Linguistic features of easy-to- read texts for students with intellectual disability Background
by user
Comments
Transcript
Linguistic features of easy-to- read texts for students with intellectual disability Background
15/09/2010 Linguistic features of easy-toread texts for students with intellectual disability Fajardo, I., Ávila, V., Tavares, G. and Ferrer, A. University of Valencia Background • Low reading competence in students with mild and borderline Intellectual disability (ID) – Decoding: • Weak decoding skills (e.g. Conners, Rosenquist, Atwell & Sligh, 2001) – Comprehension (literal and inferential): • Small mental lexicon (equivalent age of receptive vocabulary of 4-5 years in people with ID aged 14, Ferrer et al., 1999). • Morphosyntactic deficits (Perovic, 2006): dificulties with pronouns, reflexives, connectives, etc. • Low recall of text statements (Wolman, 1991) • Low quality inferences in expository texts (Bos and Tierney, 1984) September 6 - 7, 2010 Frankfurt am Main 1 15/09/2010 Background • Two types of interventions: – Training – Adaptation • Matching texts and ID students´ reading levels optimally: – selection – adaptation from originals Use of easy-to read texts – Ad-hoc design September 6 - 7, 2010 Frankfurt am Main How to optimally match texts and students? • Guidelines for the Design of Easy-to-Read text – Principles for adapting texts: linguistics, illustrations and graphics. – Ej. International Federation of Library Associations- IFLA (Tronbacke, 1998) – Not empirically supported – Too general – Difficult to measure or quantify: readbility measures • • E.g. “use a simple, straightforward language” or “use a clear and logical structure” Readbility measures – Shallow: Flesch Index (Flesh, 1948), word frequency… – Deep: Coh-metrix (McNamara at al., 2002): connectives, co-references… – Contradictory evidence for word frequency, co-references, etc. September 6 - 7, 2010 Frankfurt am Main 2 15/09/2010 General Goals 1. Validate empirically a corpus of easy-to-read texts for students with Intellectual Disability (ID). 2. Examine the relationship between texts’ linguistic variables and reading comprehension: literal vs. inferential September 6 - 7, 2010 Frankfurt am Main Method • Selection of a corpus of ad-hoc easy-to-read texts • Measurement of: – Texts linguistic features – Students’ comprehension performance. • Analyses: – Correlation – Regression September 6 - 7, 2010 Frankfurt am Main 3 15/09/2010 Participants • • • 28 ID students (17 males and 11 females): 16 valid cases Center of Vocational Training ‘Camí Obert’ The average age was 19 (ranged from 16 to 22). Processes Test Result K-Bit (Kaufman, 1997) 64.9 IQ (mild intelletual disability) Vocabulary Knowledge PPVT-III (Peabody, Dunn, 2006) 9.3 (average equivalent age) Reading comprehension ECL test (De la Cruz, 2005) second year of primary school (PROLEC-SE, Ramos y Cuetos, 1999) 5th (average percentil) Syntactic knowledge September 6 - 7, 2010 Frankfurt am Main Material: The corpus • 48 pieces of news selected from www.noticiasfacil.es Lexical variables Nº of Words Average syllables per word Average word frequency M 93.8 1.9 21361 (high freq. according to normative data) Sentence variables Nº of sentences 11.6 Average words per sentence 8.3 INFLESZ index 74.7 (high legibility according to normative data) Textual cohesion variables Nº of connectives 4 N° co-references (noun, argument and stem overlap) Nº of ellipsis 2 4 15/09/2010 Material: The comprehension test • Reading comprehension test (adapted from Ozuru, Rowe, O’Reilly & McNamara, 2008): – Shallow questions included literal and rewording questions: the answer to this type of questions was either explicitly stated or restructured within a single sentence. – Deep questions required between-sentence integration of information. Task and Procedure • During a period of 16 weeks, participants were asked weekly to read 3 pieces of news and to answer a reading comprehension test. • There was no time limit to complete the task. September 6 - 7, 2010 Frankfurt am Main 5 15/09/2010 Results September 6 - 7, 2010 Frankfurt am Main Reading comprehension Analyses of Variance per item (text): Effect of type of comprehension question, F(1,43)=11.26; MSE=.248; p<.01. September 6 - 7, 2010 Frankfurt am Main 6 15/09/2010 Correlation analyses M r (n) SD Lexical variables Shallow questions (47 texts) Deep questions (45 texts) Nº of Words 93.8 40.9 -.30 (47 ) * - .32 (45) * Sentence variabl Nº of sentences 11.6 5.6 -.30 (47) * -.37 (45) * Nº of connectives 4.0 3.3 -.35 (47) * -.13 (45) N° co-references (noun, argument and stem overlap) 12.5 6.2 -.39 (47 ) ** -.24 (45) Textual cohesion variables *p>.05, **p<.01 September 6 - 7, 2010 Frankfurt am Main Multiple regression analyses • Number of co-references predicted shallow comprehension: 14% (Adjusted R2 = .14, F(1, 45) = 8.42, p < .001; Beta=-.40,p<.01). • Number of sentences predicted inferential comprehension: 12 % (Adjusted R2 = .12, F(1, 43) = 6.74, p < .01; Beta=-.37, p<.01). September 6 - 7, 2010 Frankfurt am Main 7 15/09/2010 Discussion I: co-references • The corpus of easy-to-read texts is valid for ensuring comprehension especially at the superficial level. • Number of co-references, predicted shallow comprehension: inverse relationship, contrarily to what previous literature seemed to indicate (Crossley et al., 2008). – Several types of co-references mixed – The redundancy introduced in simplified texts by means of noun overlaps, apart from an extension of utterances, would make the sentences´ grammar to be more complex and unnatural (Meisel, 1980). September 6 - 7, 2010 Frankfurt am Main Discussion II: number of sentences • The number of sentences predicted inferential comprehension – Co-variation with conceptual complexity – Low capacity of ID students to storage and retrieve information from memory (Wolman, 1991) – Many info units -> no possibility of retain them and make inferences-> more info units to process -> overload-> less inferences and more superficial processing September 6 - 7, 2010 Frankfurt am Main 8 15/09/2010 Discussion III: null effects • Superficial measures: effects of word frequency and Flesch Index on our measures of comprehension were null – Bring into question the validity of some popular design guidelines to optimally match texts and ID students´ reading levels. • No facilitatitive effect of connective (contrary to Sanders et al., 2007) – It depends on the type of semantic relationship between clauses they signal (Crosson et al, 2008) – Acquisition order: additive, temporal, causal, and adversative . – Is the acquisition delayed or impared in intellectual disability? September 6 - 7, 2010 Frankfurt am Main Methodological shortcomings • Small variability of linguistic features • Small participant sample size September 6 - 7, 2010 Frankfurt am Main 9 15/09/2010 Questions… September 6 - 7, 2010 Frankfurt am Main 10