing that cuts (perhaps a saw? cf. adam3S.cha. line 1492) 4 Brown adam3S.cha Adam 3;08.0 finger p~ople [N] ?? Brown adam3S.cha Adam 3;08.0 finger to~ a toe ?? 3 Brown adam35.cha Adam 3:08.0 mixer man [N] ?? 1 Brown adam35.cha Adam 3;08.0 screwer a person who screws things 1 Brown adam35.cha Adam 3:08.0 tiger sontJ songs tigers sing ?? 1 Towards an explication ofthe role ofchild languagt in /anguagt change Pagt41 researcher file speaker age word gloss # Brown adam36.cha Adam 3:08.14 throwfr a person who throws things 1 MacWhinney ross45.cha Ross 3:08.18 dog+horu [animal] ?? 1 MacWhinney boys63.cha Mark 3:08.18 Light+Light [name of a mouse] 1 MacWhinney boys63.cha Mark 3:08.18 picky+uppy a piggy-back ride 2 MacWhinney boys63.cha Mark 3:08.18 Shump+Shump [name of a mouse] 2 MacWhinney boys63.cha Mark 3:08.18 Vluffy [name of a wasp) 1 Brown adam37.cha Adam 3:08.26 boy fuh a fish that is like a boy 1 Brown adam37.cha Adam 3:08.26 cowboy mak" a person who makes cowboys 1 Brown adam37.cha Adam 3:08.26 fing" fish a fish that is like a finger Brown adam37.cha Adam 3:08.26 glovf hand the hand a glove is worn on Brown adam37.cha Adam 3:08.26 lamb moth" either the mother of a lamb, 2 or a mother who is a lamb Brown adam37.cha Adam 3:08.26 tOf fing" a finger ?? (toe finger and finger toe seem to be used imerchangeably, so I'm not sure about the headedness of this one) Brown adam37.cha Adam 3:08.26 whale fish either a whale, or a fish that is like a whale 2 Brown sarah075.cha Sarah 3:08.27 train+tray+ching [N] ?? 2 Gathercole 08.cha Megan 3:09 nap p"(son) a person who takes naps Gathercole 08.cha Sarah 3:09 stick ... to stick ... out MacWhinney boys79.cha Mark 3:09.13 fon+tim~ the time when you have fun (at school ??) MacWhinney boys79.cha Mark 3:09.13 work+tjm~ the time when you do work (at school ??) Kuczaj abel34.cha Abe 3;09.14 /awn +mower to mow with a lawn-mower Brown sarah077 .cha Sarah 3:09.18 Sugar+Babi~s [candy] ?? Kuczaj abel35.cha Abe 3:09.19 Jmooth~r a thing that makes things smooth Kuczaj abel36.cha Abe 3:09.23 humnrycuck [Nj ?? MacWhinney boys64.cha Mark 3:09.26 ov"+float to over-flow MacWhinney boys64.cha Mark 3:09.26 walkie +talk to talk on a walkie-talkie Gathercole 10.cha Megan 3:10 pop+in+law a father-in-law Towards an explication ofthe role ofchild language in kznguage change 1 1 2 Page 42 researcher file speaker age word gloss Kuczaj abel 42.cha Abe 3;10.14 hanktx a handkerchief (a blend from handkerchief and kleenex) Kuczaj abe I 42.cha Abe 3;10.14 slab+rock a smooth rock you can make arrowheads with 4 Kuczaj abel 43.cha Abe 3;10.15 cowed [N] ?? 3 Brown adam39.cha Adam 3;10.15 scoot" a thing chac scoots (unclear whether it is the vehicle) 2 Kuczaj abel 43.cha Abe 3;10.15 spearrock a cock shaped like an arrowhead 1 Kuczaj abel43.cha Abe 3;10.15 sword" a fencer 2 MacWhinney boys65.cha Mack 3;10.20 heal stuff stuff chac heals people 1 MacWhinney boys80.cha Mack 3;10.22 super+duper(ed) in a manner suggestive of 5 # possessing super powers MacWhinney boys81.cha Mack 3;10.28 raisin +branios flakes of raisin-bran 1 Kuczaj abe139.cha Abe 3;10.3 teller the person who tells you what to do 2 MacWhinney boys66.cha Mark 3;10.6 caterki//er a caterpillar ?? 2 MacWhinney boys66.cha Mark 3;10.6 gargle+man perhaps name of another superhero ?? 3 MacWhinney boys66.cha Mack 3;10.6 hand+cuffided to handcuff 1 Kuczaj abel46.cha Abe 3;11.0 dinosaur+land a land where dinosaurs live 2 Kuczaj abel 46.cha Abe 3;11.0 gooderest best 1 MacWhinney boys67.cha Mark 3;11.18 Linry+man [proper name] MacWhinney boys67.cha Mack 3:11.18 Ridey+man [proper name] MacWhinney boys67.cha Mark 3:11.18 Sleepy+wo/f [proper name] 2 Brown adam42.cha Adam 4;0.14 arch" [N] ?? 1 Brown sarah088.cha Sarah 4:0.14 trac" ?? MacWhinney boys83in.cha Mack 4;0.24 Creepy +craw/ys [N] ?? 1 Brown sarah091.cha Sarah 4;01.11 f/ip+f/op ?? 2 Brown adam43.cha Adam 4;01.15 mgine"man an engineer Brown adam43.cha Adam 4;01.15 rubb" band man a man who is like a rubber band or who has lots of rubber bands 1 Brown sarah092.cha Sarah 4;01.18 Charmin(g) + Chatty [N] ?? 2 Towards an explication ofthe role ofchild language in language change Page 43 researcher file speaker age word gloss # Brown sarah092.cha Sarah 4:01.18 Chatty +Charming (possibly analogous to Prince Charming ?? ) 2 Brown sarah092.cha S~ 4;01.18 topp~r [N] ?? 2 Kuczaj abe163.cha Abe 4:01.29 hegainst caused to run against 1 Kuczaj abe163.cha, -166.cha, -167.cha, -169.cha, -I72.cha, -174.cha, -191.cha, Abe 4:01.29 4:08.2 Chop +wood [name of an action figure] 36 MacWhinney boys68.cha Mark 4:01.5 jesuHguy [N] ?? 1 Kuczaj abe159.cha Abe 4:01.9 crumpks soap shavings 1 Gleason dinner: david.cha David 4:02.1 Sam+l+am the book G"m Eggs and 1 Brown adam44.cha Adam 4:02.17 airman drivn- [N] ?? 1 Brown adam44.cha Adam 4:02.17 airport drivn- [N] ?? 1 Brown adam44.cha Adam 4:02.17 driller a person who drills Brown adam44.cha Adam 4:02.17 push" someone who pushes people Brown adam44.cha Adam 4;02.17 water moving ma(hin~ a machine for moving water Kuczaj abel67.cha Abe 4;02.19 Thunder +laying+ wood [proper name] 3 Kuczaj abe167.cha Abe 4:02.19 Thundn-+light [proper name, apparently with the same referent as Thunder-laying-wood] 1 MacWhinney boys70.cha Mark 4:02.2 harr~d+thingy ?? (cage ?) 1 Kuczaj abe168.cha Abe 4:02.24 refridges food kept in a refridgerator 1 MacWhinney boys71.cha Mark 4;02.28 haking+man a baker 2 MacWhinney boys71.cha Mark 4;02.28 coo/ry+man either a chef of a cookiebaker 2 MacWhinney boys71.cha Mark 4:02.28 Hungry+ Wolf [proper name] 1 MacWhinney boys71.cha Mark 4:02.28 Lazy + Wolf [proper name] 1 MacWhinney boys71.cha Mark 4:02.28 rough+up [action] 3 MacWhinney boys71.cha Mark 4:02.28 runny +man a man who runs ?? MacWhinney boys71.cha Mark 4:02.28 saw+man a man who saws 1 MacWhinney boys71.cha Mark 4:02.28 swim+man a man who swims 1 MacWhinney boys71.cha Mark 4:02.28 teaching+lady a lady who is a teacher 1 Ham Towards an txplication ofthe role ofchild language in language change Page 44 # researcher file speaker age word gloss Gleason mother: david.cha David 4;02.3 Mr Gathercole 10.cha . Lily 4;03 Sarahgots Sarah has 2 Kuczaj abel69.cha Abe 4;03.1 spac~+boards [N] ?? 2 Man+~ating fish [proper name] (perhaps like surfboards for space ??) Kuczaj abel 69.cha Abe 4;03.1 sw~~pm~d swept 1 Kuczaj abel 69.cha Abe 4;03.1 upsid~+up right-side-u p 1 Brown adam47.cha Adam 4;03.13 cutUr someone who cuts Brown adam47.cha Adam 4:03.13 m~chanical Brown adam47.cha Adam 4;03.13 pen Kuczaj abe 172.cha Abe 4;03.15 Kuczaj abel 72.cha Abe Kuczaj abel 72.cha Kuczaj writer a mechanical writer 1 a pen for writing with ?? 1 Thund" +wood [proper name. apparendy with the same referent as Thunder-laying-wood] 2 4;03.15 w~igh~r a thing you weigh things with 2 Abe 4;03.15 w~ight~r a thing you find the weight of things with 1 abel73.cha Abe 4;03.21 red ready 7 Brown sarah102.cha Sarah 4;03.26 tru+top ?? Brown sarah099.cha Sarah 4:03.7 stoppity+sloppity ?? Brown adam45.cha Adam 4:03.9 cowboy driv" [N] ?? Brown adam45.cha Adam 4;03.9 magic+four ?? 7 Brown adam45.cha Adam 4;03.9 police driver [N] ?? 1 Brown sarah 103.cha Sarah 4;04.1 start" [N] ?? 1 Kuczaj abel76.cha Abe 4;04.21 ~arsight hearing 1 Brown sarah 106.cha Sarah 4;04.25 clap+clap hands [N] ?? 2 Gathercole 15.cha Lily 4;05 tuny something small ?? 1 Brown sarah 109.cha Sarah 4;05.14 Baby+Boo [name of a doll] ?? 4 Kuczaj abel78.cha Abe 4;05.14 ft~tcuffi leg irons 3 Brown sarah 108.cha Sarah 4;05.8 doo+bee [N] ?? 3 Brown sarah 108.cha Sarah 4;05.8 wall+tainer ?? 2 Gathercole 06.cha Gil 4;06 bunk ?? 1 MacWhinney ross53.cha Ross 4;06.1 work+job a job writ~r Towards an explication ofthe role ofchild language in language change 4 Page 45 researcher file speaker age word gloss Kuczaj abe184.cha Abe 4:06.14 blut+jtan shorts cutoffs ?? Brown adam49.cha Adam 4:06.24 bird dinn" a dinner for a bird 1 Brown adam49.cha Adam 4:06.24 chocoUzu blast" [N] ?? 1 Brown adam49.cha Adam 4:06.24 cOvn'+bag [Nl ?? Brown adam49.cha Adam 4:06.24 someone who digs Brown adam49.cha Adam 4:06.24 digg" flying sauctr dinosaur Brown adam49.cha Adam 4:06.24 rac" [N] ?? Brown adam49.cha Adam 4:06.24 tht spillntr something that spins Kuczaj abel 86.cha Abe 4:06.27 outsidt+out the opposite of inside-out Brown adam50.cha Adam 4:07.0 can't+ull+boy a boy who can't tell (the difference between two things) Brown adam50.cha Adam 4;07.0 gun shoot" [N] ?? Brown adam50.cha Adam 4:07.0 gun+pm a pen that is also a gun (from James Bond ?) Brown sarahl16.cha Sarah 4;07.0 hop +bang ?? 3 Brown adam50.cha Adam 4;07.0 ull+boy a boy who tells people things or who can tell (the difference between two things). probably the latter 2 Brown adam50.cha Adam 4:07.0 train +go +round either a merry-go-round or a trolley roundabout 1 MacWhinney boys77.cha Mark 4;07.10 stick to hit with a stick MacWhinney boys77.cha Mark 4;07.10 und"+arm the skin under your arm Brown sarah 1 17.cha Sarah 4:07.11 Misur+G [proper name] Brown sarah 121.cha Sarah 4:08.13 sticktrbird [bird] ?? Brown sarah 121.cha Sarah 4:08.13 tot+bow [N] ?? 1 Kuczaj abe191.cha Abe 4:08.2 tyt+out ?? 2 Kuczaj abel 94.cha Abe 4:08.20 infinity infinitely many 4 Brown sarah 122.cha Sarah 4:08.20 ping+pong little (and round ?) 1 # 1 a dinosaur in a flying saucer 2 ?? 1 1 5 (adj. describing a nose) Brown sarah 122.cha Sarah 4:08.20 two+poinud having two points Brown sarah 120.cha Sarah 4:08.7 basky+back ?? Brown sarah 120.cha Sarah 4:08.7 masky+bat ?? 1 Brown sarah 124.cha Sarah 4:09.12 btlly +bat+posttr [N) ?? 2 Towards an txplication ofthe role ofchild language in language change 2 Page 46 researcher file speaker age word Brown sarah 124.cha Sarah 4:09.12 daddy Brown sarah 124.cha Sarah 4:09.12 dog moth~r either the mother of a dog or a mother who is a dog :2 Kuczaj abel98.cha Abe 4:09.19 policutown +guy [N] ?? 1 Kuczaj abe 198.cha Abe 4:09.19 town+guy a guy from town ?? Wells ellen21.cha Ellen 4:09.22 catty a watering can Kuczaj abel99.cha Abe 4:09.24 thumb/~ ??? Gathercole 06.cha Matthew 4:10 cav~+shap~d cave-shaped Gathercole 06.cha Matthew 4:10 Popry~ th~ multiple instances of Popeye Brown adam55.cha Adam 4:10.23 grain thrower something which throws grain 6 Brown adam55.cha Adam 4:10.23 grainer having more grain 1 Brown adam55.cha Adam 4:10.23 hand+buckler ? either a buckle for your hand or a small shield (unlikely) 1 Brown adam55.cha Adam 4;10.23 monsUr a magnet which attracts 6 # gloss moth~r sailor mans magn~t a daddy who is a mother 1 monsters Brown adam55.cha Adam 4:10.23 record+phon~ an answering machine ?? Brown adam55.cha Adam 4:10.23 wat~rthing a thing that can go in water ?? Brown sarah 129.cha Sarah 4;10.27 FIy{(r/+a)+kiu a kice chac makes you fly if you hold its string (from the song. "Let's go fly a kice" in Mary Poppins ?) 4 Kuczaj abe201.cha Abe 4:10.9 ajter+vitamin a pill you cake after a vitamin (which is noc itself a vitamin) 2 Garvey glojoy.cha Glo 5:0 dresHups clothes for dressing up in 1 Brown sarah 136.cha Sarah 5:0.16 squ~akr someone who squeals 3 Fletcher 5:hnclah.cha Claire 5:02 tmd ducks imaginary ducks ?? Brown adam52.cha Adam 5:02.12 sliding+stairs either an escalator or stairs for sliding down. probably che former MacWhinney boys87.cha Mark 5;04.14 blob +baby ?? Towards an ~plication ofthe role ofchild language in language change 4 7 Page 47 researcher file speaker age word gloss # MacWhinney boys87.cha Mark 5;04.14 cold+base [N] ?? 1 MacWhinney boys87.cha Mark 5;04.14 cove+bau [N] ?? MacWhinney boys87.cha Mark 5;04.14 Easter Ewok an ewok who delivers easter baskets MacWhinney boys87.cha Mark 5;04.14 fixer +man a man who fIxes things MacWhinney ross60.cha Ross 5;04.20 banana +brain [insult name) MacWhinney ross60.cha Ross 5;04.20 fiftJ+armed having fIfty arms MacWhinney ross60.cha Ross 5;04.20 fiftJ+forty+five+armed having fifty+forty+five arms MacWhinney ross60.cha Ross 5;04.20 fifty +four+five [number] ?? MaeWhinney ross60.cha Ross 5;04.20 flur+armed having four arms MacWhinney ross60.cha Ross 5;04.20 klum1J+head [insult] MacWhinney ross60.cha Ross 5;04.20 Long+armed+man [proper name] MacWhinney ross60.cha Ross 5;04.20 Long+Arms nickname 4 MacWhinney ross60.cha Ross 5;04.20 Underroo's+Man a superhero who wears underroos. a friend of feetmans 1 1 1 I MacWhinney ross60.cha Ross 5;04.20 water+gun a water pistol MacWhinney boys84.cha Mark 5;04.23 carrying+upstairser the person who carries something upstairs MacWhinney boys84.cha Mark 5;04.23 ghostm +monsters ghosts ?? MacWhinney boys84.cha Mark 5;04.23 snow +time the winrer;when it snows MacWhinney boys84.cha Mark 5;04.23 spud+fost very quickly MacWhinney boys84.cha Mark 5;04.23 un +loou to tighten MacWhinney boys61.cha Ross 5;06.16 brownish+orange brown ish +orange MacWhinney boys89.cha Mark 5;06.9 Big+Rope [proper name] 2 MacWhinney boys89.cha Mark 5;06.9 Little +Rope [proper name] 2 MacWhinney boys89.cha Mark 5;06.9 Medium+siu+Rope [proper name] 2 MacWhinney boys89.cha Mark 5;06.9 one +eye one-eyed 1 MacWhinney boys89.cha Mark 5;06.9 su+rye+dog a seeing-eye dog 2 Gathercole 07.cha Nicole 5;07 crackier more cracked ?? Gathercole 07.cha Nicole 5;07 re+tape recorder a tape recorder Gathercole 07.cha Nicole 5;07 smush [0 Towards an explication ofthe role ofchild language in language change squish Page 48 # researcher file speaker age word gloss MacWhinney boys90.cha Mark 5j07.23 Hot+wir~ [N] ?? MacWhinney boys90.cha Mark 5j07.23 ow+wir~ [N] ?? 1 MacWhinney boys90.cha Mark 5j07.23 sup"+bar a candybar chac gives you 1 super powers MacWhinney boys66.cha Ross 5j08.31 tam" [N] ?? 1 MacWhinney boys65.cha Ross 5j09.14 Radio+Star+ Wars che NPR producrion of Star 2 Wars MacWhinney boys81.cha Ross 5j09.24 iC(+br~ak~r someching chac breaks ice 1 MacWhinney boys81.cha Ross 5j09.24 ict+cracktr someching thar cracks ice 1 MacWhinney boys80.cha Ross 5j09.6 human +was ex-human-beings 3 MacWhinney boys82.cha Ross 5jlO.18 Bad+nos~ Bill [proper name] 1 MacWhinney boys82.cha Ross 5;10.18 room a person who keeps up a 1 kup~r+upp~r room MacWhinney ross 57.cha Ross 5;11.24 Boy+oh+brothtrs ?? 1 Evans dyad20.cha Jack 6:0.26 pizza + fingtr + slic~ ?? 1 Evans dyad20.cha Jack 6:0.26 pizza+mak" someone who makes pizzas 1 MacWhinney ross58.cha Ross 6j01.20 Jump+ovtr+hand [0 jump over your hand 2 MacWhinney boys71.cha Ross 6;01.23 pur+ray ?? MacWhinney boys71.cha Ross 6;01.23 up+and+away [N] ?? Warren sandra.cha Sandra 6;02.0 honHnut bet [N] ?? Warren sandra.cha Sandra 6:02.0 horntt+nut bu [NJ ?? 1 Gathercole 07.cha Brian 6j02.15 chippy chipped or chippable 1 MacWhinney boys74.cha Ross 6;03.23 paints+man a painter 4 MacWhinney boys91.cha Mark 6;04.15 top +skin either rhe skin on che rap of 2 rhe body or the ourer skin (epidermis) MacWhinney boys91.cha Mark 6;04.15 up +skin either the skin on rhe top of the body or the ourer skin MacWhinney boys75.cha Ross 6;04.23 knQck +knock a joke MacWhinney boys75.cha Ross 6;04.23 pu+room the lavatory Flercher 7:bcdav.cha David 6j11 bunnyhop to hop like a bunny Evans dyadl4.cha Erin 6; 11.17 split+in +halfcar a car char splits in half Towards an explication ofthe role ofchild language in language change 1 1 Page 49 researcher file speaker age word gloss # Evans dyad04.cha Jessica 7;01.6 °brown+hair mtln unknown person 1 Evam dyad 14.cha Aimee 7;02.25 sp/it+car a car that splits into pieces (and presumably goes back together afterwards) 2 MacWhinney boys84.cha Ross 7;03.18 hous~+/ook ?? 1 MacWhinney boys84.cha Ross 7;03.18 mis+thing [N] ?? 1 MacWhinney boys91.cha Ross 8;05.21 sky +wiS( ?? Bohannon debbie.cha Nat h~l/+copter a helicopter Bohannon noel.cha Nat knit+making knitting Snow nath12.cha Nathan ?? alpha+butl~ [Nj ?? MacWhinney boys72in.cha Mark ?? big+Mouth [proper name] Bohannon thad5.cha Baxter ?? cnr+hra [Nj ?? Wells laura06.cha Sarah ?? ch~w + gum chewing-gum 2 Wells darren21.cha Robert ?? cow+lndian a native american (from cowhoy-indian) 1 Wells darren21.cha Robert ?? Cowboy+lndian a native american Wells benjarn04.cha Nicola ?? gran+gran a grandparent ?? MacWhinney boys69.cha Ross ?? Mr Brian +Snowman [proper name] Wells abigai09.cha Rebecca ?? MrS+Wu+Wu [proper name] Wells darren06.cha Nicola ?? br +lop ?? MacWhinney boys72in.cha Mark ?? Jea+hone a bone that is found in the 1 sea (chewed on by sea-dogs) MacWhinney boys72in.cha Mark ?? sea+dog a dog who Iives in the sea Gleason dinner. victor.cha Victor ?? tichtock a clock ?? Gleason dinner. victor.cha Victor ?? tick+tock-" a clock ?? Wells benjarn02.cha Nicola n umherbrella an umbrella 14 Wells abigai07.cha Louise ?? scally+wag a scalawag 1 Towards an fXplication oftht rolt ofchild languagt in language change 1 1 1 2 Page 50 researcher file speaker age word gloss BOOannon angcla.cha Nat a light+color Iight-coiorcd BOOannon angela.cha Nat a zump ?? bAbewa _seand m()QU # more or less interchangeably. a Nat was 2;8 when he interacted with the undergraduates and 3;0 when he interacted with the graduate students. Unfortunately, the documentation doesn't identify the status of Angela. Debbie, or Noel. From hard copy transcriptions Since the data in the transcriptions from Parkhurst & Gottman 1986 are not on computer (and are therefore less readily searchable), I am including line numbers for the initial occurrance of each word. Line numbers of subsequent occurrances have been omitted to save space. researcher tape line speaker age word gloss # Parkhurst 19 439 Andreas 2;09 bandit b~an a bandit who steals beans ?? 1 Parkhurst 19 257 Andreas 2;09 m~ss to make a mess ?? 1 Parkhurst 19 220 Mara 2;11 Angi~ Purp/~ [proper name] 2 Parkhurst 19 444 Mara 2;11 DrellJ Andreas (proper name) 1 Parkhurst 19 174 Mara 2;11 kay okay 3 Parkhurst 19 236 Mara 2;11 pap" [N] ?? 8 Parkhurst 19 438 Mara 2;11 shar~ ... in to share equally between two people 1 Parkhurst 15,1 396 Claire 3;06 bangta knife a knife that you can shoot with 1 Parkhurst 0,1 12 Naomi 4,9 jumping board a board for people to jump on so they can see you swim Parkhurst 15,1 303 Meighan 4;0 big girl kittm a kitten old enough to take care of itself Parkhurst 15,1 360 Meighan 4;0 gllJ station lady the lady at the gas station Parkhurst 15,1 195 Meighan 4;0 hairdms ?? 3 Parkhurst 15,1 305 Meighan 4;0 kittycat balloon [N] ?? 1 Parkhurst 16 161 Meighan 4;0 starvy thin 3 Parkhurst 4 154 Kimberly 4;0 stop-cold [N] ?? 2 Parkhurst A 18 Naomi 4;07 cord to record 2 Towards an ~xplication ofth~ role ofchild language in languag~ m~an half change 1 Pag~ 51 Parkhurst E 36 Naomi 4:09 dtuldy ekntist either a dentist who is a daddy or one who works on daddies Parkhurst E 246 Naomi 4:09 day plac~ a place you go in the day Parkhurst E 377 Naomi 4:09 diap" blanlut a cloth diaper used as a blanket ?? Parkhurst E 6 Naomi 4:09 grandsist" kinship term. meaning unclear 2 Parkhurst B 926 Naomi 4:09 nuru-doctor a doctor who is also a nurse (a nurse practitioner ??) 1 Parkhurst E 38 Naomi 4:09 wift dmtist a dentist who is a wife Parkhurst 575 Naomi 4:09.16 baby sid~ the side babies sit on Parkhurst J J 84 Naomi 4:09.16 blindfold gam~ a game where someone is blindfolded and given things to feel Parkhurst J 575 Naomi 4:09.16 pmon Parkhurst F 246 Naomi 4:10 Parkhurst A 126 Eric Parkhurst F 522 Parkhurst 4 Parkhurst sid~ 1 the side non-babies sit on 1 chalk to mark with chalk 2 4:10 runny-in-oh plact [N] ?? 1 Naomi 4:11 waUr [0 121 Angela 5:0 bumpy bouncy ?? 5 H 41 Naomi S:O click~r a thing that clicks 4 Parkhurst E 161 Eric 5;0 cold chocolat~ chocolate milk Parkhurst J 482 Elise · 5;0 diap" pillow [N] ?? 1 Parkhurst B 307 Eric 5:0 dynamiu balloons balloons that explode 1 Parkhurst D.1 242 Eric 5;0 Eric robot a robot who is Eric 2 Parkhurst D.l 166 Eric 5;0 groc", stor~ Parkhurst DJ 1 154 Eric 5;0 jail groc", stor~ a grocery store in jail 1 Parkhurst D.l 194 Eric 5;0 Iock~r machin~ a machine that locks things 2 hous~ get something wet 1 [Nj ?? up Parkhurst E 245 Eric 5;0 night place a place you go at night 2 Parkhurst E 81 Eric 5:0 puff a down comforter 2 Parkhurst D.l 222 Eric 5;0 robot moth" a mother who is a robot 2 Parkhurst B 748 Eric 5;0 smtmus book a book with sentences in it 1 ?? Parkhurst Parkhurst J J 755 Elise 5;0 sn~aks 282 Elise 5;0 start~d tennis shoes (from sneakers) out tomato Towards an (Xplication ofthe role ofchild language in language change 1 an unripe tomato Page 52 Parkhurst 8 60 Banu, Julia 5:0,4;10 kangarule a rule about having kangaroos 5 Parkhurst L 505 Greta 5;01 bang to shoot 1 Parkhurst L 442 Naomi 5;01 button gun a gun with buttons on it 1 Parkhurst F 24 Eric 5;01 marry-place a place where you get married 2 Parkhurst L 406 Naomi 5;01 quipment equipment 4 Parkhurst L 458 Naomi 5;01 traption a contraption 10 733 Naomi 5:02 hankychief a handkerchief 2 Parkhurst Parkhurst H 408 Eric 5;03 grown uppy grown up [A] Parkhurst 9 383 Yael 6;10 halts-top halter-top Parkhurst D 160 Eric ?? garbage to make into garbage ?? 1 Parkhurst 22 222 Behnam ?? hole poker a thing which pokes holes 6 Parkhurst M 94 Eric ?? farm alarm From other bibliographic sources Random attested coinages (Clark 1982, 390-1): child word adult gloss tell-wind weathervane lessoner teacher shorthander someone who writes in shorthand winder ice-cream maker driver ignition k~y (of a car) toothachey having a toothache windy blown by the wind bumpy making bumping motions or noises flyable capable of flight salter saltier sliverest most slivery lawning mowing the lawn fire-dog dog found at the site of a fire apple-juice-chair chair by the glass of apple juice Denominal verbs English Instrumem verbs (Clark 1982, 402): child age word adult gloss Towards an explication ofth~ rol~ ofchild languag~ in languag~ chang~ Pag~53 S 2;04 scale to weigh S 2;04 button to S 2;07 broom to hit with a broom S 2;11 broom to sweep S 2;11 nipple to breastfeed S 3;0 key [0 S 3:02 gun to shoot with a gun S 3;02 needle to sew or mend (with a needle) S 3:02 string (up) to tie (a hat) on with a string EB 3;10 pliers (out) co remove with pliers EP 4;0 hatchet co chop with a hatchet CB 4;0 paper to give a paper cut to CB 4;04 shoelace co tie with a shoelace CB 4;06 seat-belt to secure with a seat-belt I-ll. 5:0 axe to chop with an axe jW 5;07 stick to hit with a stick turn on (with a button) open a door (with a key) S is probably Shem from the ChiLDES data. French Instrument verbs(Clark 1982, 403): child age word adult English gloss LA LA VA 2;0 deconstruire to unbuild 2;0 bulldozer to bulldoze(r) 3:09 boutonner to button/close, fasten (said of an envelope) VA VA 3;10 CG 4;07 4;1 XI) YD ) anixonner to Nixon cralver to chalk/draw with chalk rater to rakeN/ use a rake pincer to paintbrushN.luse a.paintbrush meccer co meterN.lmeasure German Instrument verbs (Clark 1982, 404): child age word adult English gloss RN 1;11 leitern to ladder/climb by means of a ladder RN 1: 11 schnuren co cord/tie a cord RN 2;1 fomadeln to wheelN (away) RN 2;1 peitschen cowhipN' Towards an explication ofthe role ofchild language in language change Page 54 RN 2;02 anbroschen co brooch/fascen wich a brooch (in chis case a buckle) RN 2;02 zangen co cong/pick up wich congs RN 2;09 scockeln to scickN/hic wich a stick Sc 3;06 angeschnauzeln to snout/nuzzle HS 3;08 vergilrteln to girdle/fasten about the waisc with tics HS 3;09 metern to mcterN' HF 3;09 ma'ichinen [0 GS 3;11 spliccern to splinccrN/hurt wich splinters HS 4;02 besen to broom AP ? messen to machineN/sew wich :! sewing machine knifeN' English Locatum verbs (Clark 1982, 406): child age word adult gloss DH 2;03 trouser to put trousers on X J 2;06 pillow to chrow a pillow at EB 3;04 dusc to become dusty EB 3;04 Band-Aid to put a Band-Aid on CB 3;11 cracker to put crackers in JA 4;0 cast to puc a cast on CB 4;02 water to water (done to non-plants) CB 4;05 bead to CB 4;05 rubber-band to puc a rubber band in ME 4:11 decoration co puc decoracions on SA 5:0 chocolate to put chocolace in JW 5:07 shirt to put a shirc on puc a bead in French Locatum verbs (Clark 1982, 406): child age word adult English gloss VA 4;05 pantoufler to slipper X EG 6:08 enoeuffer to egg/put egg on CG 7:0 enconfiturer to jamN/put jam on CG 7;0 ensiroter to syrup CG 7;03 en carter to put cartN on/cover with tarcN' CG 13;0 chocolater to give (hoc) chocolate co Towards an (xp/ication ofth( ro/( ofchild languag( in languag( chang( Pag( 55 CG 16 briquer to brick/puc bricks on German Locatum verbs (Clark 1982. 407): child age word adult English gloss RN 2;02 reinspiezen to in-poinclpuc che point in HS 3;0 emilchen to milkN/drink milk Sc 3;02 handtucheln to hand-towel HS 3;06 besuppt to have soup on. HS 3;06 einbllittern to in-leaf/put leaves in. FS 3;09 vollaschen to ash well/ cover in ashes GS 3;11 aufperlen to string beads HS 4;07 zubanden to ribbonltie a ribbon DL 5;06 anstreifen to stripe English Location. goal. agenc verbs (Clark 1982. 408): child age word adult gloss CA 4;06 funnd to force into a funnel SO 5;0 basket to put in a basket CB 5:05 thread to put on ~ thread K 7;0 towel to wrap in a towel S 3;01 cement [0 CB 5;06 dogear to put one's hair in 'dogears' 83 2;08 monster to behave as a monster to AK 5;01 ballerine to dance like a ballerina (backformation from bal/mna via rebracketing as bal/mnrn) HO 5;0 governess to be the governess of make cement German Location. goal. agent verbs (Clark 1982, 409): child age RN 2;07.15 Sc word adult English gloss bewassern to be watered/in the water 3;11 vererden to earth/bury RN 2;10 ausgeplliezeln to cake (lit. to --->/make into cakes RF 3;06.15 be7.almen to tooth/show one's teeth FS 3;11 musiken to music HS 3;11 zuschliefen to loop HS 5;01.15 lichten to light/shine a light Towards an aplication ofthe role ofchild language in language change Page 56 GS 4:04 dieben to thief Characteristic activity verbs in English (Clark 1982.410) child age word adult gloss fB 2;03 buzzer to buzz! make a sound like a buzzer RG 3:0 bell to ring like a bell . S 3;0 truck (of a truck) to drive by S 3;02 flag to flap (suddenly) in the wind S 3;02 wind (of wind) to blow CB 3;11 snowflake to snow CB 4:0 storm to storm (for me this is recreation of an existing lexeme) CB 4:04 wind (of wind) to blow S 2:04 hair to brush hair S 2;04 soup to eat soup J 2;06 rocker to rock in a rocker S 2:08.15 rug to vacuum a rug S 2;09 lawn to mow a lawn fB 3:02 match to light D 5:0 tea to have tea CB 6;0 IU to crack a nut H 6;0 deck to cut a deck (of cards) Characteristic activity words in French (Clark 1982. 411) child age word adult gloss VA 3;06 miauner to meow CG 6:03.17 grelonner to hail CG 4:08.15 pipe to EG 11;0 argileronner to clay CG 13;0 microscoper to microscope/use a microscope WD ?? pianer to piano/play the piano pipe/smoke a pipe Characteristic activity verbs in German (Clark 1982. 412) child age word adult gloss RN 1;10 miezeln to cat/meow RN 1: 10 hummeln to hum To 2:03 w:wen to woof Towards an lXplication ofthf rolf ofchild languagf in languagf change Page 57 RN RN 2:04.15 tunndeln to tunnel 2:04.15 gefli.igeln to wing/flap SS 2:06 glocken to RF RF 2:06.15 raupen to caterpillar 2:06.15 spiczen [0 GS 3;02 gemilhlen [0 mill/grind Sc 4;0 winden [0 wind/blow Sc 5;07 runterwellen to wave-down HS 6;06 wimpern to (eye)lash RN RN 1:10 bildern to picturelturn pages in a picture-book 2;08 nasdn to nose/wipe a nose GS 2:09 ewieren to piano HS 3:08 kawieren to piano belliring shrew Towards an txplication ofth~ role ofchild language in language change Page 58 Bibliography Aronoff. Mark. A. 1974. Word formation in gen(rariv( grammar. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Bauer, Laurie. 1983. Eng/ish Word-formation. Cambridge:Cambridge UP. Bellinger, David C. and Jean Berko Gleason. 1982. Sex differences in parental directives to young children. Sex Roks8. 1123-39. Bennett-Kastor, Tina. 1988. Analyzing children's /anguag(: m(thods and th(ori(s. Oxford: Basil Blackwell. Bloom, Lois. 1978. R(adings in /anguag( dev(/opm(nt. New York: Wiley. Bohannon, John Neil, and A. L. Marquis. 1977. Children's control of adult speech. Child D(V(/opment48. 1002-8 Bowerman, Melissa. 1982. Reorganizational processes in lexical and syntactic development. In Wanner & Gleitman 1982a. Brown, Roger. 1973. A first languag(: th( early stages. Cambridge. MA: Harvard UP. Bybee, Joan. 1985. Morphology: A study of th( r(/arion bmvun m(aning and form. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. - - - , and Dan Siobin. 1982. Rules and schemas in the development and use of the English past. Languag( 58. 265-89. Cannon, Garland. Blends in English word formation. Linguistics 24-4. 1986. pp 725-753. Carroll. David W. 1986. Psychology of languag(. Pacific Grove: Brooks. Clark, Eve V. 1978a. Discovering what words can do. In Papers from th( parasession on the lexicon. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society. - - - . 1978b. Strategies for communicating. Child Droelopment49. 953-9. - - - . 1979. When nouns surface as verbs. Language 55. 767-811. - - - . 1981. Lexical innovations: How children learn to create new words. In Deutsch (ed.) 1981. - - - . 1982. The young word maker: a case study of innovation in the child's lexicon. In Wanner & Gleitman (eds.) 1982a. - - - . 1986. Coining complex compounds in English: affixes and word order in acquisition. Linguistics 24-1. pp 7-29. - - - u alii. 1988. A thrower-button or a button-thrower? Children's judgments of grammatical and ungrammatical compound nouns. Linguistics 26-1. pp 3-19. de Villiers. Jill and Peter. 1973. A cross-sectional study of the acquisition of grammatical morphemes in child speech. Journal of Psycho linguistic Research 2. 267-78. Reprinted in Bloom (ed.) 1978. Towards an explication ofthe role ofchild language in language change Page 59 Derwing, Bruce L. and William J. Baker. 1979. Recent research on the acquisition of English morphology. in Paul Fletcher and Michael Garman (eds.) 1979. Languagt acquisition. 209-24. Cambridge: Cambridge UP. Deutsch, Werner. (ed.) 1981. Tht childs construction oflanguagt. London: Academic Press. Garvey, Catherine, and R Hogan. 1973. Social speech and social interaction: egocentrism revisited. Child Dnlt/opment44. 562-8. Gleason. Jean Berko. 1958. The child's learning of English morphology. Word 14. 150-77. reprinted in Bloom (ed.) 1978. Hock. Hans Heinrich. 1986. Principks ofhistorical linguistics. Berlin: Mouton. Hoenigswald, Henry M. 1960. Languagt changt and linguistic reconstruction. Chicago: Universiry of Chicago Press. Kastovsky. Dieter. 1986. The problem of productiviry in word formation. Linguistics 24-1. 585-600. King, Robert D. Historical linguistics and gentrative grammar. Englewood Cliffs. NJ: Prentice-Hall. Kuczaj, Stan. 1976. Arguments against Hurford's 'Aux copying rule.' Journal of Child Language 3. 423-7. - - - , . 1977. The acquisition of regular and irregular past tense forms. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour 16. 589-600. McNeill, David. 1970. The acquisition of language: The study ofdevelopmental psycholinguistics. New York: Harper MacWhinney, Brian. 1978. Processing a first language: the acquisition of morphophollology. Monographs of the Socitty for Research in Child Development 43. no. 174. MacWhinney, Brian, and Catherine Snow. 1985. The Child Language Data Exchange System. Journal of Child Language 12. 271-96. ---.1990. The Child Language Data Exchange System: an update. Journal ofChild Languagt 17. 457-72. Masur, Elise Frank, & Jean Berko Gleason. 1980. Parent-child interaaion and the acquisition of lexical information during play. Developmental Psychology 16-5. 404-9. Napoli, Donna Jo. and Judy Anne Kegl (eds.) 1991. Bridges between psychology and linguistics: A Swarthmore flst5chrift for Lila Gkitman. Hillsdale. NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum. Parkhurst. Jennifer T, and John Mordechai Gottman. 1986. How young children get what they want. in John M. Gottman and Jeffrey G. Parker. 1986. The conversations offriends: speculations on affictive development. Cambridge: Cambridge UP. data used by permission of Dr. Parkhurst. Pinker, Steven. 1984. Language karnability and language droelopment. Cambridge, MA: Harvard UP. Roeper. Thomas. 1981. On the deduaive model and the acquisition of productive morphology. In Baker & McCarthy (eds.) 1981. Towards an explication ofthe role ofchild language in language change Pagt60 Sachs, Jacqueline S. 1983. Talking about the there and then: the emergence of displaced reference in parent-child discourse. in Keith Nelson (ed.) 1983. Child Language 4. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum. Shipley, Elizabeth F. 1991. A farewell to 'thee.' in Napoli & Kegl 1991. Stine, Elizabeth Lon, and John Neil Bohannon. 1983. Imitations, interactions, and language acquisition. Journal of Child Language 10. 589-603. Wanner, Eric, and Lila Gleitman (eds.) 1982a. Cambridge UP. Languag~ acquisition: th~ stau of the art. Cambridge: - - - . 1982b. Language acquisition: the state of the state of the art. in Wanner & Gleitman 1982a. Warren-Leubecker, Amye. 1982. Sex diffirences in child-dirteted speech. Unpublished masters' thesis, Georgia Institute of Technology. - - - , and John Neil Bohannon. 1984. Intonation patterns in child-directed speech: mother-father differences. Child Development 55. 1379-85. Wells, Gordon. 1981. Learning through interaction: the study of language tkvelopment. Cambridge: Cambridge UP - - - . 1986. The meaning makers: children learning language and using language to learn. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann Extras for CHILDES Fletcher and Garman (1988) Gathercole (1980). Johnson (1986). And thanks to Mary Evans, Brian MacWhinney. and Catherine Snow for their data This thesis has been made possible by Brian MacWhinney and Catherine Snow, who set up the ChiLDES.database from which I have gotten most of my data, by my advisor. Donna Jo Napoli. who taught me syntax and morphology, by my apartmentmate, Naomi Parkhurst, who discussed the word creation sections with me, and who put up with my stressing out during the two final weeks of writing, and by the letter Q, and the number e. Towards an explication ofthe role ofchild language in language change Page 61