Comments
Transcript
in which those changes were induced. Adaptive
66 Opinion TRENDS in Ecology & Evolution Vol.17 No.2 February 2002 Testing the beneficial acclimation hypothesis Robbie S. Wilson and Craig E. Franklin Recent developments in evolutionary physiology have seen many of the long-held assumptions within comparative physiology receive rigorous experimental analysis. Studies of the adaptive significance of physiological acclimation exemplify this new evolutionary approach. The beneficial acclimation hypothesis (BAH) was proposed to describe the assumption that all acclimation changes enhance the physiological performance or fitness of an individual organism. To the surprise of most physiologists, all empirical examinations of the BAH have rejected its generality. However, we suggest that these examinations are neither direct nor complete tests of the functional benefit of acclimation. We consider them to be elegant analyses of the adaptive significance of developmental plasticity, a type of phenotypic plasticity that is very different from the traditional concept of acclimation that is used by comparative physiologists. To bring t r adition al compa r a tive physiology in to line wit h con tempor a ry evolu tion a ry biology, physiologists over t he past decade or so h ave been using a more t heoretical a nd hypot hesis-driven approach to evolu tion a ry questions in physiological resea rch. H istorically, m a ny st udies in compa r a tive physiology proposed post-hoc adaptive stories to explain t he fu nction al significa nce of a physiological t r ait after elucida ting its mech a nistic basis. H owever, m a ny critics of t he adapt a tionist progr a m me h ave highligh ted t h a t t here a re m a ny alter n a tives to adaptive scen a rios [1,2], including genetic drift, past selection, genetic cor rela tions a nd historical a t t ribu tes [3]. T he st rengt h a nd success of t his new evolu tion a ry approach to compa r a tive physiology is reflected by t he diversity of st udies t h a t a re producing a deeper u nderst a nding of t he evolu tion of physiological systems (e.g. Refs [4–6], reviewed in Ref. [7]). The beneficial acclimation hypothesis Robbie S. Wilson* Dept of Biology, University of Antwerp, Universiteitsplein 1, B-2610 Wilrijk, Belgium. *e-mail: rwilson @ uia.ua.ac.be Craig E. Franklin Physiological Ecology Laboratory, Dept of Zoology, The University of Queensland, St Lucia, QLD 4072, Australia. O ne of the best examples of this new approach to physiological research has been the experimental analysis of the adaptive significance of physiological A C C L I M A T I O N [8–12] (see G lossary). Traditionally, acclimation has been defined as the adjustment of physiological traits in response to changes in a single environmental variable in the lab [13], whereas A C C L I M A T IZ A T I O N refers to physiological responses to environmental variables in the field [13]. Physiologists often assumed that all acclimation changes to the phenotype enhanced the physiological performance or fitness of an individual organism in the environment http://tree.trends.com in which those changes were induced. A daptive arguments were often formulated after identifying the functional role of the phenotypic modification and usually involved logical arguments that showed how the phenotype enhanced reproductive success, growth or survival. T his long-held assumption, now referred to as the beneficial acclimation hypothesis (B A H) [8], has recently received a significant amount of experimental interrogation. T he B A H h as been tested predomin a ntly by exa mining the acclim a tory responses of ectotherms to temper a ture. T he hypothesis predicts th a t a nim als acclim a ted to a pa rticula r temper a ture h ave enh a nced perform a nce or fitness a t th a t temper a ture in compa rison with a nim als acclim a ted to other temper a tures. However, to the surprise of m a ny compa r a tive physiologists, all empirical exa min a tions of the B A H so fa r h ave rejected its gener ality [8,10–12]. T hese studies h ave demonstr a ted th a t the phenotypic ch a nges (P H E N O T Y P I C P L A S T I C I T Y ) th a t occur in orga nisms during development in different therm al environments do not alw ays lead to a n increased fitness in th a t environment when compa red with the fitness of orga nisms r aised a t other temper a tures. I n the first test of the B A H , L eroi et a l. [8] exposed genetically identical lines of the bacteria Escherichi a coli to either 32°C or 41.5°C for 24 h (~6.7 cell gener a tions d−1 a t 37°C) a nd then competed the two groups a t both exposure temper a tures ( F ig. 1). T he B A H w as used to predict th a t the 32°C group would outcompete the 41.5°C group a t 32°C, a nd vice versa a t 41.5°C. However, bacteria grown up a t 32°C outcompeted the 41.5°C-group a t both temper a tures, a nd so the B A H w as rejected ( F ig. 1). I n a more extensive test of the B A H , Bennett a nd L ensk i [9] r aised E . coli a t 22, 27, 32, 37 or 40°C a nd then competed the different acclim a tion groups against each other a t each temper a ture. As in the previous study, m a ny groups were outcompeted a t their ‘acclim a tion’ temper a ture by bacteria r aised a t other temper a tures (benefit for acclim a tion w as found in only seven out of the 12 compa risons). Again, these results were used to reject the gener ality of the B A H . G ibert a nd co-wor kers [14] recently outlined a nother experiment al test of the B A H . T hey r aised D rosophil a mel a nogaster from two different popula tions a t 18, 25 or 29°C a nd then tested the w al k ing speed of each development al group a t each temper a ture. T he B A H w as used to predict th a t flies would w al k faster a t their actu al development al temper a ture th a n would flies developed a t other temper a tures. However, in contr ast with their predictions, flies rea red a t 25°C w al ked faster a t all other temper a tures th a n did those r aised a t 18 or 29°C, a nd the B A H w as again rejected. Acclimation or developmental plasticity? We suggest th a t the empirical studies discussed here a re neither direct nor complete tests of the function al 0169-5347/02/$ – see front matter © 2002 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved. PII: S0169-5347(01)02384-9 Opinion TRENDS in Ecology & Evolution Vol.17 No.2 February 2002 benefit of therm al acclim a tion, as defined from tr adition al physiological studies of acclim a tion. R a ther, we suggest these studies a re elega nt a n alyses of the A D A P T I V E SI G N I F I C A N C E of D E V E L O P M E N T A L P L A S T I C I T Y . Acclim a tion responses studied by tr adition al compa r a tive physiologists differ subst a ntially to the development al plasticity exa mined by L eroi et a l. [8], Bennett a nd L ensk i [9] a nd G ibert et a l. [14]. H istorically, compa r a tive physiologists considered acclim a tion as a reversible response by a n orga nism to ch a nges (often season al) in a single environment al va riable [13]. B y contr ast, development al plasticity deals with the entire suite of phenotypic ch a nges th a t occur as a result of differences in the development al environment, not just the facult a tive physiological responses of a n orga nism (acclim a tion). Beca use of the highly sensitive n a ture of orga nisms during the ea rly st ages of development, sm all va ria tions in the development al environment ca n lead to a cascade of phenotypic ch a nges [15–17]. Besides acclim a tion responses, development al plasticity ca n a rise from the direct biophysical effects of the environment, a nd ca n be detriment al, neutr al or beneficial. E nvironment al factors th a t lead to these un avoidable, a nd often nonreversible phenotypic ch a nges include temper a ture [18], oxygen tension [19–21], nutrition [22,23] a nd density of conspecifics [24]. F or exa mple, M a tsch a k et a l. [19] found th a t temper a ture-induced ch a nges in muscle cellula rity during embryonic development of the A tla ntic salmon S a lm a sa l a r were pa rtly due to restricted oxygen availability a t higher temper a tures r a ther th a n to facult a tive responses to temper a ture. T he egg capsule of embryonic salmon ca n act as a n oxygen ba rrier, pa rticula rly a t higher temper a tures when there is a n increased oxygen dem a nd. I rreversible ch a nges in the size a nd number of muscle fibres occur a t high development al temper a ture as a direct consequence of a constr aint in oxygen availability. T hese hightemper a ture-induced development al ch a nges in muscle cellula rity a re clea rly not facult a tive acclim a tion responses. O bliga tory developmen t al ch a nges a re pa r ticula rly prevalen t following exposu re to st ressful conditions, bu t t heir effects a re often subtle. H offm a n n a nd H ew a- K apuge [18] distinguished t he rela tive con t ribu tions of differen t types of phenotypic ch a nge following exposu re to high temper a t u res in t he pa r asitic w asp Tr ichogr a m m a n r. br assicae . I mpor t a n tly, t hey fou nd t h a t some bu t not all phenotypic ch a nges du ring developmen t were t he result of facult a tive acclim a tion responses. H offm a n n a nd H ew a- K apuge [18] initially observed t h a t adults of T. n r. br assicae exhibited a n increased resist a nce to st ressful temper a t u res following exposu re to 33°C as pupae, bu t t h a t t hese ch a nges were accompa nied by deleterious fit ness effects. T hey suggested t h a t t hese fit ness decreases eit her reflected a gener al cost of increasing resist a nce to http://tree.trends.com 67 Exposure of bacteria for~7 generations 32oC 41.5oC Compete both groups against each other 32oC-group versus 41.5oC-group 41.5oC-group versus 32oC-group 32oC-group outcompeted 41.5oC-group at 32oC 32oC-group outcompeted 41.5oC-group at 41.5oC Results TRENDS in Ecology & Evolution Fig. 1. Experimental investigation of the Beneficial Acclimation Hypothesis (BAH) by Leroi et al. [8]. The BAH was rejected in this case. st ressful temper a t u res (acclim a tion response) or were associa ted wit h direct phenotypic effects a rising from da m age or developmen t al const r ain ts ca used by t he high temper a t u res. To test this idea, the a uthors exa mined whether the increased resist a nce to high temper a tures in T. nr. br assicae could occur without a ny of the observed decreases in fitness [18]. Pupae of T. nr. br assicae were exposed to 33°C for 2, 3 or 4 h d−1 for four days [18]. F or each trea tment group, there w as a n increase in adult resist a nce to higher temper a tures. However, fitness decreased only in the groups exposed to 33°C for 3 or 4 h d−1 . T hus, decreases in fitness following exposure to high temper a tures were clea rly not ca used by the increased resist a nce to stressful temper a ture (acclim a tion) but r a ther to either gener al da m age to the phenotype or development al constr aints imposed by the high temper a tures. I ncreased resist a nce to temper a ture without fitness costs h as also been observed in the egg pa r asitoid T. ca rver ae in both labor a tory a nd field experiments [25]. E xposing orga nisms to stressful conditions confounds a ny a n alysis of the B A H (Box 1). Besides acclim a tion responses possibly aimed a t minimizing the stress of the environment, pa thological da m age to the phenotype also occurs. I n spite of these confounding effects, sever al a n alyses of the B A H h ave incorpor a ted stressful conditions [8,9,26,27]. Met abolic costs a nd gener al phenotypic da m age could overwhelm a ny positive acclim a tion responses in a stressful environment a nd the B A H might be incorrectly rejected. R a ther th a n compa risons a mong orga nisms r aised under stressful conditions for a n alyses of the B A H , Woods a nd H a rrison [27] advoca te exa mining the costs a nd benefits of specific acclim a tion responses. N ot a ll developmen t a lly i nevi t able ch a nges to t he phenot ype a re ca used by st ressfu l condi t ions. I n t he specific case of temper a t u re, i t is doubtfu l t h a t ever y ph ysiologica l process t h a t is affected by temper a t u re 68 Opinion TRENDS in Ecology & Evolution Vol.17 No.2 February 2002 Box 1. Experimental analysis of the BAH under stressful conditions Woods and Harrison [a] addressed whether the acclimation of Manduca sexta caterpillars to water stress was beneficial. • Treatments Larvae were raised from the first instar on low- (69%) or high-water (80%) artificial diets. After reaching the fifth instar, growth rates for both groups were measured on the same or opposite diet for 36 h. Several determinants of larval water budget were also recorded. • Results Caterpillars raised on high-water diet grew faster than did those raised on low-water diet when tested on both diets. Thus, no benefit for previous exposure to low-water diet was observed. However, larvae responded to short-term hydric stress (low-water diet) by minimizing water excretion by increasing rectal water absorption, and to long-term hydric stress by significantly reducing faecal water excretion. • Conclusions The authors concluded that, under current usage, the beneficial acclimation hypothesis (BAH) had to be rejected in this case. However, we suggest that phenotypic damage owing to hydric stress confounds any analysis of the benefits of acclimation. Do the lower growth rates of larvae raised on a low-water diet (when tested on both diets) reflect pathological phenotypic changes owing to stress OR a lack of beneficial acclimation? We suggest that the pathological effects of chronic stress might overwhelm any possible beneficial acclimation responses. Reference a Woods, H . A. a nd H a rrison, J. F. (2001) T he beneficial acclim a tion hypothesis versus acclim a tion of specific tr aits: physiological ch a nges in w a ter-stressed M a nduca sexta ca terpilla rs. Physiol. Zool. 74, 32–44 ca n ‘accli m a te’ to differen t temper a t u res. T h is w ill i nevi t ably lead to phenot ypic differences bet ween orga n isms from differen t developmen t a l temper a t u res t h a t a re si mply due to t he di rect effects of temper a t u re on t hese developmen t a l pa t h w a ys [17,19–21]. T hese phenot ypic ch a nges a re cer t a i n ly not t hose t h a t were t r adi t ion a lly descr ibed as accli m a t ion responses by compa r a t ive ph ysiologists, bu t a re u ndoubtedly i ncl uded i n t he st udies of L eroi et a l. [8] a nd B en net t a nd L ensk i [9]. T h us, previous a n a lyses of t he B A H usi ng developmen t a l pl ast ici t y a re confou nded by i ncl udi ng sever a l t ypes of phenot ypic pl ast ici t y. A more compelli ng exper i men t a l a n a lysis of t he benefi t of accli m a t ion wou ld be based a rou nd t he concept of accli m a t ion t h a t t r adi t ion a l compa r a t ive ph ysiologists were cr i t icized for assu m i ng w as a l w a ys benefici a l. Exploring the BAH using competing hypotheses I n t wo addi t ion a l st udies explor i ng t he B A H , bot h H uey a nd B er r iga n [11] a nd H uey et a l. [12] advoca ted a st rong i nference approach to ex a m i n i ng quest ions rel a t i ng to t he t her m a l accli m a t ion of ectot her ms. T hei r approach i n volved test i ng a mong compet i ng h ypot heses t h a t m a k e differen t predict ions as to how developmen t a l temper a t u re i nfl uences t he t her m a l sensi t ivi t y of perfor m a nce (Box 2). H uey a nd B er r iga n [11] a nd H uey et a l. [12] t hen used t he da t asets of sever a l previous st udies, such as t h a t by Z w a a n et a l. [28], to compa re t he h ypot heses. Z w a a n et a l. [28] a n a lysed t he effect of developmen t a l temper a t u re on adu l t longevi t y i n D . mel a nogaster a nd fou nd t h a t flies r a ised a t i n ter medi a te temper a t u res su r vived longer as adu l ts t h a n did t hose flies r a ised a t cool or h igh Box 2. Set of competing hypotheses This set of competing hypotheses is as suggested by Huey and Berrigan [a], and Huey et al. [b]. Beneficial Acclimation Hypothesis (BAH): organisms acclimated to a particular environment have enhanced performance or fitness in that environment relative to organisms acclimated to other environments [c]. Optimal developmental Temperature Hypothesis (OTH): organisms raised at intermediate temperatures have higher relative fitness across all temperatures than do organisms raised at high or low temperatures. The OTH was suggested as an alternative to the BAH by Zamudio et al. [d], Huey and Berrigan [a] and Huey et al. [b]. Cooler is Better Hypothesis (CBH): organisms raised at cool temperatures have higher relative fitness across all temperatures than do organisms raised at intermediate or high temperatures. The CBH is based on the assumption that the larger size of cool-developed organisms is http://tree.trends.com sufficiently advantageous to outweigh any benefits of acclimation [b]. Warmer is Better Hypothesis (WBH): organisms raised at high temperatures have higher relative fitness across all temperatures than do those raised at intermediate or cool temperatures. The WBH is the reciprocal of the CBH of Huey et al. [b]. References a H uey, R. B. a nd Berriga n, D . A. (1996) Testing evolution a ry hypotheses of acclim a tion. I n A nim a ls a nd Temper ature: Phenotypic a nd E volution a ry A d aptation. Society for E xperimenta l B iology Semin a r Series (Johnston, I. A. a nd Bennett, A. F., eds), pp. 205–237, C a mbridge U niversity Press b H uey, R. B. et a l. (1999) Testing the adaptive significa nce of acclim a tion: a strong inference approach. A m. Zool. 39, 323–336 c L eroi, A. M. et a l. (1994) Temper a ture acclim a tion a nd competitive fitness: a n experiment al test of the beneficial acclim a tion assumption. Proc. N atl. Aca d. Sci. U . S. A. 91, 1917–1921 d Z a mudio, K .R. et a l. (1995) B igger isn’t alw ays better: body size, temper a ture a nd m ale territorial success in D rosophil a mel a nogaster . A nim. Beh av. 49, 671–677 Opinion TRENDS in Ecology & Evolution Vol.17 No.2 February 2002 Conclusions and future directions Glossary Acclimation: any facultative modification in a physiological trait in response to changes in an environmental variable in the lab. Changes can be in response to the developmental environment or long-term environmental shifts during the later stages of the life history of an organism (more traditionally studied). Responses can be beneficial, neutral or negative. Acclimatization: facultative modifications in a physiological trait in response to changes in one or more environmental variables in the field. Adaptive significance: (in context of plasticity) fitness advantages and disadvantages associated with the expression of phenotypic plasticity across a range of environments. Fitness consequences can be positive, negative or neutral. Developmental plasticity: phenotypic changes induced by variation in the developmental environment. Changes can include facultative responses by the organism (e.g. acclimation) or unavoidable biophysical consequences of the environment (obligatory and/or pathological). Phenotypic plasticity: environmentally induced variation in morphology, physiology and/or behaviour of an organism. Acknowledgements We thank Ian A. Johnston, Helga Guderley, Andy Clarke, Craig Moritz and Raoul Van Damme for stimulating discussions and/or reading the article. This article was improved substantially by comments from several anonymous referees. This work was supported by a small ARC grant awarded to C.E.F. and R.S.W. and a ARC Large Grant to C.E.F. and Craig Moritz. 69 temper a t u res, rega rdless of t he temper a t u re a t w h ich t he adu l t flies were k ept. T hese da t a were used by H uey a nd B er r iga n [11] a nd H uey et a l. [12] to aga i n reject t he gener a li t y of t he B A H i n fa vou r of t he opt i m a l developmen t a l temper a t u re h ypot hesis (O T H ). However, as with the previous a n alyses of the B A H , we suggest this experiment al design is also confounded by sever al different types of plasticity underlying the phenotypic ch a nges, not just acclim a tion responses. We consider th a t, in this context, the O T H , cooler is better hypothesis (C B H) a nd w a rmer is better hypothesis (W B H) all deal specifically with the adaptive consequences of the development al environment, rega rdless of the source of phenotypic ch a nges. However, the B A H refers only to the facult a tive physiological responses of the orga nisms a nd is thus only one specific type of phenotypic plasticity. We suggest th a t the O T H , C B H a nd W B H a re not mutu ally exclusive to the B A H . F or exa mple, it is possible th a t the development al constr aints imposed on the phenotype by some temper a tures a re so grea t th a t the over all perform a nce is domin a ted not by the acclim a tion responses (if they occur), but by these phenotypic inevit abilities. I n other words, there might be a n optim al temper a ture for development th a t is determined solely by the un avoidable ch a nges to the phenotype th a t occur in the different therm al environments. T his, of course, says nothing about the rela tive merit of the ‘acclim a tion’ ch a nges in each environment. References 1 F eder, M. E . (1987) T he a n alysis of physiological diversity: the prospects for pa ttern document a tion a nd gener al questions in ecological physiology. I n N ew D irections in E cologica l Physiology ( F eder, M. E . et a l., eds), pp. 38–75, C a mbridge U niversity Press 2 G arland, T., Jr and C arter, P.A. (1994) E volutionary physiology. Annu. Rev. E col. Syst. 56, 579–621 3 Gould, S.J. a nd L ewontin, R. C. (1979) T he spa ndrels of Sa n M a rco a nd the P a nglossia n P a r adigm: a critique of the adapt a tionist progr a mme. Proc. R. Soc. London B B iol. Sci. 205, 581–598 http://tree.trends.com Previous empirical tests of the B A H h ave elega ntly demonstr a ted the evolution a ry significa nce of therm ally induced development al plasticity [8,9], especially with the advent of a rigorous experiment al design testing sever al competing hypotheses [11,12]. I mport a ntly, these studies of the B A H h ave forcefully m ade the point th a t acclim a tion ch a nges ca nnot just be assumed to be beneficial, but this is a hypothesis th a t must be rigorously tested. However, we believe th a t a det ailed empirical exa min a tion of the adaptive significa nce of more tr adition al measures of physiological acclim a tion is now required to test the B A H . We ch allenge compa r a tive physiologists to develop new inventive experiment al designs to explore the benefits a nd costs of the more tr adition al acclim a tion responses. However, this will not be easy a nd using a nything less th a t a close correla te of fitness to test the hypothesis, such as survival, reproductive success or competitive ability, would be less th a n desir able. Previous empirical tests of the B A H h ave cert ainly set a benchm a r k for exa mining the adaptive significa nce of phenotypic plasticity, rega rdless of the source of phenotypic va ria tion. O ne entertaining possibility for future tests of the B A H would be to examine the thermal acclimation of reproductive performance, especially in a system where females discriminate between displaying males. For example, in a species where females are choosy about their mates, females could be given the opportunity to discriminate between cool- and warmacclimated males at various temperatures. T he ability to attract and procure a female might depend on characters such as swimming performance, aerobic capabilities and general activity, all of which have been shown to acclimate to temperature in a variety of taxa [13]. I n this specific case, the B A H would predict that, at high temperatures, females would find the warm-acclimated males more attractive than they would the cool-acclimated males and vice versa at cool temperatures. E ven more compelling would be the inclusion of males that had been raised at different temperatures, so the relative merits of developmental plasticity could be compared with the more reversibletype acclimation responses. We suggest that future tests of the B A H should investigate traditional types of acclimation using the protocols developed for analysing developmental plasticity. 4 K ingsolver, J. G . a nd H uey, R. B. (1998) E volution a ry a n alyses of morphological a nd physiological plasticity in therm ally va riable environments. A m. Zool. 38, 545–560 5 Bennett, A. F. a nd L ensk i, R. E . (1999) E xperiment al evolution a nd its role in evolution a ry physiology. A m. Zool. 39, 346–362 6 B r adley, T.J. et a l. (1999) Physiological responses to selection for desicca tion resist a nce in D rosophil a mel a nogaster . A m. Zool. 39, 337–345 7 F eder, M. E . et a l. (2000) E volution a ry physiology. A nnu. Rev. E col. Syst. 31, 315–341 8 L eroi, A. M. et a l. (1994) Temper a ture acclim a tion a nd competitive fitness: a n experiment al test of the beneficial acclim a tion assumption. Proc. N atl. Aca d. Sci. U . S. A. 91, 1917–1921 9 Bennett, A. F. a nd L ensk i, R. E . (1997) E volution a ry adapt a tion to temper a ture: V I. Phenotypic acclim a tion a nd its evolution in Escherichi a coli. E volution 51, 36–44 10 Hoffm a nn, A. A. (1995) Acclim a tion: increasing survival a t a cost. Trends E col. E vol. 10, 1–2 11 H uey, R.B. and Berrigan, D.A. (1996) Testing evolutionary hypotheses of acclimation. In A nimals and Temperature: Phenotypic and E volutionary A daptation. Society for E xperimental B iology Seminar Series (Johnston, I.A. and Bennett, A. F., eds), pp. 205–237, C ambridge U niversity Press 70 Opinion TRENDS in Ecology & Evolution Vol.17 No.2 February 2002 12 H uey, R. B. et a l. (1999) Testing the adaptive significa nce of acclim a tion: a strong inference approach. A m. Zool. 39, 323–336 13 Prosser, C. L . (1991) E nvironmenta l a nd Metabolic A nim a l Physiology: Compa r ative A nim a l Physiology , 4th edn, Wiley– L iss 14 G ibert, P. et a l. (2001) Locomotor perform a nce of D rosophil a mel a nogaster : inter actions a mong development al a nd adult temper a tures, age, a nd geogr aphy. E volution 55, 205–209 15 U sher, M. L . et a l. (1994) M uscle development in A tla ntic salmon ( S a lmo sa l a r ) embryos a nd the effect of temper a ture on muscle cellula rity. J. F ish B iol. 44, 953–964 16 Vieir a, V. L . A. a nd Johnston, I. A. (1992) I nfluence of temper a ture on muscle-fibre development in la rvae of the herring C lupea h a rengus. M a r. B iol. 112, 333–341 17 Johnston, I. A. et a l. (1998) E mbryonic temper a ture modula tes muscle growth ch a r acteristics in la rval a nd juvenile herring. J. E xp. B iol. 201, 623–646 18 Hoffm a nn, A. A. a nd H ew a-K apuge, S. (2000) Acclim a tion for hea t resist a nce in Trichogr a mm a nr. br assicae: ca n it occur without costs? F unct. E col. 14, 55–60 19 M atscha k, T.W. et al. (1995) Is physiological hypoxia the driving force behind temperature effects on muscle development in embryonic A tlantic salmon (Salmo salar)? D ifferentiation 59, 71–77 20 M a tsch a k, T.W. et a l. (1998) Met abolic enzyme activities in A tla ntic salmon ( S a lmo sa l a r L .) embryos respond more to chronic ch a nges in oxygen availability th a n to environment al temper a ture. F ish Physiol. B iochem. 18, 115–123 21 M a tsch a k, T.W. et a l. (1998) Temper a ture a nd oxygen tension influence the development of muscle cellula rity in embryonic r ainbow trout. J. F ish B iol. 53, 581–590 22 Shine, R. a nd Downes, S.J. (1999) C a n pregn a nt liza rds adjust their offspring phenotypes to environment al conditions? Oecologi a 119, 1–8 23 Rasmussen, K .M. and F ischbeck, K .L. (1987) E ffect of repeated reproductive cycles on pregnancy Putting predators back into behavioral predator–prey interactions Steven L. Lima In the study of behavioral predator–prey interactions, predators have been treated as abstract sources of risk to which prey respond, rather than participants in a larger behavioral interaction. When predators are put back into the picture by allowing them to respond strategically to prey behavior, expectations about prey behavior can change. Something as simple as allowing predators to move in response to prey movements might not only (radically) alter standard expectations of prey behavior, but might also reveal new classes of behavioral phenomena that occur at large spatial scales. Similar revelations undoubtedly await many well-studied aspects of the behavioral interaction between predator and prey. Most examples studied to date, both theoretical and empirical, require attention from this ‘predatory’ perspective. Putting predators back into the picture will be challenging, but doing so might change the way in which biologists think about predator–prey interactions in general. Steven L. Lima Dept of Life Sciences, Indiana State University, Terre Haute, IN 47809, USA. e-mail: S-Lima @ indstate.edu O ver the past 20 years, the study of behavioral interactions between predator and prey has shed much light on prey behavior, and it is now clear that almost any aspect of prey decision-ma king (from foraging behavior to mate choice) can be influenced by the risk of predation [1–3]. A growing literature also suggests that nonlethal interactions between predator and prey (those driven by prey avoidance of predation) might be an important component of predator–prey interactions http://tree.trends.com 24 25 26 27 28 outcome in ad libitum -fed and chronically foodrestricted rats. J. N utr. 117, 1959–1966 N ewm a n, R. A. (1998) E cological constr aints on a mphibia n met a morphosis: inter actions of temper a ture a nd la rval density with responses to ch a nging food level. Oecologi a 115, 9–16 T homson, L .J. et a l. (2001) F ield a nd labor a tory evidence for acclim a tion without costs in a n egg pa r asitoid. F unct. E col. 15, 217–221 G ibbs, A. G . et a l. (1998) E ffects of cuticula r lipids a nd w a ter bala nce in a desert D rosophil a : is therm al acclim a tion beneficial? J. E xp. B iol. 201, 71–80 Woods, H . A. a nd H a rrison, J. F. (2001) T he beneficial acclim a tion hypothesis versus acclim a tion of specific tr aits: physiological ch a nges in w a ter-stressed M a nduca sexta ca terpilla rs. Physiol. Zool. 74, 32–44 Zw a a n, B. et a l. (1995) A rtificial selection for development al time in D rosophil a mel a nogaster in rela tion to the evolution of aging: direct a nd correla ted responses. E volution 49, 635–648 in general [4–8]. Wor k on behavioral predator–prey interactions therefore provides an important bridge between the studies of behavior and ecology. I n spite of t hese m a ny adva nces, ou r u nderst a nding of beh avior al preda tor–prey in ter actions is limited by a simple oversigh t: we vir t u ally forgot abou t t he beh avior of preda tors! H istorically, we h ave been so focused on prey beh avior t h a t we (myself included) beca me comfor t able wit h t rea ting preda tors as u n responsive ‘black boxes’ r a t her t h a n pa r ticipa n ts in a beh avior al in ter action. T his oversigh t h as not only led to a n incomplete view of beh avior al in ter actions bet ween preda tors a nd prey, bu t h as also obscu red a n en tire class of such in ter actions t h a t occu rs a t la rge spa tial scales. M y goal is to explore some of t he insigh ts gained from pu t ting preda tors back in to beh avior al preda tor–prey in ter actions. How were predators removed from the interaction? T he removal of preda tors from the beh avior al preda tor–prey inter action is appa rent in the ubiquitous ‘fixed-risk’ assumptions of const a nt a tt ack r a tes over time a nd pa tch-specific risks of preda tion (e.g. Ref. [9]); such assumptions imply th a t preda tors a re not influenced by prey beh avior. As few would a rgue for the strict validity of this assumption, why were preda tors relega ted to the st a tus of unresponsive entities? I n m a ny w ays, the fixed-risk approach (i.e. the assumption of unresponsive preda tors) w as a sensible st a rting point. C h a r acterizing preda tion risk as a n environment al const a nt seemed reason able given th a t preda tors ca n stri ke opportunistically a nd could be a nywhere a t a given time. M a them a tical convenience might h ave also played a role: models of a ntipreda tor decisionm a k ing a re much simpler under a n assumption of fixed risk th a n they a re when both preda tor a nd prey a re allowed to respond to one a nother. F urthermore, 0169-5347/02/$ – see front matter © 2002 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved. PII: S0169-5347(01)02393-X