...

in which those changes were induced. Adaptive

by user

on
Category: Documents
32

views

Report

Comments

Transcript

in which those changes were induced. Adaptive
66
Opinion
TRENDS in Ecology & Evolution Vol.17 No.2 February 2002
Testing the beneficial
acclimation
hypothesis
Robbie S. Wilson and Craig E. Franklin
Recent developments in evolutionary physiology have seen many of the
long-held assumptions within comparative physiology receive rigorous
experimental analysis. Studies of the adaptive significance of physiological
acclimation exemplify this new evolutionary approach. The beneficial
acclimation hypothesis (BAH) was proposed to describe the assumption that
all acclimation changes enhance the physiological performance or fitness of
an individual organism. To the surprise of most physiologists, all empirical
examinations of the BAH have rejected its generality. However, we suggest
that these examinations are neither direct nor complete tests of the functional
benefit of acclimation. We consider them to be elegant analyses of the adaptive
significance of developmental plasticity, a type of phenotypic plasticity that
is very different from the traditional concept of acclimation that is used by
comparative physiologists.
To bring t r adition al compa r a tive physiology in to
line wit h con tempor a ry evolu tion a ry biology,
physiologists over t he past decade or so h ave been
using a more t heoretical a nd hypot hesis-driven
approach to evolu tion a ry questions in physiological
resea rch. H istorically, m a ny st udies in compa r a tive
physiology proposed post-hoc adaptive stories to
explain t he fu nction al significa nce of a physiological
t r ait after elucida ting its mech a nistic basis.
H owever, m a ny critics of t he adapt a tionist
progr a m me h ave highligh ted t h a t t here a re m a ny
alter n a tives to adaptive scen a rios [1,2], including
genetic drift, past selection, genetic cor rela tions a nd
historical a t t ribu tes [3]. T he st rengt h a nd success
of t his new evolu tion a ry approach to compa r a tive
physiology is reflected by t he diversity of st udies
t h a t a re producing a deeper u nderst a nding of t he
evolu tion of physiological systems (e.g. Refs [4–6],
reviewed in Ref. [7]).
The beneficial acclimation hypothesis
Robbie S. Wilson*
Dept of Biology,
University of Antwerp,
Universiteitsplein 1,
B-2610 Wilrijk, Belgium.
*e-mail:
rwilson @ uia.ua.ac.be
Craig E. Franklin
Physiological Ecology
Laboratory, Dept of
Zoology, The University
of Queensland, St Lucia,
QLD 4072, Australia.
O ne of the best examples of this new approach to
physiological research has been the experimental
analysis of the adaptive significance of physiological
A C C L I M A T I O N [8–12] (see G lossary). Traditionally,
acclimation has been defined as the adjustment of
physiological traits in response to changes in a single
environmental variable in the lab [13], whereas
A C C L I M A T IZ A T I O N refers to physiological responses to
environmental variables in the field [13]. Physiologists
often assumed that all acclimation changes to the
phenotype enhanced the physiological performance or
fitness of an individual organism in the environment
http://tree.trends.com
in which those changes were induced. A daptive
arguments were often formulated after identifying
the functional role of the phenotypic modification and
usually involved logical arguments that showed how
the phenotype enhanced reproductive success, growth
or survival. T his long-held assumption, now referred
to as the beneficial acclimation hypothesis (B A H) [8],
has recently received a significant amount of
experimental interrogation.
T he B A H h as been tested predomin a ntly by
exa mining the acclim a tory responses of ectotherms to
temper a ture. T he hypothesis predicts th a t a nim als
acclim a ted to a pa rticula r temper a ture h ave
enh a nced perform a nce or fitness a t th a t temper a ture
in compa rison with a nim als acclim a ted to other
temper a tures. However, to the surprise of m a ny
compa r a tive physiologists, all empirical
exa min a tions of the B A H so fa r h ave rejected its
gener ality [8,10–12]. T hese studies h ave
demonstr a ted th a t the phenotypic ch a nges
(P H E N O T Y P I C P L A S T I C I T Y ) th a t occur in orga nisms during
development in different therm al environments do
not alw ays lead to a n increased fitness in th a t
environment when compa red with the fitness of
orga nisms r aised a t other temper a tures.
I n the first test of the B A H , L eroi et a l. [8] exposed
genetically identical lines of the bacteria Escherichi a
coli to either 32°C or 41.5°C for 24 h (~6.7 cell
gener a tions d−1 a t 37°C) a nd then competed the two
groups a t both exposure temper a tures ( F ig. 1). T he
B A H w as used to predict th a t the 32°C group would
outcompete the 41.5°C group a t 32°C, a nd vice versa
a t 41.5°C. However, bacteria grown up a t 32°C
outcompeted the 41.5°C-group a t both temper a tures,
a nd so the B A H w as rejected ( F ig. 1). I n a more
extensive test of the B A H , Bennett a nd L ensk i [9]
r aised E . coli a t 22, 27, 32, 37 or 40°C a nd then
competed the different acclim a tion groups against
each other a t each temper a ture. As in the previous
study, m a ny groups were outcompeted a t their
‘acclim a tion’ temper a ture by bacteria r aised a t other
temper a tures (benefit for acclim a tion w as found in
only seven out of the 12 compa risons). Again, these
results were used to reject the gener ality of the B A H .
G ibert a nd co-wor kers [14] recently outlined
a nother experiment al test of the B A H . T hey r aised
D rosophil a mel a nogaster from two different
popula tions a t 18, 25 or 29°C a nd then tested the
w al k ing speed of each development al group a t each
temper a ture. T he B A H w as used to predict th a t flies
would w al k faster a t their actu al development al
temper a ture th a n would flies developed a t other
temper a tures. However, in contr ast with their
predictions, flies rea red a t 25°C w al ked faster a t all
other temper a tures th a n did those r aised a t 18 or
29°C, a nd the B A H w as again rejected.
Acclimation or developmental plasticity?
We suggest th a t the empirical studies discussed here
a re neither direct nor complete tests of the function al
0169-5347/02/$ – see front matter © 2002 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved. PII: S0169-5347(01)02384-9
Opinion
TRENDS in Ecology & Evolution Vol.17 No.2 February 2002
benefit of therm al acclim a tion, as defined from
tr adition al physiological studies of acclim a tion.
R a ther, we suggest these studies a re elega nt a n alyses
of the A D A P T I V E SI G N I F I C A N C E of D E V E L O P M E N T A L
P L A S T I C I T Y . Acclim a tion responses studied by
tr adition al compa r a tive physiologists differ
subst a ntially to the development al plasticity
exa mined by L eroi et a l. [8], Bennett a nd L ensk i [9]
a nd G ibert et a l. [14]. H istorically, compa r a tive
physiologists considered acclim a tion as a reversible
response by a n orga nism to ch a nges (often season al)
in a single environment al va riable [13]. B y contr ast,
development al plasticity deals with the entire suite
of phenotypic ch a nges th a t occur as a result of
differences in the development al environment, not
just the facult a tive physiological responses of a n
orga nism (acclim a tion).
Beca use of the highly sensitive n a ture of
orga nisms during the ea rly st ages of development,
sm all va ria tions in the development al environment
ca n lead to a cascade of phenotypic ch a nges [15–17].
Besides acclim a tion responses, development al
plasticity ca n a rise from the direct biophysical effects
of the environment, a nd ca n be detriment al, neutr al
or beneficial. E nvironment al factors th a t lead to these
un avoidable, a nd often nonreversible phenotypic
ch a nges include temper a ture [18], oxygen tension
[19–21], nutrition [22,23] a nd density of conspecifics
[24]. F or exa mple, M a tsch a k et a l. [19] found th a t
temper a ture-induced ch a nges in muscle cellula rity
during embryonic development of the A tla ntic salmon
S a lm a sa l a r were pa rtly due to restricted oxygen
availability a t higher temper a tures r a ther th a n to
facult a tive responses to temper a ture. T he egg capsule
of embryonic salmon ca n act as a n oxygen ba rrier,
pa rticula rly a t higher temper a tures when there is a n
increased oxygen dem a nd. I rreversible ch a nges in
the size a nd number of muscle fibres occur a t high
development al temper a ture as a direct consequence
of a constr aint in oxygen availability. T hese hightemper a ture-induced development al ch a nges in
muscle cellula rity a re clea rly not facult a tive
acclim a tion responses.
O bliga tory developmen t al ch a nges a re
pa r ticula rly prevalen t following exposu re to st ressful
conditions, bu t t heir effects a re often subtle.
H offm a n n a nd H ew a- K apuge [18] distinguished t he
rela tive con t ribu tions of differen t types of phenotypic
ch a nge following exposu re to high temper a t u res in
t he pa r asitic w asp Tr ichogr a m m a n r. br assicae .
I mpor t a n tly, t hey fou nd t h a t some bu t not all
phenotypic ch a nges du ring developmen t were
t he result of facult a tive acclim a tion responses.
H offm a n n a nd H ew a- K apuge [18] initially observed
t h a t adults of T. n r. br assicae exhibited a n increased
resist a nce to st ressful temper a t u res following
exposu re to 33°C as pupae, bu t t h a t t hese ch a nges
were accompa nied by deleterious fit ness effects.
T hey suggested t h a t t hese fit ness decreases eit her
reflected a gener al cost of increasing resist a nce to
http://tree.trends.com
67
Exposure of
bacteria for~7
generations
32oC
41.5oC
Compete both
groups against
each other
32oC-group
versus
41.5oC-group
41.5oC-group
versus
32oC-group
32oC-group
outcompeted
41.5oC-group
at 32oC
32oC-group
outcompeted
41.5oC-group
at 41.5oC
Results
TRENDS in Ecology & Evolution
Fig. 1. Experimental investigation of the Beneficial Acclimation
Hypothesis (BAH) by Leroi et al. [8]. The BAH was rejected in this case.
st ressful temper a t u res (acclim a tion response) or
were associa ted wit h direct phenotypic effects
a rising from da m age or developmen t al const r ain ts
ca used by t he high temper a t u res.
To test this idea, the a uthors exa mined whether
the increased resist a nce to high temper a tures in
T. nr. br assicae could occur without a ny of the
observed decreases in fitness [18]. Pupae of
T. nr. br assicae were exposed to 33°C for 2, 3 or 4 h d−1
for four days [18]. F or each trea tment group, there
w as a n increase in adult resist a nce to higher
temper a tures. However, fitness decreased only in
the groups exposed to 33°C for 3 or 4 h d−1 . T hus,
decreases in fitness following exposure to high
temper a tures were clea rly not ca used by the
increased resist a nce to stressful temper a ture
(acclim a tion) but r a ther to either gener al da m age to
the phenotype or development al constr aints imposed
by the high temper a tures. I ncreased resist a nce to
temper a ture without fitness costs h as also been
observed in the egg pa r asitoid T. ca rver ae in both
labor a tory a nd field experiments [25].
E xposing orga nisms to stressful conditions
confounds a ny a n alysis of the B A H (Box 1). Besides
acclim a tion responses possibly aimed a t minimizing
the stress of the environment, pa thological da m age
to the phenotype also occurs. I n spite of these
confounding effects, sever al a n alyses of the B A H
h ave incorpor a ted stressful conditions [8,9,26,27].
Met abolic costs a nd gener al phenotypic da m age
could overwhelm a ny positive acclim a tion responses
in a stressful environment a nd the B A H might be
incorrectly rejected. R a ther th a n compa risons a mong
orga nisms r aised under stressful conditions for
a n alyses of the B A H , Woods a nd H a rrison [27]
advoca te exa mining the costs a nd benefits of specific
acclim a tion responses.
N ot a ll developmen t a lly i nevi t able ch a nges to t he
phenot ype a re ca used by st ressfu l condi t ions. I n t he
specific case of temper a t u re, i t is doubtfu l t h a t ever y
ph ysiologica l process t h a t is affected by temper a t u re
68
Opinion
TRENDS in Ecology & Evolution Vol.17 No.2 February 2002
Box 1. Experimental analysis of the BAH under stressful conditions
Woods and Harrison [a] addressed whether the acclimation of
Manduca sexta caterpillars to water stress was beneficial.
• Treatments
Larvae were raised from the first instar on low- (69%) or high-water
(80%) artificial diets. After reaching the fifth instar, growth rates for
both groups were measured on the same or opposite diet for 36 h.
Several determinants of larval water budget were also recorded.
• Results
Caterpillars raised on high-water diet grew faster than did those
raised on low-water diet when tested on both diets. Thus, no
benefit for previous exposure to low-water diet was observed.
However, larvae responded to short-term hydric stress (low-water
diet) by minimizing water excretion by increasing rectal water
absorption, and to long-term hydric stress by significantly
reducing faecal water excretion.
• Conclusions
The authors concluded that, under current usage, the
beneficial acclimation hypothesis (BAH) had to be rejected in
this case. However, we suggest that phenotypic damage owing
to hydric stress confounds any analysis of the benefits of
acclimation.
Do the lower growth rates of larvae raised on a low-water
diet (when tested on both diets) reflect pathological
phenotypic changes owing to stress OR a lack of beneficial
acclimation? We suggest that the pathological effects of chronic
stress might overwhelm any possible beneficial acclimation
responses.
Reference
a Woods, H . A. a nd H a rrison, J. F. (2001) T he beneficial acclim a tion hypothesis
versus acclim a tion of specific tr aits: physiological ch a nges in w a ter-stressed
M a nduca sexta ca terpilla rs. Physiol. Zool. 74, 32–44
ca n ‘accli m a te’ to differen t temper a t u res. T h is
w ill i nevi t ably lead to phenot ypic differences
bet ween orga n isms from differen t developmen t a l
temper a t u res t h a t a re si mply due to t he di rect
effects of temper a t u re on t hese developmen t a l
pa t h w a ys [17,19–21]. T hese phenot ypic ch a nges
a re cer t a i n ly not t hose t h a t were t r adi t ion a lly
descr ibed as accli m a t ion responses by compa r a t ive
ph ysiologists, bu t a re u ndoubtedly i ncl uded i n t he
st udies of L eroi et a l. [8] a nd B en net t a nd L ensk i [9].
T h us, previous a n a lyses of t he B A H usi ng
developmen t a l pl ast ici t y a re confou nded by
i ncl udi ng sever a l t ypes of phenot ypic pl ast ici t y.
A more compelli ng exper i men t a l a n a lysis of t he
benefi t of accli m a t ion wou ld be based a rou nd t he
concept of accli m a t ion t h a t t r adi t ion a l compa r a t ive
ph ysiologists were cr i t icized for assu m i ng w as
a l w a ys benefici a l.
Exploring the BAH using competing hypotheses
I n t wo addi t ion a l st udies explor i ng t he B A H , bot h
H uey a nd B er r iga n [11] a nd H uey et a l. [12]
advoca ted a st rong i nference approach to
ex a m i n i ng quest ions rel a t i ng to t he t her m a l
accli m a t ion of ectot her ms. T hei r approach i n volved
test i ng a mong compet i ng h ypot heses t h a t m a k e
differen t predict ions as to how developmen t a l
temper a t u re i nfl uences t he t her m a l sensi t ivi t y of
perfor m a nce (Box 2).
H uey a nd B er r iga n [11] a nd H uey et a l. [12]
t hen used t he da t asets of sever a l previous st udies,
such as t h a t by Z w a a n et a l. [28], to compa re t he
h ypot heses. Z w a a n et a l. [28] a n a lysed t he effect
of developmen t a l temper a t u re on adu l t longevi t y
i n D . mel a nogaster a nd fou nd t h a t flies r a ised a t
i n ter medi a te temper a t u res su r vived longer as
adu l ts t h a n did t hose flies r a ised a t cool or h igh
Box 2. Set of competing hypotheses
This set of competing hypotheses is as suggested by Huey and
Berrigan [a], and Huey et al. [b].
Beneficial Acclimation Hypothesis (BAH): organisms acclimated
to a particular environment have enhanced performance or
fitness in that environment relative to organisms acclimated to
other environments [c].
Optimal developmental Temperature Hypothesis (OTH):
organisms raised at intermediate temperatures have higher
relative fitness across all temperatures than do organisms raised
at high or low temperatures. The OTH was suggested as an
alternative to the BAH by Zamudio et al. [d], Huey and Berrigan [a]
and Huey et al. [b].
Cooler is Better Hypothesis (CBH): organisms raised at cool
temperatures have higher relative fitness across all
temperatures than do organisms raised at intermediate or
high temperatures. The CBH is based on the assumption
that the larger size of cool-developed organisms is
http://tree.trends.com
sufficiently advantageous to outweigh any benefits of
acclimation [b].
Warmer is Better Hypothesis (WBH): organisms raised at high
temperatures have higher relative fitness across all temperatures
than do those raised at intermediate or cool temperatures. The
WBH is the reciprocal of the CBH of Huey et al. [b].
References
a H uey, R. B. a nd Berriga n, D . A. (1996) Testing evolution a ry hypotheses of
acclim a tion. I n A nim a ls a nd Temper ature: Phenotypic a nd E volution a ry
A d aptation. Society for E xperimenta l B iology Semin a r Series (Johnston, I. A.
a nd Bennett, A. F., eds), pp. 205–237, C a mbridge U niversity Press
b H uey, R. B. et a l. (1999) Testing the adaptive significa nce of acclim a tion: a
strong inference approach. A m. Zool. 39, 323–336
c L eroi, A. M. et a l. (1994) Temper a ture acclim a tion a nd competitive fitness: a n
experiment al test of the beneficial acclim a tion assumption. Proc. N atl. Aca d.
Sci. U . S. A. 91, 1917–1921
d Z a mudio, K .R. et a l. (1995) B igger isn’t alw ays better: body size, temper a ture
a nd m ale territorial success in D rosophil a mel a nogaster . A nim. Beh av.
49, 671–677
Opinion
TRENDS in Ecology & Evolution Vol.17 No.2 February 2002
Conclusions and future directions
Glossary
Acclimation: any facultative modification in a physiological trait in response to changes in
an environmental variable in the lab. Changes can be in response to the developmental
environment or long-term environmental shifts during the later stages of the life history of an
organism (more traditionally studied). Responses can be beneficial, neutral or negative.
Acclimatization: facultative modifications in a physiological trait in response to changes in
one or more environmental variables in the field.
Adaptive significance: (in context of plasticity) fitness advantages and disadvantages associated
with the expression of phenotypic plasticity across a range of environments. Fitness
consequences can be positive, negative or neutral.
Developmental plasticity: phenotypic changes induced by variation in the developmental
environment. Changes can include facultative responses by the organism (e.g. acclimation)
or unavoidable biophysical consequences of the environment (obligatory and/or
pathological).
Phenotypic plasticity: environmentally induced variation in morphology, physiology and/or
behaviour of an organism.
Acknowledgements
We thank Ian A. Johnston,
Helga Guderley,
Andy Clarke, Craig Moritz
and Raoul Van Damme for
stimulating discussions
and/or reading the article.
This article was improved
substantially by
comments from several
anonymous referees. This
work was supported by a
small ARC grant awarded
to C.E.F. and R.S.W. and a
ARC Large Grant to C.E.F.
and Craig Moritz.
69
temper a t u res, rega rdless of t he temper a t u re a t
w h ich t he adu l t flies were k ept. T hese da t a were
used by H uey a nd B er r iga n [11] a nd H uey et a l. [12]
to aga i n reject t he gener a li t y of t he B A H i n fa vou r
of t he opt i m a l developmen t a l temper a t u re
h ypot hesis (O T H ).
However, as with the previous a n alyses of the
B A H , we suggest this experiment al design is also
confounded by sever al different types of plasticity
underlying the phenotypic ch a nges, not just
acclim a tion responses. We consider th a t, in this
context, the O T H , cooler is better hypothesis (C B H)
a nd w a rmer is better hypothesis (W B H) all deal
specifically with the adaptive consequences of the
development al environment, rega rdless of the source
of phenotypic ch a nges. However, the B A H refers only
to the facult a tive physiological responses of the
orga nisms a nd is thus only one specific type of
phenotypic plasticity. We suggest th a t the O T H , C B H
a nd W B H a re not mutu ally exclusive to the B A H .
F or exa mple, it is possible th a t the development al
constr aints imposed on the phenotype by some
temper a tures a re so grea t th a t the over all
perform a nce is domin a ted not by the acclim a tion
responses (if they occur), but by these phenotypic
inevit abilities. I n other words, there might be a n
optim al temper a ture for development th a t is
determined solely by the un avoidable ch a nges to
the phenotype th a t occur in the different therm al
environments. T his, of course, says nothing about
the rela tive merit of the ‘acclim a tion’ ch a nges in
each environment.
References
1 F eder, M. E . (1987) T he a n alysis of physiological
diversity: the prospects for pa ttern
document a tion a nd gener al questions in
ecological physiology. I n N ew D irections in
E cologica l Physiology ( F eder, M. E . et a l., eds),
pp. 38–75, C a mbridge U niversity Press
2 G arland, T., Jr and C arter, P.A. (1994) E volutionary
physiology. Annu. Rev. E col. Syst. 56, 579–621
3 Gould, S.J. a nd L ewontin, R. C. (1979) T he
spa ndrels of Sa n M a rco a nd the P a nglossia n
P a r adigm: a critique of the adapt a tionist
progr a mme. Proc. R. Soc. London B B iol. Sci.
205, 581–598
http://tree.trends.com
Previous empirical tests of the B A H h ave elega ntly
demonstr a ted the evolution a ry significa nce of
therm ally induced development al plasticity [8,9],
especially with the advent of a rigorous experiment al
design testing sever al competing hypotheses [11,12].
I mport a ntly, these studies of the B A H h ave forcefully
m ade the point th a t acclim a tion ch a nges ca nnot just
be assumed to be beneficial, but this is a hypothesis
th a t must be rigorously tested. However, we believe
th a t a det ailed empirical exa min a tion of the adaptive
significa nce of more tr adition al measures of
physiological acclim a tion is now required to test the
B A H . We ch allenge compa r a tive physiologists to
develop new inventive experiment al designs to
explore the benefits a nd costs of the more tr adition al
acclim a tion responses. However, this will not be
easy a nd using a nything less th a t a close correla te
of fitness to test the hypothesis, such as survival,
reproductive success or competitive ability, would be
less th a n desir able. Previous empirical tests of the
B A H h ave cert ainly set a benchm a r k for exa mining
the adaptive significa nce of phenotypic plasticity,
rega rdless of the source of phenotypic va ria tion.
O ne entertaining possibility for future tests of the
B A H would be to examine the thermal acclimation
of reproductive performance, especially in a system
where females discriminate between displaying
males. For example, in a species where females are
choosy about their mates, females could be given the
opportunity to discriminate between cool- and warmacclimated males at various temperatures. T he ability
to attract and procure a female might depend on
characters such as swimming performance, aerobic
capabilities and general activity, all of which have
been shown to acclimate to temperature in a variety of
taxa [13]. I n this specific case, the B A H would predict
that, at high temperatures, females would find the
warm-acclimated males more attractive than they
would the cool-acclimated males and vice versa at cool
temperatures. E ven more compelling would be the
inclusion of males that had been raised at different
temperatures, so the relative merits of developmental
plasticity could be compared with the more reversibletype acclimation responses. We suggest that future
tests of the B A H should investigate traditional types
of acclimation using the protocols developed for
analysing developmental plasticity.
4 K ingsolver, J. G . a nd H uey, R. B. (1998)
E volution a ry a n alyses of morphological a nd
physiological plasticity in therm ally va riable
environments. A m. Zool. 38, 545–560
5 Bennett, A. F. a nd L ensk i, R. E . (1999)
E xperiment al evolution a nd its role in
evolution a ry physiology. A m. Zool. 39, 346–362
6 B r adley, T.J. et a l. (1999) Physiological responses
to selection for desicca tion resist a nce in
D rosophil a mel a nogaster . A m. Zool. 39, 337–345
7 F eder, M. E . et a l. (2000) E volution a ry physiology.
A nnu. Rev. E col. Syst. 31, 315–341
8 L eroi, A. M. et a l. (1994) Temper a ture acclim a tion
a nd competitive fitness: a n experiment al test of
the beneficial acclim a tion assumption. Proc. N atl.
Aca d. Sci. U . S. A. 91, 1917–1921
9 Bennett, A. F. a nd L ensk i, R. E . (1997)
E volution a ry adapt a tion to temper a ture:
V I. Phenotypic acclim a tion a nd its evolution in
Escherichi a coli. E volution 51, 36–44
10 Hoffm a nn, A. A. (1995) Acclim a tion: increasing
survival a t a cost. Trends E col. E vol. 10, 1–2
11 H uey, R.B. and Berrigan, D.A. (1996) Testing
evolutionary hypotheses of acclimation. In A nimals
and Temperature: Phenotypic and E volutionary
A daptation. Society for E xperimental B iology
Seminar Series (Johnston, I.A. and Bennett, A. F.,
eds), pp. 205–237, C ambridge U niversity Press
70
Opinion
TRENDS in Ecology & Evolution Vol.17 No.2 February 2002
12 H uey, R. B. et a l. (1999) Testing the adaptive
significa nce of acclim a tion: a strong inference
approach. A m. Zool. 39, 323–336
13 Prosser, C. L . (1991) E nvironmenta l a nd Metabolic
A nim a l Physiology: Compa r ative A nim a l
Physiology , 4th edn, Wiley– L iss
14 G ibert, P. et a l. (2001) Locomotor perform a nce of
D rosophil a mel a nogaster : inter actions a mong
development al a nd adult temper a tures, age, a nd
geogr aphy. E volution 55, 205–209
15 U sher, M. L . et a l. (1994) M uscle development in
A tla ntic salmon ( S a lmo sa l a r ) embryos a nd the
effect of temper a ture on muscle cellula rity. J. F ish
B iol. 44, 953–964
16 Vieir a, V. L . A. a nd Johnston, I. A. (1992) I nfluence
of temper a ture on muscle-fibre development in
la rvae of the herring C lupea h a rengus. M a r. B iol.
112, 333–341
17 Johnston, I. A. et a l. (1998) E mbryonic
temper a ture modula tes muscle growth
ch a r acteristics in la rval a nd juvenile herring.
J. E xp. B iol. 201, 623–646
18 Hoffm a nn, A. A. a nd H ew a-K apuge, S. (2000)
Acclim a tion for hea t resist a nce in Trichogr a mm a
nr. br assicae: ca n it occur without costs? F unct.
E col. 14, 55–60
19 M atscha k, T.W. et al. (1995) Is physiological hypoxia
the driving force behind temperature effects on
muscle development in embryonic A tlantic salmon
(Salmo salar)? D ifferentiation 59, 71–77
20 M a tsch a k, T.W. et a l. (1998) Met abolic enzyme
activities in A tla ntic salmon ( S a lmo sa l a r L .)
embryos respond more to chronic ch a nges in
oxygen availability th a n to environment al
temper a ture. F ish Physiol. B iochem. 18, 115–123
21 M a tsch a k, T.W. et a l. (1998) Temper a ture a nd
oxygen tension influence the development of
muscle cellula rity in embryonic r ainbow trout.
J. F ish B iol. 53, 581–590
22 Shine, R. a nd Downes, S.J. (1999) C a n pregn a nt
liza rds adjust their offspring phenotypes to
environment al conditions? Oecologi a 119, 1–8
23 Rasmussen, K .M. and F ischbeck, K .L. (1987) E ffect
of repeated reproductive cycles on pregnancy
Putting predators
back into behavioral
predator–prey
interactions
Steven L. Lima
In the study of behavioral predator–prey interactions, predators have been
treated as abstract sources of risk to which prey respond, rather than
participants in a larger behavioral interaction. When predators are put back
into the picture by allowing them to respond strategically to prey behavior,
expectations about prey behavior can change. Something as simple as allowing
predators to move in response to prey movements might not only (radically)
alter standard expectations of prey behavior, but might also reveal new classes
of behavioral phenomena that occur at large spatial scales. Similar revelations
undoubtedly await many well-studied aspects of the behavioral interaction
between predator and prey. Most examples studied to date, both theoretical and
empirical, require attention from this ‘predatory’ perspective. Putting predators
back into the picture will be challenging, but doing so might change the way in
which biologists think about predator–prey interactions in general.
Steven L. Lima
Dept of Life Sciences,
Indiana State University,
Terre Haute, IN 47809,
USA.
e-mail:
S-Lima @ indstate.edu
O ver the past 20 years, the study of behavioral
interactions between predator and prey has shed much
light on prey behavior, and it is now clear that almost
any aspect of prey decision-ma king (from foraging
behavior to mate choice) can be influenced by the risk
of predation [1–3]. A growing literature also suggests
that nonlethal interactions between predator and prey
(those driven by prey avoidance of predation) might be
an important component of predator–prey interactions
http://tree.trends.com
24
25
26
27
28
outcome in ad libitum -fed and chronically foodrestricted rats. J. N utr. 117, 1959–1966
N ewm a n, R. A. (1998) E cological constr aints on
a mphibia n met a morphosis: inter actions of
temper a ture a nd la rval density with responses to
ch a nging food level. Oecologi a 115, 9–16
T homson, L .J. et a l. (2001) F ield a nd labor a tory
evidence for acclim a tion without costs in a n egg
pa r asitoid. F unct. E col. 15, 217–221
G ibbs, A. G . et a l. (1998) E ffects of cuticula r lipids
a nd w a ter bala nce in a desert D rosophil a : is
therm al acclim a tion beneficial? J. E xp. B iol.
201, 71–80
Woods, H . A. a nd H a rrison, J. F. (2001) T he
beneficial acclim a tion hypothesis versus
acclim a tion of specific tr aits: physiological
ch a nges in w a ter-stressed M a nduca sexta
ca terpilla rs. Physiol. Zool. 74, 32–44
Zw a a n, B. et a l. (1995) A rtificial selection for
development al time in D rosophil a mel a nogaster
in rela tion to the evolution of aging: direct a nd
correla ted responses. E volution 49, 635–648
in general [4–8]. Wor k on behavioral predator–prey
interactions therefore provides an important bridge
between the studies of behavior and ecology.
I n spite of t hese m a ny adva nces, ou r
u nderst a nding of beh avior al preda tor–prey
in ter actions is limited by a simple oversigh t: we
vir t u ally forgot abou t t he beh avior of preda tors!
H istorically, we h ave been so focused on prey
beh avior t h a t we (myself included) beca me
comfor t able wit h t rea ting preda tors as u n responsive
‘black boxes’ r a t her t h a n pa r ticipa n ts in a beh avior al
in ter action. T his oversigh t h as not only led to a n
incomplete view of beh avior al in ter actions bet ween
preda tors a nd prey, bu t h as also obscu red a n en tire
class of such in ter actions t h a t occu rs a t la rge spa tial
scales. M y goal is to explore some of t he insigh ts
gained from pu t ting preda tors back in to beh avior al
preda tor–prey in ter actions.
How were predators removed from the interaction?
T he removal of preda tors from the beh avior al
preda tor–prey inter action is appa rent in the
ubiquitous ‘fixed-risk’ assumptions of const a nt a tt ack
r a tes over time a nd pa tch-specific risks of preda tion
(e.g. Ref. [9]); such assumptions imply th a t preda tors
a re not influenced by prey beh avior. As few would
a rgue for the strict validity of this assumption,
why were preda tors relega ted to the st a tus of
unresponsive entities? I n m a ny w ays, the fixed-risk
approach (i.e. the assumption of unresponsive
preda tors) w as a sensible st a rting point.
C h a r acterizing preda tion risk as a n environment al
const a nt seemed reason able given th a t preda tors ca n
stri ke opportunistically a nd could be a nywhere a t a
given time. M a them a tical convenience might h ave
also played a role: models of a ntipreda tor decisionm a k ing a re much simpler under a n assumption of
fixed risk th a n they a re when both preda tor a nd prey
a re allowed to respond to one a nother. F urthermore,
0169-5347/02/$ – see front matter © 2002 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved. PII: S0169-5347(01)02393-X
Fly UP